Abstract

Scientific evidence to support the recommended salt intake of < 5.8 g/day is virtually non-existingent. There are no randomized controlled trials (RCTs) to investigate the effect of salt reduction (SR) below 5.8 g on health outcomes. The effect of SR on blood pressure (BP) reaches maximal efficacy at 1 week. RCTs in healthy individuals lasting at least 1 week show that the effect of SR on BP is <1 mmHg, but that SR has significant side effects, including increases in renin, aldosterone, noradrenalin, adrenalin, cholesterol and triglyceride. Still, disregarding confounders and side effects, health authorities use BP effects obtained in studies of pre-hypertensive and hypertensive patients to recommend SR in the healthy population and use these biased BP effects in statistical models indirectly to project millions of saved lives. These fantasy projections are in contrast to real data from prospective observational population studies directly associating salt intake with mortality, which show that salt intake <5.8 g/day is associated with an increased mortality of ∼15%. The population studies also show that a very high salt intake >12.2 g is associated with increased mortality. However, since <5% of populations consume such high amounts of salt, SR at the population level should not be a public health priority. Consequently, this policy should be abolished, not because any attempt to implement it has failed, and not because it costs taxpayers and food consumers unnecessary billions of dollars, but because—if implemented—it might kill people instead of saving them.

INTRODUCTION

A 2013 Institute of Medicine (IOM) report [1] concluded that there is not sufficient data to conclude that individuals benefit from salt intake lower than the present usual intake, which in 95% of the world's population is 6.6–12.2 g (2620–4830 mg sodium) (114–210 mmol) [2], a conclusion confirmed by observational studies [3–5]. Still, many health organizations recommend a radical reduction in salt intake to a level <5.8 g (2300 mg sodium). However, this public health priority is flawed [6, 7].

THE POLICY TO REDUCE SALT INTAKE HAS ITS ORIGIN IN IDEOLOGY

The ideological salt–BP conflict was initiated in 1904, summarized in 1949 [8, 9] and ran for 70 years before the first randomized controlled trial (RCT) in 1973 [10] and an observational study in 1985 [11] were performed in humans.

THE MYTH OF THE FOOD INDUSTRY CONSPIRACY

According to advocates for salt reduction (SR), the food industries, by adding salt to pre-prepared food, undermine the health of their customers. The devoted advocates claim that sceptics are under the influence of the food industry and compare sceptics with tobacco lobbyists [12]. However, the salt-reduction-leads-to-blood-pressure-reduction idea is not that brilliant and does call for an independent critique, verified by observational studies and RCTs and two large meta-analyses relating salt to mortality, BP and side effects [5, 13], which all are independent of commercial interests. Finally, public health institutions and the food industry nowadays cooperate to promote the production of low-salt foods [14], indicating a new reciprocity between previous opponents, which creates an unanswered question of potential risk: what are the health consequences of the methods or substances used to substitute salt.

SALT IS THE ONLY EXISTING NUTRIENT DEEMED TO BE UNHEALTHY WITHIN THE USUAL INTAKE RANGE

IOM's definition of adequate nutrient intake (AI) is ‘the approximate intake found in apparently healthy populations’ [15]. For salt, this intake is 6.6–12.2 g [2]. Conflicting with its own definition, however, IOM defined the AI for salt to be 3.3–3.8 g [15]. The World Health Organization (WHO), the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), the American Heart Association (AHA) and the British National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) have adopted this position and defined an upper limit for a healthy salt intake to be 5.8 g, i.e. radically below the established salt intake in healthy populations.

SALT INTAKE IS DEEMED ‘NORMAL’ AT LEVELS CONSUMED BY VIRTUALLY NO ONE

According to these health institutions, the 2.5% of the world's populations that meet their recommendations has a normal salt intake, whereas the other 7 billion people have a high intake. This idea is based on the evolutionary discordance hypothesis according to which modern man genetically is identical with his Palaeolithic ancestors, who are claimed to be genetically predisposed to a low-salt diet [16]. However, during the 2.5 million year Palaeolithic period, many diet patterns have been identified [17]. Furthermore, the fact that humans are equipped with a neuro-endocrine system [18] and kidneys, which handle enormous variations in salt intake, indicates that humans are not genetically designed to one specific salt diet.

SALT-CONSERVING HORMONES INCREASE EXPONENTIALLY AT LOW SALT INTAKE

Renin and aldosterone levels are low within the usual salt intake range, but increase exponentially below 5.8 g [19], verified to be a persistent effect in Brazilian Indians on a chronic low-salt diet [20]. The latter finding supports that humans are not genetically determined to a low-salt diet. A meta-analysis of 14 studies, which reduced salt to levels in the vicinity of 5.8 g, found small effects of SR on renin and aldosterone [21]. However, a meta-analysis of 53 studies, which included many studies reducing salt below 5.8 g, showed a significant proportional effect of SR on renin and aldosterone [22].

STRESS HORMONES INCREASE SIGNIFICANTLY AT SALT INTAKE LEVELS <5.8 G

Several studies have shown significant increases in adrenalin and noradrenalin, confirmed in a meta-analysis of 33 studies reducing usual salt intake to <5.8 g [13]. However, this was not observed in a meta-analysis of six studies reducing salt to a level in the vicinity of 5.8 g [21].

THE MYTH OF THE AUTHORITY OF HEALTH INSTITUTIONS I: NATIONAL HEART LUNG AND BLOOD INSTITUTE–SPONSORED STUDIES ARE BIASED TO EXAGGERATE THE EFFECT OF SR ON BLOOD PRESSURE

The advocate's unconditional acceptance of the SR position of public health institutions and reference to these institutions as ‘authorities’ is circular, because the advocates and the public health institutions all belong to the same network, who distribute funding, perform trials and, on behalf of governments, make decisions. The biased procedures in this closed, authoritarian network have nothing to do with science. Table 1 shows the effect of RCTs sponsored by the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute (NHLBI) [23–26]. The characteristics of the typical overweight study populations show higher baseline BP and mean age every time a new study appears and an increase in the number of African American participants. In the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH)-sodium trial [26], the potassium content in the control diet was only half that in the average American diet. Low potassium intake is well known to increase sodium sensitivity [27], and therefore the DASH-sodium trial was designed with at least five confounding variables favouring SR: high baseline BP, low potassium intake, older age, high BMI and high percentage of African American participants. In accordance with these concerns, the DASH-sodium researchers went through litigation to avoid publishing the individual participant data [28], contributing to the notion that these data may include ‘controversial’ information. The fact that studies biased in favour of salt sensitivity show only small effects (Table 1) [23–26] indicates that SR would have minimal effects on BP in the general population. This is emphasized by a supplementary publication of the DASH trial [29] that—in a disregarded subanalysis—shows that in individuals between 21 and 41 years of age there is no significant effect of a low-sodium/high-potassium diet on BP. As the median age of the American population is 37 years, this indicates that SR has no effect on BP in >50% of Americans. Finally, the much shorter duration of the last study [26] (1 month) probably reflects a deliberate strategy change, as the previous studies [23–25] (duration 6–36 months) revealed that significant SR is difficult to implement for a longer period. To conclude, these trials do not reflect the authority of a public institution, but rather a bias intended to sustain a questionable policy.

Table 1.

The effect of SR on BP in studies sponsored by the NHLBI

StudyReferencePotassiumaAgebBaseline BPcBMIdAfrican American, %Duration, monthsSR, mmolEffect BP, mmHg
HPT 1990Arch Intern Med 1990; 150: 153256239124.3/82.729163617+0.1/+0.2
TOHP I 1992JAMA 1992; 267: 1213242342.6125.1/83.929.515644−1.7/−0.9
TOHP II 1997Arch Intern Med 1997; 157: 657257444.2127.7/86.131183642−2.9/−1.6
DASH 2001 DASH dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3315947134/862957177−3.0/−1.6
DASH 2001 control dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3163849135/863056177−6.7/−3.5
StudyReferencePotassiumaAgebBaseline BPcBMIdAfrican American, %Duration, monthsSR, mmolEffect BP, mmHg
HPT 1990Arch Intern Med 1990; 150: 153256239124.3/82.729163617+0.1/+0.2
TOHP I 1992JAMA 1992; 267: 1213242342.6125.1/83.929.515644−1.7/−0.9
TOHP II 1997Arch Intern Med 1997; 157: 657257444.2127.7/86.131183642−2.9/−1.6
DASH 2001 DASH dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3315947134/862957177−3.0/−1.6
DASH 2001 control dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3163849135/863056177−6.7/−3.5

aMean potassium intake of American population: 2640 mg.

bMedian age of American population: 37 years.

cMedian baseline BP of American population: 119/71 mmHg.

dMean BMI of American population (>20 years): 28.

Table 1.

The effect of SR on BP in studies sponsored by the NHLBI

StudyReferencePotassiumaAgebBaseline BPcBMIdAfrican American, %Duration, monthsSR, mmolEffect BP, mmHg
HPT 1990Arch Intern Med 1990; 150: 153256239124.3/82.729163617+0.1/+0.2
TOHP I 1992JAMA 1992; 267: 1213242342.6125.1/83.929.515644−1.7/−0.9
TOHP II 1997Arch Intern Med 1997; 157: 657257444.2127.7/86.131183642−2.9/−1.6
DASH 2001 DASH dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3315947134/862957177−3.0/−1.6
DASH 2001 control dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3163849135/863056177−6.7/−3.5
StudyReferencePotassiumaAgebBaseline BPcBMIdAfrican American, %Duration, monthsSR, mmolEffect BP, mmHg
HPT 1990Arch Intern Med 1990; 150: 153256239124.3/82.729163617+0.1/+0.2
TOHP I 1992JAMA 1992; 267: 1213242342.6125.1/83.929.515644−1.7/−0.9
TOHP II 1997Arch Intern Med 1997; 157: 657257444.2127.7/86.131183642−2.9/−1.6
DASH 2001 DASH dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3315947134/862957177−3.0/−1.6
DASH 2001 control dietN Engl J Med 2001; 344: 3163849135/863056177−6.7/−3.5

aMean potassium intake of American population: 2640 mg.

bMedian age of American population: 37 years.

cMedian baseline BP of American population: 119/71 mmHg.

dMean BMI of American population (>20 years): 28.

THE MYTH OF THE AUTHORITY OF HEALTH INSTITUTIONS II: UNBIASED ANALYSES OF THE TRIALS OF HYPERTENSION PREVENTION SHOW NO EFFECT OF SR ON MORBIDITY AND MORTALITY

In 2007, data from the Trials of Hypertension Prevention (TOHP) studies [24, 25], randomizing participants to low-salt and usual-salt diets, were analysed with cardiovascular disease (CVD) events (available in 77% of the participants) and all-cause mortality (ACM) (available in 100% of the participants) as outcomes according to the randomized groups (intention-to-treat) and in a supplementary analysis after adjustment for seven confounders [30]. In the adjusted analysis, the CVD outcome was lower in the low-salt group (P = 0.018), but not ACM (P = 0.34). There was no difference in CVD (P = 0.19) or ACM (P = 0.58) in the intention-to-treat analysis. In 2009, a reanalysis was published, but participants on salt restriction were excluded, transforming the study from an RCT to an observational study associating quartiles of salt intake on the basis of 24-h urine excretions with CVD, but not with ACM [31]. The primary analysis showed no significant association between salt intake and CVD events (P = 0.38). A borderline significant inverse association was found between potassium and CVD events (P = 0.08). Consequently, the identified association between the sodium:potassium ratio and CVD events (P = 0.04) was driven by potassium and not by sodium. In 2014, a third elaboration of the TOHP studies was performed [32]. The authors investigated four sodium intake intervals divided by the values 2300, 3600 and 4800 mg, now including potassium excretion as a covariate in the primary analysis, which, however, still did not show a significant association between sodium excretion and CVD outcomes (P = 0.13). They also constructed a spline plot, which just reached statistical significance for ‘linearity’ (P = 0.044). However, due to several biases, the value of this analysis is limited: (i) as outcome events were only recorded in 77% of the patients, ∼57 events were not accounted for; (ii) the outcome in the low-salt group was based on only 17 events; (iii) the removal of just 2 of 74 events from the reference group to the 17 events in the lowest salt group would change the ‘continuously descending straight line’ to a U-shaped curve; (iv) after so many efforts to reach a significant P-value, a statistical correction for multiple comparisons would have been appropriate; (v) the authors did not respond to several challenges concerning the design of the study [33], but stated that the analyses were pre-specified, a statement that is invalid, as such pre-specifications have never been published; (vi) the omission not to analyze the most reliable outcome, ACM, which was recorded in 100% of the participants, is inappropriate; and (vii) the data were obtained in obese borderline hypertensive persons and therefore these results, even if they were correct, are not suitable for public policymaking.

THE MYTH OF THE AUTHORITY OF HEALTH INSTITUTIONS III: TRIALS BY ‘WORLD ACTION ON SALT AND HEALTH’ ARE BIASED BY HYPERTENSION AND AGE

The World Action on Salt and Health (WASH) group has performed 10 trials of the effects of salt restriction on BP. They are all biased by older age and high baseline BP and show an effect that is twice that of comparable trials [34]. Therefore, they do not reflect reliable authority, but a specific knowledge of how to identify a significant effect of SR on BP and a biased use of this effect: investigate older individuals with high BP and apply the effect to healthy individuals.

THE ASSUMED BP EFFECT OF SR IN HEALTHY INDIVIDUALS IS VIRTUALLY NON-EXISTENT

About 50% of RCTs investigating the effect of SR on BP have been performed in participants with BP in the upper 25th percentile of the population. This has contributed to the general impression of a significant association between salt intake and BP. However, just as antihypertensive treatments are not indicated for healthy individuals, an effect of SR on BP in hypertensive individuals does not justify a general SR policy, verified by the fact that in 64 RCTs with a duration of at least 7 days, a salt intake of maximally 15 g (250 mmol) and BP <131/78 mmHg, corresponding to the limit for the upper 25th percentile of the BP distribution in the American population, the effect of 6 g (108 mmol) SR on BP is 1/0.4 mmHg (unpublished data from Graudal et al. [13]). In another meta-analysis, the effect in persons with normal BP was claimed to be 2/1 mmHg [21]. The authors of this analysis included only studies lasting at least 4 weeks, because of a claim that the effect of SR depended on the duration. However, a recent meta-analysis of longitudinal studies measuring the BP effect of SR several times during the observation period showed that there was no difference in systolic BP (SBP) effect or diastolic BP (DBP) effect between weeks 1 and 6, thus defining the time point for maximal efficacy to be 1 week [35]. The restricted inclusion criteria of the alternative review [21] resulted in the inclusion of study populations that, all except two, had baseline BP within the upper 50th percentile of the American population, and therefore this meta-analysis is not suitable for public policymaking. The missing link between salt and BP in >50% of a population is verified by the previously mentioned disregarded outcome of the DASH study, which showed no effect of SR on BP in individuals <41 years of age [29].

NO RCTs INVESTIGATE THE EFFECT OF SR BELOW 5.8 G ON HEALTH OUTCOMES

A recently updated meta-analysis of eight RCTs with follow-up data on morbidity and mortality found a non-significant trend versus reduced CVD morbidity, but could not demonstrate reduced ACM in the low-salt group [36]. The reported 24-h sodium intakes in the low-salt group in these eight studies varied between 2300 mg (5.8 g salt) and 3800 mg (9.6 g salt).

THERE ARE NO RCT INVESTIGATING THE EFFECT OF SR ON HEALTH OUTCOMES IN HEALTHY INDIVIDUALS

A pooled analysis of the above-mentioned eight trials [36] shows a marginally significant increase in CVD outcomes, but still no difference in the most reliable outcome, ACM. As the trials were not performed in healthy individuals, but in older, overweight pre-hypertensive or hypertensive individuals, the results are not suitable for public policymaking, although the identical ACM risk in the low and usual salt intake groups of study populations with a theoretical increased risk of CVD indicates that SR is not beneficial.

RCTS SHOW THAT SR BELOW 5.8 g LEADS TO INCREASES IN CHOLESTEROL AND TRIGLYCERIDE

A meta-analysis of eight studies showed no effect of SR within the usual salt intake range on serum cholesterol and serum triglyceride [21]. In a meta-analysis of 24 studies, which reduced salt intake below 5.8 g, there was a significant increase in both serum cholesterol and serum triglyceride, and these increases were independent of the baseline BP [13].

PROJECTED EFFECTS ON MORTALITY BASED ON RCTS ARE MISLEADING

Modelling studies use data from RCTs and meta-analyses to establish a dose–response relationship between salt intake and BP. By means of data from observational studies, which link BP to mortality, this dose–response relationship is used to translate SR to a reduction in mortality. However, most of the RCTs in the dose–response analyses include participants with a high baseline BP and older age, resulting in overestimation of the dose–response relationship, which is further amplified by forcing the dose–response relationship through zero to increase the slope of the relationship. Potential side effects are consistently ignored in these models. The latest example of a modelling study to use this biased technique [37] adopted our Cochrane data [13] to design the dose–response relationship. Although the authors knew that the very same studies, which showed a BP effect, also showed an increase in renin, aldosterone, adrenalin, noradrenalin, cholesterol and triglyceride, the authors ignored these data. The amplification of the BP effect and the accompanying disregard of side effects explain why modelled outcomes are not in accordance with the outcomes of observational studies based on real data.

OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES SHOW THAT SALT INTAKE BELOW 5.8 g IS ASSOCIATED WITH INCREASED MORTALITY

The main objections against observational studies are (i) that the methods to measure salt intake are inaccurate and (ii) the risk of reverse causality due to the possibility that unhealthy individuals eat less salt. However, in the Prospective Urban Rural Epidemiology (PURE) study of >100 000 participants worldwide [4], the authors documented a direct relationship between salt intake and BP, which was stronger in participants with hypertension than in participants with normal BP [38]. These results are similar to those identified in RCTs [13, 22]. The study also demonstrated that at the low end, sodium intake was inversely associated with mortality, while at the high end, it was directly associated with mortality [4]. If the ‘inaccurate’ salt intake measurements are sufficient to identify this well-known BP pattern [38], there is no reason to assume that they are insufficient to identify mortality outcomes [4].

The U-shaped association observed in the PURE study was also found in a contemporary independent analysis [3]. Furthermore, the mortality risk associated with lowsalt intake increased when sick participants were eliminated from the analyses, indicating that reverse causality was not a problem [4]. A similar finding was seen in a meta-analysis of 25 observational studies, which showed that the association between lowsalt intake and mortality was greater when only studies of healthy populations adjusted for multiple confounders were included in the meta-analysis [5]. Figure 1 shows an updated version of this analysis. These findings indicate that lowsalt intake is not a confounder, but a distinct risk factor for increased mortality.

All-cause mortality (ACM), usual sodium versus low sodium. Results from nine population-representative observational studies adjusted for multiple confounders. Exchanging the first analyses of the NHANES studies (1, 7) with the reanalyses (2, 8): HR = 0.87 [0.82, 0.91], P < 0.00001. Further exchanging primary analyses (5, 11, 13) with analyses of low-risk populations (6, 12, 14): HR = 0.86 [0.81, 0.92], P < 0.00001. LR, low risk (reproduced with permission, American Journal of Hypertension).41
FIGURE 1:

All-cause mortality (ACM), usual sodium versus low sodium. Results from nine population-representative observational studies adjusted for multiple confounders. Exchanging the first analyses of the NHANES studies (1, 7) with the reanalyses (2, 8): HR = 0.87 [0.82, 0.91], P < 0.00001. Further exchanging primary analyses (5, 11, 13) with analyses of low-risk populations (6, 12, 14): HR = 0.86 [0.81, 0.92], P < 0.00001. LR, low risk (reproduced with permission, American Journal of Hypertension).41

CONCLUSION

A PubMed search using ‘dietary salt or dietary sodium’ as a searchterm reveals ∼27 000 hits with a variety of outcomes. Therefore, a review of evidence can be biased to support any view. Opposing views are frequently occurring in the salt conflict, which is well characterized by a quote by Brooks, who stated, ‘authors can be pictured as intellectual partisans of their own opinions, scouring the literature for justification’ [39]. Scoured literature was used to define the present adequate intake of salt by a 2005 IOM committee in conflict with IOM's general definition of an adequate intake of a nutrient [40]. The advocates mask scouring as ‘selection of quality’, where ‘quality’ in reality is nothing but a euphemism for the ‘right’ outcome. The fact is that scoured RCTs [23–26, 30], meta-analyses [21] and modelled projection studies [37] supporting the idea of SR below 5.8 g are biased by multiple confounders and therefore not suitable for public policymaking, which should be based on complete systematic reviews.

The link between salt intake and BP is minimal in individuals with a normal BP eating salt within the present usual intake range. In contrast, the potential side effects of SR on salt-conserving hormones, stress hormones and lipids are independent of the baseline BP and will affect the majority of the population. This exposure to classic CVD risk factors with an intervention that will have no or at best a minimal effect on BP seems imprudent. In accordance with this finding, prospective observational studies linking individual salt intake with mortality show that low intake below 5.8 g is associated with increased mortality, as is very high intake above 12.2 g. However, since <5% of populations consume such high amounts of salt, reducing salt intake at the population level should not be a public health priority.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT

Some opinions, arguments and statements used in this opinion paper have been published in other articles, but the present presentation approach has not been published previously in whole or part. None declared.

(See related articles by Cappuccio. Pro: Reducing salt intake at population level: is it really a public health priority? Nephrol Dial Transplant 2016; 31: 1392–1396; Zoccali and Mallamaci. Moderator’s view: Salt, cardiovascular risk, observational research and recommendations for clinical practice. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2016; 31: 1405–1408)

REFERENCES

1

Institute of Medicine
.
Sodium Intake in Populations: Assessment of Evidence
.
Washington, DC
:
National Academies Press
,
2013

2

McCarron
DA
,
Kazaks
AG
,
Geerling
JC
et al. .
Normal range of human dietary sodium intake: a perspective based on 24-hour urinary sodium excretion worldwide
.
Am J Hypertens
2013
;
26
:
1218
1223

3

Pfister
R
,
Michels
G
,
Sharp
SJ
et al. .
Estimated urinary sodium excretion and risk of heart failure in men and women in the EPIC-Norfolk study
.
Eur J Heart Fail
2014
;
16
:
394
402

4

O'Donnell
M
,
Mente
A
,
Rangarajan
S
et al. .
Urinary sodium and potassium excretion, mortality, and cardiovascular events
.
N Engl J Med
2014
;
371
:
612
623

5

Graudal
N
,
Jürgens
G
,
Baslund
B
et al. .
Compared with usual sodium intake, low- and excessive-sodium diets are associated with increased mortality: a meta-analysis
.
Am J Hypertens
2014
;
27
:
1129
1137

6

Heaney
RP
.
Making sense of the science of sodium
.
Nutr Today
2015
;
50
:
63
66

7

Graudal
N
.
Dietary sodium: where science and policy conflict: impact of the 2013 IOM Report on Sodium Intake in Populations
.
Curr Hypertens Rep
2015
;
17
:
9

8

Graudal
N
.
Commentary: possible role of salt intake in the development of essential hypertension
.
Int J Epidemiol
2005
;
34
:
972
974

9

Chapmann
CP
,
Gibbons
TB
.
The diet and hypertension. A review
.
Medicine
1949
;
29
:
29
69

10

Parijs
J
,
Joossens
JV
,
Van der Linden
L
et al. .
Moderate sodium restriction and diuretics in the treatment of hypertension
.
Am Heart J
1973
;
85
:
22
34

11

Kagan
A
,
Popper
JS
,
Rhoads
GG
et al. .
Dietary and other risk factors for stroke in Hawaiian Japanese men
.
Stroke
1985
;
16
:
390
396

12

Cappuccio
FP
,
Capewell
S
,
Lincoln
P
et al. .
Policy options to reduce population salt intake
.
BMJ
2011
;
343
:
d4995

13

Graudal
N
,
Hubeck-Graudal
T
,
Jürgens
G
.
Effects of sodium restriction on blood pressure, renin, aldosterone, catecholamines, cholesterols, and triglyceride
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
2011
;
11
:
CD004022

14

Dötsch
M
,
Busch
J
,
Batenburg
M
et al. .
Strategies to reduce sodium consumption: a food industry perspective
.
Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr
2009
;
49
:
841
851

15

Institute of Medicine
.
Dietary Reference Intakes: Water, Potassium, Sodium, Chloride, and Sulfate
.
Washington, DC
:
National Academies Press
,
2005

16

Eaton
SB
,
Konner
M
.
Paleolithic nutrition. A consideration of its nature and current implications
.
N Engl J Med
1985
;
312
:
283
289

17

Turner
BL
,
Thompson
AL
.
Beyond the Paleolithic prescription: incorporating diversity and flexibility in the study of human diet evolution
.
Nutr Rev
2013
;
71
:
501
510

18

Geerling
JC
,
Loewy
AD
.
Central regulation of sodium appetite
.
Exp Physiol
2008
;
93
:
177
209

19

Brunner
HR
,
Laragh
JH
,
Baer
L
et al. .
Essential hypertension: renin and aldosterone, heart attack and stroke
.
N Engl J Med
1972
;
286
:
441
449

20

Oliver
JW
,
Cohen
EL
,
Neel
JV
.
Blood pressure, sodium intake, and sodium related hormones in the Yanomamo Indians, a ‘no-salt’ culture
.
Circulation
1975
;
52
:
146
151

21

He
FJ
,
Li
J
,
MacGregor
GA
.
Effect of longer-term modest salt reduction on blood pressure
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
2013
;
4
:
CD004937

22

Graudal
NA
,
Galløe
AM
,
Garred
P
.
Effects of sodium restriction on blood pressure, renin, aldosterone, catecholamines, cholesterols, and triglyceride
.
JAMA
1998
;
279
:
1383
1391

23

The Hypertension Prevention Trial: three-year effects of dietary changes on blood pressure
.
Arch Intern Med
1990
;
150
:
153
162

24

The effects of nonpharmacologic interventions on BP of persons with high normal levels
.
JAMA
1992
;
267
:
1213
1220

25

Effects of weight loss and sodium reduction intervention on BP and hypertension incidence in overweight people with high-normal blood pressure. The Trials of Hypertension Prevention, phase II
.
Arch Intern Med
1997
;
157
:
657
667

26

Sacks
F
,
Svetkey
LP
,
Vollmer
WM
et al. .
Effects on BP of reduced dietary sodium and the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) diet
.
N Engl J Med
2001
;
344
:
3
10

27

Binia
A
,
Jaeger
J
,
Hu
Y
et al. .
Daily potassium intake and sodium-to-potassium ratio in the reduction of blood pressure: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials
.
J Hypertens
2015
;
33
:
1509
1520

28

Kaiser
J
.
Industry groups petition for data on salt and hypertension
.
Science
2003
;
300
:
1350

29

Sacks
FM
,
Campos
H
.
Dietary therapy in hypertension
.
N Engl J Med
2010
;
362
:
2102
2112

30

Cook
NR
,
Cutler
JA
,
Obarzanek
E
et al. .
Long term effects of dietary sodium reduction on cardiovascular disease outcomes: observational follow-up of the trials of hypertension prevention (TOHP)
.
BMJ
2007
;
334
:
885
888

31

Cook
NR
,
Obarzanek
E
,
Cutler
JA
et al. .
Joint effects of sodium and potassium intake on subsequent cardiovascular disease: the Trials of Hypertension Prevention follow-up study
.
Arch Intern Med
2009
;
169
:
32
40

32

Cook
NR
,
Appel
LJ
,
Whelton
PK
.
Lower levels of sodium intake and reduced cardiovascular risk
.
Circulation
2014
;
129
:
981
989

33

Cohen
HW
,
Alderman
MH
.
Letter from Cohen and Alderman regarding article, “lower levels of sodium intake and reduced cardiovascular risk”
.
Circulation
2014
;
130
:
e268

34

Graudal
N
,
Galløe
A
,
Garred
P
.
Modest salt restriction in older people
.
Lancet
1997
;
350
:
1702

35

Graudal
NA
,
Hubeck-Graudal
T
,
Jurgens
G
et al. .
The significance of duration and dose of sodium reduction intervention in normotensive and hypertensive individuals. A meta-analysis
.
Adv Nutr
2015
;
6
:
169
177

36

Adler
AJ
,
Taylor
F
,
Martin
N
et al. .
Reduced dietary salt for the prevention of cardiovascular disease
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
2014
;
12
:
CD009217

37

Mozaffarian
D
,
Fahimi
S
,
Singh
GM
et al. .
Global sodium consumption and death from cardiovascular causes
.
N Engl J Med
2014
;
37
:
624
634

38

Mente
A
,
O'Donnell
MJ
,
Rangarajan
S
et al. .
Association of urinary sodium and potassium excretion with blood pressure
.
N Engl J Med
2014
;
371
:
601
611

39

Brooks
TA
.
Private acts and public objects: an investigation of citer motivations
.
J Am Soc Inf Sci
1985
;
36
:
223
229

40

Folkow
B
.
On bias in medical research; reflections on present salt-cholesterol controversies
.
Scand Cardiovasc J
2011
;
45
:
194
197

41

Graudal
N
.
A radical sodium reduction policy is not supported by randomized controlled trials or observational studies: grading the evidence
.
J Hypertens
2016
;
29
:
543
8

Comments

1 Comment
Reducing salt intake at the population level, a comment to the comment
6 September 2016
Niels Graudal
MD, Copenhagen University Hospital, Rigshospitalet

Dr. Cappucio uses the word "hostile" to describe the scientific disagreement expressed in the present author's con view, but paradoxically at the same time uses a series of negatively charged words such as "scarce mongering", "false", "conspiracy biasing", unscrupulous", "surreptituos", "fabrication", "irresponsible", "flawed", and "unreliable". From a scientific position Dr. Cappucio's repetition of his belief in a series of public health institutions, in some of which Dr. Cappucio is a scientific advisor, is still not relevant. Neither is the food industry, which has not been involved in the long series of university performed RCTs on BP, and population studies on mortality. Sodium is an essential element for all animals, and therefore a nutrient. Any nutrient, even water, is toxic if overdosed. In contrast none of the known 70 carcinogens in tobacco smoke are essential nutrients. Therefore the comparison of sodium with tobacco is not appropriate. The question is not whether sodium is a nutrient; the question is which dose of sodium leads to adverse effects. I agree with Dr. Cappucio that habitual/usual is not necessarily "normal" and this is why I use the word "usual" and not the word "normal" to describe the habitual sodium intake. In contrast I disagree with Dr. Cappucio that "adequate" is "normal". "Adequate" is "low normal" but is not necessarily optimal. I agree with Dr. Cappucio that there is not much difference between the various meta-analyses on the effect of sodium reduction on BP. The small differences between them can be ascribed to differences in baseline BP of the included studies. The higher the average baseline BP of the included studies is, the higher is the effect. In study populations with a baseline BP in the lowest 50 percentile of the general population, the BP effect is negligible, and in the 50-75 percentile interval the effect is about 1.5 mmHg. These effects are hardly sufficient to recommend general sodium reduction for the whole population. I agree that treatment with diuretics reduces stroke mortality and other CVD events due to the fall in BP, but only in hypertensives. BP reducing treatment has never been proven to reduce mortality in normotensive, healthy individuals, whereas a further reduction of a normal BP in diabetes patients with anti-hypertensive drugs in contrast increases cardiovascular mortality and has no effect on all-cause mortality (1). If Dr. Cappucio knows of any published positions dismissing my concerns about the TOHP studies and the DASH study I would be pleased to receive a reference. For instance, have the DASH authors dismissed that the potassium content of the control diet in the DASH study is 1600 mg? WASH was founded by Dr. Graham MacGregor in 2005. Dr. MacGregor is a co- author on all 10 studies and many of the remaining authors, of whom several are members of WASH, including Dr. Cappucio, appear on several of the papers. I think it is reasonable to consider these studies as appearing from the same group. Finally, Dr. Cappucio has objections against population studies. In principle the design of population studies is not optimal and therefore the evidence from such studies is not on the same level as the evidence from RCTs. The number of potential confounders is infinite, the risk of residual confounding can never be excluded and the individual measurements are often imprecise. This is also the case for population studies which relates imprecisely measured blood pressure with mortality. However, attempts to reduce the significance of the confounders (age, cholesterol, smoking, history of CVD and diabetes) mentioned by Dr. Cappucio in the population studies, did not attenuate the relationship between low salt intake and mortality, but strengthened the relationship. In this context it is important to emphasize that there is no signal in any of these studies to indicate that a low sodium intake is beneficial.

1) Brunstroem M, Carlberg B. Effect of antihypertensive treatment at different blood pressure levels in patients with diabetes mellitus: systematic review and meta-analyses. BMJ. 2016 Feb 24;352:i717.

Conflict of Interest:

None declared

Submitted on 06/09/2016 8:00 PM GMT
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.