Abstract

Influential models of cortical organization propose a close relationship between heteromodal association areas and highly connected hubs in the default mode network. The “gradient model” of cortical organization proposes a close relationship between these areas and highly connected hubs in the default mode network, a set of cortical areas deactivated by demanding tasks. Here, we used a decision-making task and representational similarity analysis with classic “empathy for pain” stimuli to probe the relationship between high-level representations of imminent pain in others and these areas. High-level representations were colocalized with task deactivations or the transitions from activations to deactivations. These loci belonged to 2 groups: those that loaded on the high end of the principal cortical gradient and were associated by meta-analytic decoding with the default mode network, and those that appeared to accompany functional repurposing of somatosensory cortex in the presence of visual stimuli. These findings suggest that task deactivations may set out cortical areas that host high-level representations. We anticipate that an increased understanding of the cortical correlates of high-level representations may improve neurobiological models of social interactions and psychopathology.

Introduction

Early models of cortical organization based on data on axonal projections from nonhuman primates have drawn attention to a qualitative distinction concerning high-level cortical areas, such as those in prefrontal cortex and heteromodal association areas (Goldman-Rakic 1988; Mesulam 1990). These areas are characterized by sparse long-range connectivity, absent in upstream unimodal cortical areas dedicated to progressively encoding incoming stimuli. A related notion is that of cortical “converge zones” to integrate information that is fragmentally represented in unimodal areas (Damasio 1989). These models suggest a hierarchical organization to achieve increasing abstraction and combination of information from multiple modalities (Mesulam 1990).

More recently, the study of connectivity in man with neuroimaging methods has delivered data largely consistent with this distinction (Yeo et al. 2011). Graphic models of connectivity, for example, have identified a network of “core hubs” located upstream of sensory areas (van den Heuvel and Sporns 2013; see also Bassett and Bullmore 2006; Sporns et al. 2007; Braga et al. 2013). Among studies of resting state connectivity, the “gradient model” has recently characterized the cortex in terms of a collection of connectivity gradients, where each cortical point occupies a position representing topological distance to the gradient poles. The main gradient contains the highly connected core hubs of heteromodal association areas at one pole and sensory/motor areas at the other (Margulies et al. 2016; Huntenburg et al. 2018; Smallwood et al. 2021), capturing the functional gamut from perception and action to high-level cognitive functions. Confirming earlier observations (Buckner et al. 2009), these studies also found the heteromodal association pole to be partially coextensive with the default mode network, a set of cortical areas deactivated by cognitively demanding tasks (Shulman et al. 1997; Raichle et al. 2001). However, it remains difficult to characterize the computations that take place in these core hubs (Avena-Koenigsberger et al. 2018).

Because of their topological distance to primary sensory and motor areas (Mesulam 1990), the hubs at the high end of the cortical gradient may be involved in computations that require decoupling from the environment, as in the creation of an internal mental space populated by high-level semantic representations (Binder et al. 1999; Buckner et al. 2008; Spreng et al. 2008; Smallwood et al. 2013, 2021). Recent meta-analyses also suggest these hubs to be involved in social cognition (Schurz et al. 2021), confirming observations of previous studies (Spreng et al. 2008; Schurz et al. 2014). A second gradient in the same decomposition places visual and sensorimotor areas at opposite ends (Margulies et al. 2016).

In the present study, we employed visual stimuli used in the classic “empathy for pain” neuroimaging literature (Jackson et al. 2005; for review, see Lamm et al. 2011) to probe the cortical organization of representations of anticipated pain in others at high levels of abstraction (Fig. 1A). These stimuli show hands or feet in scenes in which injuries of different types are imminent (painful images), or control scenes where the same situation does not imply a possible injury (neutral images). Our aim was to provide evidence for the embedding of these representations within the large-scale cortical organization of the brain described above, including their relationship with the default mode network probed by task deactivations, and their position within the cortical gradient defined by the gradient model. We intended to use this position to reciprocally inform the interpretation of function of cortical areas, and of the nature of computations at the high end of the cortical gradient.

A) Examples of images from Jackson et al. (2005), showing trial arrangements leading to trials with high evidence for decisions (one image painful, one neutral) and trials with low evidence (both images of the same type). Note that this design makes it possible to select trial mages so that in the contrast for decision evidence the same images are involved in both terms of the contrast. B) Representational similarity maps used in the study (rearranged to highlight structure). C) Boxplot of linear predictions of right-side choice (random effects of trials) in mixed trials and painful/painful or neutral/neutral trial (left), and density of predicted probabilities of choice of right side for trials in the 2 conditions of the experiment (right).
Fig. 1

A) Examples of images from Jackson et al. (2005), showing trial arrangements leading to trials with high evidence for decisions (one image painful, one neutral) and trials with low evidence (both images of the same type). Note that this design makes it possible to select trial mages so that in the contrast for decision evidence the same images are involved in both terms of the contrast. B) Representational similarity maps used in the study (rearranged to highlight structure). C) Boxplot of linear predictions of right-side choice (random effects of trials) in mixed trials and painful/painful or neutral/neutral trial (left), and density of predicted probabilities of choice of right side for trials in the 2 conditions of the experiment (right).

Beside the widely used pain vs neutral contrast, we used 2 distinct approaches to characterize the cortical correlates of representations of pain. The first draws on a binary decision making design (Heekeren et al. 2008; Rangel and Clithero 2014) to identify areas associated with evidence for imminent physical pain in the visual stimuli. The decision making framework provides an explicit formulation of the high-level representations based on which decisions are made: the evidence for the decision (Heekeren et al. 2008; Shadlen and Kiani 2013). These representations may be the furthest removed from the encoding of specific stimuli, as they may be directly translated into motor responses (Cisek 2012), and are therefore candidates for being hosted at the high end of the cortical gradient (Viviani et al. 2020). Furthermore, they emerge in a distributed network that integrates signals from different parts of the brain into a low-dimensional criterion (Cisek 2012; Yoo and Hayden 2018; Fine et al. 2021). In the current study, participants were presented with 2 visual scenes and were asked to rate them with the question “which situation is more painful?” We assumed that evidence for decisions was large when one scene represented a painful situation and the other represented a non-painful or neutral situation (“mixed trials”). In contrast, we reasoned that there was little evidence to decide between 2 images when both depicted the same type of situations (painful/painful or neutral/neutral, Fig. 1A). Mixed trials and trials with the same type of situations correspond to the “easy” and “difficult decisions” trials of the perceptual decision making literature (Heekeren et al. 2008).

The second approach took stock of the evidence that semantic representations of stimuli are encoded in the cortex as vectors of activity in a local vector space (Haxby et al. 2014), whose directions vary in each individual. To seek evidence of the pattern of activity associated with the property of stimuli we used a searchlight-based representational similarity analysis approach (RSA, Kriegeskorte et al. 2008). This approach seeks evidence of the encoding of constructs and of semantic properties of stimuli by assessing the concordance of the representational similarity of these properties and the pattern of covariance in the brain signal elicited in each trial. We computed the representational similarity of each trial for recognizable figural elements of the images (such as feet or hands) as well as in terms of high-level features related to the final criterion for the decision (the imminent possibility of pain, i.e. the criterion to compute the evidence for a decision in the trial, Fig. 1B).

Our hypotheses were designed to provide insights about the computations hosted at the high-end terminals of the cortical gradients. First, we hypothesized that the high-level representations elicited by the evidence for decisions would colocalize with task deactivations (taken as functional localizers of the DMN) and areas at the high end of the gradient model, consistently with their high degree of abstractness. We also hypothesized that encoding of simple categories, such as body parts, would be located in more upstream areas, due to their more concrete character. These 2 hypotheses characterize 2 types of computational outcomes corresponding to the different connectivity of the cortex at different points of the gradient: distributed connectivity for evidence for decisions (Viviani et al. 2020), and local connectivity for categorization. We had no information from previous studies to form hypotheses on the RSA of the criterion for decisions (the imminent possibility of pain), but we assumed it to be located near evidence areas.

Materials and methods

Participants

The functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study was conducted at the Psychiatry and Psychotherapy Clinic of the University of Ulm, Germany, after approval by the Ethical Review Board. Healthy participants (n = 50) were recruited through local announcements and admitted to the study after providing written informed consent. One participant was excluded due to failure to record responses during the scan, leaving n = 49 for the final sample (mean age 24.7, standard deviation 5.1, range 19–47; 26 women).

Experimental design

Participants were shown 2 pictures of right hands or feet in situations of anticipated physical pain or neutral outcomes (source: Jackson et al. 2005). In each trial, participants were instructed to decide which situation in the 2 pictures could lead to the most painful outcome. We selected images from the original study showing hands or feet with equal frequency in each outcome and matching the type of injury causing pain (Fig. 1A). Pictures were displayed side by side for 2.5 s. After this time, blue dots appeared under the pictures to indicate participants could communicate their decision by pressing the left or right button, depending on the side of the anticipated painful outcome. If after 1.5 s no button was pressed, the trial was declared as a miss. Trial onsets were generated with a Poisson interval schedule of mean intensity of a trial every 14.75 s, bounded to a minimal interarrival time of 13 s.

To investigate the neural correlates of evidence for decisions in an interpersonal context, we looked for a signal associated with the evidence for a decision, as in sensory-based decision making studies (Heekeren et al. 2004). To form trials where evidence for decisions was high, we paired painful with neutral pictures as decision options. We paired painful with painful or neutral with neutral images to form low evidence trials. There were all combinations of outcomes (12 trials painful/neutral, 6 trials painful/painful, 6 trials neutral/neutral). Lateralization of pictures showing painful outcomes was balanced across the painful/neutral trials. The contrast of interest, representing high vs low evidence, subtracted the activity of painful/painful and neutral/neutral trials from the painful/neutral trials. The allocation of pictures to these combinations was swapped between participants such that in the overall sample the same pictures occurred in the painful/neutral and in the painful/painful or neutral/neutral combinations. Because the contrast high evidence vs low evidence contained the same images in the 2 conditions that form this contrast, it was not confounded by intrinsic properties of the images, such as their salience, the presence of imminent pain itself, or by any other property that was not matched between painful and neutral images.

The decision making design is validated by numerous functional imaging studies (for review, see Heekeren et al. 2008). The time course of neural activity in decision making tasks is well characterized by sequential sampling models, in which evidence for the available options is accumulated up to a boundary indicating sufficient evidence for one option (Gold and Shadlen 2002; Heekeren et al. 2008; Shadlen and Kiani 2013; for neuroimaging studies, see Ploran et al. 2007; Lim et al. 2011; Wheeler et al. 2015). Here, we only focus on the existence of a neuroimaging signal correlated with the evidence for decisions. In functional imaging studies of perceptual decision making, the contrast used to detect neural activity associated with evidence for decisions is referred to as the “easy vs difficult” decisions contrast (Fig. 1A), because when the evidence for decisions is large the decision is easy (Heekeren et al. 2004, 2008).

Data were collected in a Prisma 3 T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen) using a T2*-sensitive echo-planar imaging sequence (TR/TE 1,970/36 ms, bandwidth 1,776 Hz/pixel, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2, 32 transversal slices acquired in ascending order, slice thickness 2.5 mm with a slice gap of 0.625 mm, field of view 192 with matrix size 64, giving in-planar voxel size 3 x 3 mm). In each participant, 182 scans were acquired after reaching equilibration giving a total scan duration of 6 min.

Statistical analysis of behavioral data

Behavioral data were analyzed with the freely available statistical software R (http://www.R-project.org/) using the package lme4 (version 1.1-26, function glmer, Bates et al. 2015). To demonstrate the tendency to consistently choose one of the options, a logistic regression model where choice of the right side button was arbitrarily coded as “success” was fitted with an intercept and trials (identified by the combination of pictures of the trial) as random effects. The variance of the random effect indicates the tendency to depart from equal probability choices for right or left. To compute significance of the increased variance of trials with painful/neutral images (indicating a tendency to consistently choose right or left in high evidence trials), relative to trials with painful/painful or neutral/neutral images (low evidence trials), a likelihood ratio test was computed between a model with one random effect for all trials and a model with heteroscedastic random effects for high and low evidence trials. The density in Fig. 1C was computed on random effects of fitted choice probabilities with the package ggplot2 (Wickham 2009) using a Gaussian kernel with bandwidth 0.08.

Statistical analysis of neuroimaging data

Functional imaging data were preprocessed with the freely available software SPM12 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) running on Matlab (The MathWorks, Natick, MA). In the main analysis, brain data were realigned, registered to a Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template (2 mm isotropic resampling size), and smoothed (FWHM 8 mm). At the first level, the brain signal was modeled by convolving a box-car function corresponding to the trials (2.5 s) with a canonical BOLD function. Trials of 3 types were coded as separate regressors: trials with a painful and a neutral scene, trials with both painful scenes, and trials with both neutral scenes. Further confounder regressors were 6 head movement displacements estimated from realignment, and 4 regressors for denoising (Behzadi et al. 2007). Denoising is indicated in RSA (described below) as it may improve detection power (Charest et al. 2018). The denoising confounder regressors were obtained from the first 4 principal components of the data in the voxels classified as belonging to the lateral ventricles, white matter, and bone in the segmentations conducted by SPM12 as part of the registration procedure. The number of denoising regressors was based on the results reported by Kay et al. (2013). Data and regressors were high-pass filtered (128 s). Serial dependency of observations was modeled with an AR(1) autoregressive term fitted to pooled residuals. Two contrasts were computed: trials with painful and neutral scenes vs trials with scenes of the same type (evidence for decisions) and trials with only painful vs trials with only neutral scenes (standard contrast pain vs control). After estimating the model in each voxel separately at the first level using the SPM12 software, contrast volumes from each subject were brought to the second level to account for the random effect of subjects. Inference was obtained using a permutation method (8,000 resamples) to correct for multiple testing at the voxel and cluster level (cluster definition threshold, P < 0.001).

The RSA was conducted on non-smoothed data in volumes in the original space (i.e. non-registered) from coefficient estimates of a model in which each trial was modeled as a separate regressor (Kriegeskorte et al. 2008), with the same confounding covariates and autoregressive term as in the main model. We then formed the correlation matrices of the estimated model coefficients in a searchlight sphere (diameter 8 mm, 256 voxels) prior to computing the partial correlation of the off-diagonal terms of this matrix with those of the similarity matrices characterizing the trial according to the properties of the stimuli, which were computed by counting the shared properties of elements pairwise. Here, the elements consisted of the whole display of the individual trials, giving similarity matrices for trials. The properties of the trials that were used in the RSA were the anticipation of pain or no anticipation for each of the shown pictures, the body parts in the pictures (hands or feet) and the evidence for the decision (high or low evidence decisions, as a confounder; see Fig. 1B). The searchlight sphere was computed with the appropriate function in the SPM12 software (spm_searchlight). We used a partial correlation approach to adjust for possible confounders (high or low evidence for decisions) in the RSA for body parts and imminent pain and to counter possible bias due to the non-orthogonality of the BOLD-convolved regressors in the design matrix (for details, see Viviani 2021). Correlation values from the RSA were then registered with the parameters computed for the main analysis (resampling voxel size, 2 mm isotropic) and brought to the second level, where they were smoothed (full width at half-maximum, FWHM 4 mm) and processed in one-sample t-tests using permutation (8,000 resamples) to correct for multiple tests at the voxel and cluster level (cluster definition threshold, P < 0.001).

Coordinates in text, figures and tables of the main and the representation similarity analyses are expressed in the MNI standard space. Overlays of statistical parametric maps were obtained with the software package MRIcroN (Chris Rorden, freely available at https://people.cas.sc.edu/rorden/mricron/index.html).

Decoding analysis

The decoding analysis was obtained with the online tool available at the Neurosynth site (https://www.neurosynth.org, Yarkoni et al. 2011). The topics terms shown in the Results were obtained from the first 50 associations generated by the decoder after removing all anatomical terms and combining semantic duplicates (for example, “tactile” and “touch” were considered equivalent).

The average load on the principal gradient of cortical organization defined by connectivity was based on the data of Margulies et al. (2016), publicly available at the NeuroVault.org site (Gorgolewski et al. 2015) at the web site identifier https://identifiers.org/neurovault.image:31997. The significant clusters of the effect of evidence, of the pain vs neutral contrast, and of the RSAs of body parts and imminent pain were resampled on the space of the gradient data. Because of their intrinsically lower spatial resolution and/or smoothing, these clusters overstep the boundaries of the cortical mantle. To compute the average load on the main connectivity gradient, they were masked for the voxels where the gradient is present in the principal gradient data.

Results

Behavioral data

There were an average of 0.53 missed trials per participant in mixed trials and of 0.90 in pain/pain or neutral/neutral trials. This difference was significant (Poisson regression with repeated measurements, z = 2.13, P = 0.018, one-tailed). Furthermore, in mixed trials, participants gave the correct response in an average of 83.7% of trials (95% confidence interval 79.5–87.1), indicating that participants were executing the task and identifying the correct response.

Pain/pain or neutral/neutral trials had no clear correct answer, preventing the computation of an equivalent statistic. To show the difference in the existence of a clear answer in the 2 types of trials, we fitted a random effects logistic regression where we modeled 2 separate random components for trials in the mixed and in the pain/pain or neutral/neutral trials. When choices are random, the linear predictor of a logistic regression model hovers around 0, while trials with a tendency to generate a right or a left choice inflate the random component of the trial type as linear predictions for trials are correspondingly positive or negative. The variance of the random effect of mixed trial was 8.2, of pain/pain or neutral/neutral trials 2.1 (⁠|${\chi}_{(1)}^2$| = 6.1, P = 0.013), confirming that mixed trials were prevalently associated with high evidence or “easy decisions” in contrast to trials with pain/pain or neutral/neutral images. Figure 1C shows a box plot of the linear predictions in the 2 types of trials, and the density of fitted trial probabilities of right-side choice. The boxplot shows the larger dispersion of linear predictions in mixed trials, as participants were more consistently choosing one of the scenes on the right or left. The density of fitted probabilities in mixed trials was bimodal, meaning that there was a tendency to select a right or left response, while the density of pain/pain or neutral/neutral trials had one mode at about 50% chance of right or left choice. However, there were a few mixed trials where participants were not entirely consistent in selecting one option, and several pain/pain or neutral/neutral trials where participants were expressing a clear preference. There was no difference in the dispersion of the random effects for pain/pain vs neutral/neural trials (⁠|${\chi}_{(1)}^2$| < 0.01, n.s.).

Additional analysis of behavioral data is in the Supplementary Material available online.

Neuroimaging data

Evidence for decisions

In the functional imaging data analysis, we first looked at the neural correlates of evidence for decisions. As in Heekeren et al. (2004), we contrasted high vs low evidence decisions, i.e. mixed trials vs trials where both scenes were of the same type. This contrast revealed a sparse network of areas involving the motor/premotor and somatosensory cortex, extending medially to the posterior cingulum (Fig. 2A and Table S1 in the Supplementary Material).

A) Parametric maps, contrast high vs low evidence decision (thresholded at P < 0.01 for illustration purposes), overlaid on a template brain. B) The same contrast, displayed together with task activations (yellow) and deactivations (light blue, same threshold).
Fig. 2

A) Parametric maps, contrast high vs low evidence decision (thresholded at P < 0.01 for illustration purposes), overlaid on a template brain. B) The same contrast, displayed together with task activations (yellow) and deactivations (light blue, same threshold).

More anteriorly, the middle/posterior insula, and the anterior cingulum/ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) were also involved. On the lateral aspect of the brain, the middle temporal gyrus/temporoparietal junction were also strongly associated with evidence for decisions (areas PGa and PFm in the histological classification by Caspers et al. 2006).

Several of these areas (such as the posterior cingulum and the vmPFC) are typically associated with the default mode network. We therefore looked at task deactivations, represented in light blue in Fig. 2B. One can see that the effect of evidence for the decision, detected by the high vs low evidence decision contrast, involved task deactivations not only in areas classically associated with the default mode network, but also in the insula and in the motor/premotor cortex. In the insula, the association with evidence for decisions was located at the transition between the activated anterior insular and the middle and posterior portions. In the motor/premotor cortex, the same association overlapped with task deactivations, especially marked in the medial face, extending anteriorly toward the task active premotor cortex.

For completeness, we should mention that there were effects in the opposite direction (contrast high vs low evidence decisions), involving task-activated cortex in the inferior frontal gyrus and the intraparietal sulcus (see Table S2 in the Supplementary Material). These are known effects, elicited by increases in the difficulty of cognitive tasks (e.g. Rypma et al. 1999) and involved in stimulus–response mapping (Bode and Haynes 2009; Woolgar et al. 2011). They may represent attentional and executive processes required by the decision making task that are complementary to the activity elicited by high evidence decisions (for a discussion, see Heekeren et al. 2008). Since they are not the focus of the present study, we will not comment on them further.

Contrast pain vs neutral images

One question we wanted to address was the relationship of the effects of evidence for decisions with those emerging from a more traditional contrast of painful vs neutral stimuli. Because the trials with difficult decisions contained 2 images of the same type (painful or neutral), they could be used to estimate this contrast. The main effects of this contrast were localized in the SII/inferior parietal cortex (areas PFop, PFt, and OP1 in the classification by Caspers et al. 2006), anterior to the parietal effects of evidence and without substantial overlap (Fig. 3 and Table S3 in the Supplementary Material).

A) Parametric t map of the contrast painful images vs neutral (illustration threshold P < 0.01), overlaid on a template brain. B) The same contrast (blue color), displayed together with the effect of evidence for decisions (as in Fig. 2).
Fig. 3

A) Parametric t map of the contrast painful images vs neutral (illustration threshold P < 0.01), overlaid on a template brain. B) The same contrast (blue color), displayed together with the effect of evidence for decisions (as in Fig. 2).

An additional effect of the traditional contrast was in the middle insula. While some very limited overlap with the effects of evidence was observed here, most of the activity of the traditional contrast was located ventrally to the effect of evidence. There was no relation with any of the other effects of evidence documented in Table S1, Supplementary Material.

Representational similarity analysis

We then turned to the investigation of representations using the RSA approach. Because the scenes of impending pain figured either hands or feet, with equal frequency across the experiment, we first computed the RSA associated with these body parts to validate the effectiveness of our approach (for more details, see Viviani 2021). This analysis resulted in only one significant cluster of representational activity (MNI x, y, z: 54, −64, −2, right middle temporal gyrus, BA 37, t = 6.09, P = 0.001, peak level corrected, P = 0.006, cluster level corrected, 645 voxels; see Fig. 4A). This cluster matched the right association area for the term ‘body’ in the automated meta-analytic tool Neurosynth (Fig. 4B, Yarkoni et al. 2011; a similar map is generated by the term “hand”). This cluster was located at the lateral end of the large swath of task activity emanating from visual areas in the posterior occipital cortex (task activation at 54, −64, −2: t = 9.64, P < 0.001).

A) Parametric t map of the RSA analysis for hands/feet (illustration threshold P < 0.01), overlaid on a template brain. B) Neurosynth rendering for the term “body,” association test FDR P < 0.01.
Fig. 4

A) Parametric t map of the RSA analysis for hands/feet (illustration threshold P < 0.01), overlaid on a template brain. B) Neurosynth rendering for the term “body,” association test FDR P < 0.01.

We then looked at the representation of pain, based on the distinction between impending pain and neutral images presented in the trials. This similarity map captured the presence or absence of images containing the criterion based on which choices were made. A similarity map for evidence for decisions was used as a confounder and partialled out (Fig. 1B). This analysis showed extensive bilateral associations in the primary and associative sensory cortex (Fig. 5A, Table S4 in the Supplementary Material).

A) Parametric map of the RSA for pain (in blue-green), and B) The same map, shown with the contrast high vs low evidence decision (the same as shown in Fig. 2, in blue-yellow), overlaid on a template brain. Both contrasts were thresholded at P < 0.001 for illustration purposes.
Fig. 5

A) Parametric map of the RSA for pain (in blue-green), and B) The same map, shown with the contrast high vs low evidence decision (the same as shown in Fig. 2, in blue-yellow), overlaid on a template brain. Both contrasts were thresholded at P < 0.001 for illustration purposes.

When shown together with the neural correlates of evidence for the decision (Fig. 5B), the involvement of the somatosensory cortex in the RSA for imminent pain appeared to complement the prevalently motor involvement of evidence for decisions on the other side of the central sulcus. In the most ventral part, they reached the painful vs neutral effects of a traditional contrast in sensory association cortex, but were much more extensive than this latter.

Decoding analysis

To situate the areas identified in the study within the existing corpus of neuroimaging data, we looked at these 4 effects (evidence for decisions, contrast pain vs neural, RSA for body parts, RSA for imminent pain) with the automatic decoding tool provided by the Neurosynth website (see Methods for details). The decoding analysis, shown in Fig. 6A, confirmed that the effect of evidence for decisions involved mainly areas associated in previous studies with somatosensory and motor functions, together with the related body representations, and with the default mode network. An additional set of topics terms identified by the decoding analysis involved social cognition/theory of mind. The RSA of imminent pain was substantially a subset of these terms, centered on somatosensory and motor functions. In contrast, the pain vs neutral contrast and the RSA of body parts were decoded with topics terms that were specifically related to the content of the present analyses. The pain vs neutral effect identified topics terms associated with pain, somatosensory function, and, less specifically, with negative emotion. The topics term “empathy” was also elicited by this decoding, presumably in connection with ‘empathy for pain’ studies. As anticipated in Fig. 4, the RSA of body parts identified topic terms associated with visual encoding and identification of body parts.

A) Decoding analysis of the effects of evidence for decisions and the contrast pain vs neutral images (top), and of RSA of imminent pain and body parts (bottom). DMN: default mode network; TOM: theory of mind. B) Average load of significant clusters on connectivity gradient component. CGC load: Average load on connectivity gradient component: L, R: left, right. The anatomical labels refer to the peak of the cluster (from www.talairach.org). For more information about the clusters and their peaks, see the Tables in the Supplementary Material.
Fig. 6

A) Decoding analysis of the effects of evidence for decisions and the contrast pain vs neutral images (top), and of RSA of imminent pain and body parts (bottom). DMN: default mode network; TOM: theory of mind. B) Average load of significant clusters on connectivity gradient component. CGC load: Average load on connectivity gradient component: L, R: left, right. The anatomical labels refer to the peak of the cluster (from www.talairach.org). For more information about the clusters and their peaks, see the Tables in the Supplementary Material.

Average load on connectivity gradient component

To verify the position of the areas identified in our analysis within the main gradient of cortical organization defined by connectivity (Margulies et al. 2016), we computed the mean component values of this gradient for statistically significant clusters (Fig. 6B). This gradient is anchored at one end to areas serving mainly sensory and motor functions, and at the other end to heteromodal association areas serving abstract cognitive functions involving the default mode network (Margulies et al. 2016). We found that the clusters associated with evidence for decisions were characterized by high cortical gradient loadings, with the exception of the sensorimotor cluster, which loaded on the other end of the gradient. The clusters identified by the pain vs neutral contrast were also located toward the low end of this gradient. An exception was the cluster in the right inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, both RSAs gave clusters at the low end of the gradient, as appropriate for sensory representations.

Discussion

In the present study, we used a decision-making paradigm and RSA as strategies to make explicit the most abstract representations that the brain must compute to execute the task. Computationally, one may conceive of decisions as the extraction of increasingly high-level features of the stimulus up to a final stage in which neural activity computes the distance to a discriminating hyperplane, which is larger the greater the evidence for the decision (Yoo and Hayden 2018). In the present study, areas associated with evidence for decisions were consistent with those detected in previous studies that used visual stimuli (perceptual decision making, the posterior cingulum/precuneus, the posterior temporal gyrus and adjacent parietal areas, Heekeren et al. 2004), and with the social nature of the criterion used in the decision (the posterior cingulum, the middle insula, Viviani et al. 2018). Also areas reported in studies of preference-based decision making were detected (the posterior cingulum, the vmPFC, Clithero and Rangel 2014). In summary, recruitment of perceptual decision-making areas was consistent with the use of visual stimuli, and of those of social decision making with the criterion used in the decision.

Our first hypothesis was that these areas would involve the default mode network. Meta-analytic decoding confirmed that this was the case for some of these areas: the vmPFC and the posterior cingulum, the temporoparietal junction. These areas also loaded at the high end of the gradient model. The results of meta-analytic decoding may be viewed with caution, given that they necessarily reflect how activations were interpreted in the original studies. However, independent evidence on a specific role comes from the finding that these areas were colocated with task deactivations, or at the boundary between task activations and deactivations, replicating the findings of Viviani et al. (2020). This finding is consistent with the notion that the representations computed as evidence for deciding between options are hosted in the highly connected network located coextensively or in proximity of the default mode network, and that are identified by the cortical gradient model as convergence hubs. Furthermore, confirming our second hypothesis, the close association with task deactivations and the default mode network distinguished the areas associated with decision evidence from those elicited by a classic pain vs neutral contrast, or the RSA of body parts.

However, not all areas that were here associated with evidence for decisions followed this pattern. The sensorimotor cortex, in particular, is not part of the default mode network and is not located at the high end of the first gradient component. However, its recruitment is reported in studies of perception-based decision making that have demonstrated the emergence of a signal associated with evidence for decisions in the motor areas, among others (Gold and Shadlen 2002; Shadlen and Kiani 2013), leading to the inclusion of these areas in the network associated with evidence for decisions (Cisek 2012). While sensorimotor areas are characterized by poor long-range connectivity in studies based on diffusion tractography (Hagmann et al. 2008; Gong et al. 2009), they are included among the highly connected hubs in functional connectivity studies (Achard et al. 2006), confirming a role in the connectivity core. In the principal decomposition of connectivity data, the sensorimotor areas and the adjacent parietal operculum constitute the terminal end of the second component of the cortical gradient (Margulies et al. 2016).

Likewise, in the insula, a key area for encoding pain sensation (Lamm et al. 2011; Timmers et al. 2018; Jauniaux et al. 2019; Peyron and Fauchon 2019; Soyman et al. 2021) but not part of the default mode network, the locus associated with evidence for decisions was located in the middle portion, half-way between the active anterior and the mildly deactivated middle/posterior portions. The task deactivation gradient in the anterior–posterior direction follows connectivity gradients reported in the insula (Cerliani et al. 2012; Royer et al. 2020; see also the third gradient component in Margulies et al. 2016). However, for both posterior insula and somatosensory cortex, the interpretation of task deactivations is made difficult by activity decrements due to the visual modality of stimuli presentation (Langner et al. 2011).

When using RSA, we found that representations of imminent pain, which were not evidence for decisions but the criterion based on which this evidence could be computed, were colocalized in the sensorimotor cortex and in the sensory association area. The decoding analysis of these areas confirmed the prevalent association with motor and sensory tasks of previous studies. There are 2 possible interpretations of this finding. One is that the prevalent localization of its peaks in the sensory cortex and the extension into sensory association areas is a clue to the role of these areas in the representation of empathy for pain. Considerable evidence links the sensory association area to decoding emotional expressions in others (Adolphs et al. 2000; Keysers et al. 2010). Another interpretation is that this cortex may be representing information for the decision generically, much like the motor cortex represents information about the choice to be taken irrespective of the features that determine this choice. In favor of this latter interpretation are the results of a recent RSA analysis of a classic cognitive Stroop task, which found a similar correlate of cognitive control in this region (Freund, Bugg, and Braver 2021; Freund, Etzel and Braver 2021), even if emotion recognition played no role in that study. Together with motor or premotor areas, the postcentral sensory cortex also appears in studies of categorization applying machine learning approaches (Li et al. 2007; Davis and Poldrack 2014; Braunlich and Seger 2016) or tracking distance to decision boundaries (Seger et al. 2015) that required discrimination between abstract patterns or food categories. Because the task inherent in these discriminations did not involve information of social-cognitive nature, the involvement of postcentral sensory cortex may be non-specific for the decision type.

In contrast, the results of the RSA of a concrete feature of the visual scenes, the displayed body parts, were much more specific, and matched the visual association area for the topics term “body” in the meta-analytic decoding. It also closely replicated the findings of Jackson et al. (2005), obtained with the same stimulus material using a traditional contrast approach. This portion of the lateral occipital cortex that has been univocally associated with encoding task-relevant visual object features in conventional studies (Edelman et al. 1998; Downing et al. 2001; Grill-Spector et al. 2001; Braunlich et al. 2017). This area was also activated by the task, in contrast to those involved in evidence for decisions. Likewise, the effects of the contrast pain vs no pain involved the anterior inferior parietal/somatoform association area, as in meta-analyses of “empathy for pain” studies (Timmers et al. 2018; Jauniaux et al. 2019). Hence, in interpreting function, the issue arises if the same criteria should be applied to upstream perceptual areas, which compute increasingly abstract features but are still related to the perceptual modality involved, and to downstream areas linked together by long-range connectivity and involving, according to the gradient model, the default mode network. In a computational perspective, it has been suggested that downstream areas may coordinate to provide low-dimensional priors or schematic information to disambiguate signal from upstream areas (Hinton and Zemel 1994; Friston 2005). What may characterize them, relative to feedback involving the extraction of features within a sensory modality (Rao and Ballard 1999), is that the computation emerges from the interaction with other network hubs, recruited as appropriate, thus appearing in studies with heterogeneous content and defying a simple functional characterization.

Models proposed in the literature to explain recruitment of vmPFC, found here to be associated with evidence for decisions and part of the default mode network (Shulman et al. 1997), exemplify these interpretive challenges. Studies of preference-based decision-making tasks have proposed vmPFC to compute the integrated value and costs of a choice into a single summary criterion (Levy and Glimcher 2012; Rangel and Clithero 2014), irrespective of whether participants made the choices for themselves or another person (Kross et al. 2011). Recent analyses of the literature, however, have proposed a more general role for vmPFC, not limited to the computations of subjective value (Rudebeck and Murray 2014; Wilson et al. 2014; Ciaramelli et al. 2019; Mack et al. 2020; Zhou et al. 2021), consisting in selecting the information, hosted elsewhere, that is relevant to compute predictions of the consequences of responses. Recruitment of vmPFC in the present study is consistent with these revised models, especially with views of the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in decision making that emphasize its role in computing predictions of future outcomes (Rudebeck and Murray 2014). In the stimulus set used in the present study, the pictures suggested an injury about to happen, but no actual injury was shown. In contrast, vmPFC was not associated with evidence for decisions in a social cognition task in which all relevant information was contained in the facial expression of the displayed individuals (Viviani et al. 2018). These findings are consistent with vmPFC providing top–down disambiguating schematic information, where the disambiguation consists of the computation of a predictive criterion.

More posteriorly, the evidence for decisions was associated with activity in the temporoparietal junction and posterior temporal areas at the boundary to task deactivations. This involvement was responsible for the inclusion of theory of mind terms in the decoding analysis of this contrast (Caspers et al. 2010). This finding is consistent with the model advanced by Schurz et al. (2021), according to which the cortical substrates of social cognition largely overlap with the default mode network/heteromodal association areas of the gradient model (Schilbach et al. 2008; Spreng et al. 2008). In the interpretive perspective adopted here, however, functional attributions of the temporoparietal junction would differ from those of more upstream areas such as the lateral occipital cortex, here recruited for the representation of body parts, or the somatosensory association cortex, associated in the literature with the representation of pain. Instead, its recruitment may be seen as evidence of activation of schematic or conceptual information (Murphy et al. 2018; Smallwood et al. 2021), determined dynamically in interaction with other high-level connected cortical nodes. In the right temporoparietal junction, this may be schematic spatial and visual information to identify target stimuli (Shulman et al. 2007); in the left, integration of semantic information (Lanzoni et al. 2020).

Similar considerations may apply to the posterior cingulum/retrosplenial cortex, recruited here by the evidence for decisions, and a hub of numerous long-range connections with diverse cortical regions (Hagmann et al. 2008; Leech et al. 2012; van den Heuvel and Sporns 2013). This recruitment has been interpreted by some, including ourselves, as the neural correlate of psychic pain (O'Connor 2012; Labek et al. 2017), following consistent findings in several studies (Gündel et al. 2003; Kersting et al. 2009; Labek et al. 2017). However, this area is also recruited together with vmPFC by studies of subjective preference in the appetitive domain (Clithero and Rangel 2014; Viviani et al. 2019). A more general proposal about the function of this area is attentional redirection in the presence of stimuli of behavioral significance (Pearson et al. 2011; Leech and Sharp 2014). Rather than with a simple mapping between cortical areas and emotional content, these findings are consistent with accounts that emphasize the high level of conceptual elaboration of emotional information when detected in the default mode network (Satpute and Lindquist 2019).

In summary, we found that high-level representations related to the decision task were organized in a distributed network, as in models and data regarding decision making studies. Furthermore, they appeared to involve 2 types of task deactivations: one related to areas typically associated with the default mode network, as postulated by the gradient model, the other related to areas that may have been repurposed to host representations of relevance for the task at hand. Accordingly, activity attributable to evidence formation appeared to involve at least 2 cortical gradients defined by connectivity, suggesting that our initial hypothesis, involving the principal gradient only, was too simplistic. In agreement with recent proposals (Margulies et al. 2016; Smallwood et al. 2021), we argued that the functional characterization of these areas in terms of simple mappings is unlikely, especially considering proposals in the literature on the abstract nature of high-level representations and the distributed character of their computations. This contrasts with the more univocal interpretation of areas representing more elementary categories, such as body parts, or of areas identified by the subtraction of signal in neutral from pain images.

Future studies may be able to characterize the role of the connected core network of distributed hubs in clinical models. This issue arises from the observation that personality disorders, in particular, may be associated with changes in empathic processes (Ripoll et al. 2013; Luyten and Fonagy 2015; Sosic-Vasic et al. 2019) and poor representations of interpersonal events (Skodol et al. 2011). Furthermore, it has been noted that areas located at or in proximity of task deactivations are often recruited in tasks of emotion regulation (Viviani 2013), suggesting a role in controlling the encoding of representations of social and emotional relevance (Viviani 2014; Messina et al. 2016). Hence, insights from connectivity studies may facilitate the integration of models of semantic processing with the clinical neuroscience literature.

Data availability

Parametric maps of the results of this study are available at the NeuroVault site (web identifier: https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:11616).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Jean Decety for kindly providing the images of impending pain we used in the experiment. We would also like to thank Sabine Wehner, Jeff Maerz, Stefanie Bernardin, and Eun-Jin Sim for technical support.

Funding

This work was funded by collaborative grants from the Federal Institute for Drugs and Medical Devices (BfArM, Bonn, Germany) (Grant No. V-17568/68502/2017-2020); by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) (Grant No. I 5903-B) (ERA-PERMED project ArtiPro); by the “Forschungsförderungsmitteln der Nachwuchsförderung 2018 der Universität Innsbruck,” project “Analyse neuronaler Populationscodes physischer und psychischer Schmerzen mit multivariaten Musterinformationsanalysen - Anwendung der ‘Functional MRI Representational Similarity Analyses’ bei funktioneller Bildgebung”; and by the Förderkreis of the University of Innsbruck (Förderkreis 1669).

Conflict of interest statement: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Software availability

The SPM12 add-on software used to compute the RSA using the partial correlation approach is available in a public repository (https://github.com/roberto-viviani/rsa-rsm.git).

References

Achard
 
S
,
Salvador
 
R
,
Whitcher
 
B
,
Suckling
 
J
,
Bullmore
 
ET
.
A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs
.
J Neurosci
.
2006
:
26
:
63
72
.

Adolphs
 
R
,
Damasio
 
H
,
Tranel
 
D
,
Cooper
 
G
,
Damasio
 
AR
.
A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping
.
J Neurosci
.
2000
:
20
:
2683
2690
.

Avena-Koenigsberger
 
A
,
Misic
 
B
,
Sporns
 
O
.
Communication dynamics in complex brain networks
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2018
:
19
:
17
33
.

Bassett
 
DS
,
Bullmore
 
E
.
Small-world brain networks
.
Neurosicenstist
.
2006
:
12
(
6
):
512
523
.

Bates
 
D
,
Mächler
 
M
,
Bolker
 
BM
,
Walker
 
SC
.
Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4
.
J Stat Softw
.
2015
:
67
:
1
.

Behzadi
 
Y
,
Restom
 
K
,
Liau
 
J
,
Liu
 
TT
.
A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI
.
NeuroImage
.
2007
:
37
:
90
101
.

Binder
 
JR
,
Frost
 
JA
,
Hammeke
 
TA
,
Bellgowan
 
PS
,
Rao
 
SM
,
Cox
 
RW
.
Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional MRI study
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
1999
:
11
:
80
95
.

Bode
 
S
,
Haynes
 
JD
.
Decoding sequential stages of task preparation in the human brain
.
NeuroImage
.
2009
:
45
:
606
613
.

Braga
 
RM
,
Sharp
 
DJ
,
Lesson
 
C
,
Wise
 
RJS
,
Leech
 
R
.
Echoes of the brain within default mode, association, and heteromodal cortices
.
J Neurosci
.
2013
:
33
:
14031
14039
.

Braunlich
 
K
,
Seger
 
CA
.
Categorical evidence, confidence, and urgency during probabilistic categorization
.
NeuroImage
.
2016
:
125
:
941
952
.

Braunlich
 
K
,
Liu
 
Z
,
Seger
 
CA
.
Occipitotemporal category representations are sensitive to abstract category boundaries defined by generalization demands
.
J Neurosci
.
2017
:
37
:
7631
7642
.

Buckner
 
RL
,
Andrews-Hanna
 
JR
,
Schacter
 
DL
.
The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
.
2008
:
1124
:
1
38
.

Buckner
 
RL
,
Sepulcre
 
J
,
Talukdar
 
T
,
Krienen
 
FM
,
Liu
 
H
,
Hedden
 
T
,
Andrews-Hanna
 
JR
,
Sperling
 
RA
,
Johnson
 
KA
.
Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer's disease
.
J Neurosci
.
2009
:
29
:
1860
1873
.

Caspers
 
S
,
Geyer
 
S
,
Schleicher
 
A
,
Mohlberg
 
H
,
Amunts
 
K
,
Zilles
 
K
.
The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability
.
NeuroImage
.
2006
:
33
:
430
448
.

Caspers
 
S
,
Zilles
 
K
,
Laird
 
AR
,
Eickhoff
 
SB
.
ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain
.
NeuroImage
.
2010
:
50
:
1148
1167
.

Cerliani
 
L
,
Thomas
 
RM
,
Jbabdi
 
S
,
Siero
 
JCW
,
Nanetti
 
L
,
Crippa
 
A
,
Gazzola
 
V
,
D'Arceuil
 
H
,
Keysers
 
C
.
Probabilistic tractography recovers a rostrocaudal trajectory of connectivity variability in the human insular cortex
.
Hum Br Mapping
.
2012
:
33
:
2005
2034
.

Charest
 
I
,
Kriegeskorte
 
N
,
Kay
 
KN
.
GLM denoise improves multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data
.
NeuroImage
.
2018
:
183
:
606
616
.

Ciaramelli
 
E
,
De Luca
 
F
,
Monk
 
AM
,
McCormick
 
C
,
Maguire
 
EA
.
What "wins" in VMPFC: scenes, situations, or schema?
 
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2019
:
100
:
208
210
.

Cisek
 
P
.
Making decisions through a distributed consensus
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
.
2012
:
22
:
927
936
.

Clithero
 
JA
,
Rangel
 
A
.
Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
.
2014
:
9
:
1289
1302
.

Damasio
 
AR
.
The brain binds entities and events by multiregional activation from convergence zones
.
Neural Comput
.
1989
:
1
:
123
132
.

Davis
 
T
,
Poldrack
 
RA
.
Quantifying the internal structure of categories using a neural typicality measure
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2014
:
24
:
1720
1737
.

Downing
 
PE
,
Jiang
 
Y
,
Shuman
 
M
,
Kanwisher
 
N
.
A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body
.
Science
.
2001
:
293
:
2470
2473
.

Edelman
 
S
,
Grill-Spector
 
K
,
Kushnir
 
T
,
Malach
 
R
.
Toward direct visualization of the internal shape representation space by fMRI
.
Psychobiology
.
1998
:
26
:
309
321
.

Fine
 
JM
,
Yoo
 
SBM
,
Ebitz
 
RB
,
Hayden
 
BY
.
Subspace alignment as a mechanism for binding
.
bioRxiv
.
2021
. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.07.07.451472.

Freund
 
MC
,
Bugg
 
JM
,
Braver
 
TS
.
A representational similarity analysis of cognitive control during color-word stroop
.
J Neurosci
2021:
41
:7388–7402.

Freund
 
MC
,
Etzel
 
JA
,
Braver
 
TS
.
Neural coding of cognitive control: the representational similarity analysis approach
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2021
:
25
:
622
638
.

Friston
 
K
.
A theory of cortical responses
.
Proc R Soc Lond B
.
2005
:
360
:
815
836
.

Gold
 
JI
,
Shadlen
 
MN
.
Banburism and the brain: decoding the relationship between sensory stimuli, decisions, and reward
.
Neuron
.
2002
:
36
:
299
308
.

Goldman-Rakic
 
P
.
Changing concepts of cortical connectivity: parallel distributed cortical networks
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
.
1988
:
11
:
137
156
.

Gong
 
G
,
He
 
Y
,
Concha
 
L
,
Lebel
 
C
,
Gross
 
DW
,
Evans
 
AC
,
Beulieu
 
C
.
Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo diffusion tensor imaging tractography
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2009
:
19
:
524
536
.

Gorgolewski
 
KJ
,
Varoquaux
 
G
,
Rivera
 
G
,
Schwarz
 
Y
,
Ghosh
 
SS
,
Maumet
 
C
,
Sochat
 
VV
,
Nichols
 
TE
,
Poldrack
 
RA
,
Poline
 
JB
, et al.  
NeuroVault.org: a web-based repository for collecting and sharing unthresholded statistical maps of the human brain
.
Front Neuroinform
.
2015
:
9
:
8
.

Grill-Spector
 
K
,
Kourtzi
 
Z
,
Kanwisher
 
N
.
The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition
.
Vis Res
.
2001
:
41
:
1409
1422
.

Gündel
 
H
,
O'Connor
 
MF
,
Littrell
 
L
,
Fort
 
C
,
Lane
 
RL
.
Functional neuroanatomy of grief: an fMRI study
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
2003
:
160
:
1946
1953
.

Hagmann
 
P
,
Cammoun
 
L
,
Gigandet
 
X
,
Meuli
 
R
,
Honey
 
CJ
,
Wedeen
 
VJ
,
Sporns
 
O
.
Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex
.
PLoS Biol
.
2008
:
6
:
e159
.

Haxby
 
JV
,
Connolly
 
AC
,
Guntupalli
 
JS
.
Decoding neural representational spaces using multivariate pattern analysis
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
.
2014
:
37
:
435
456
.

Heekeren
 
HR
,
Marrett
 
S
,
Bandettini
 
PA
,
Ungerleider
 
LG
.
A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain
.
Nature
.
2004
:
431
:
859
862
.

Heekeren
 
HR
,
Marrett
 
S
,
Ungerleider
 
LG
.
The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2008
:
9
:
467
479
.

Hinton
 
GE
,
Zemel
 
RS
.
Autoencoders, minimum description length and Helmholtz free energy
.
NIPS
.
1994
:
1994
:
3
10
.

Huntenburg
 
JM
,
Bazin
 
PL
,
Margulies
 
DS
.
Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2018
:
22
:
21
31
.

Jackson
 
PL
,
Meltzoff
 
AN
,
Decety
 
J
.
How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy
.
NeuroImage
.
2005
:
24
:
771
779
.

Jauniaux
 
J
,
Khatibi
 
A
,
Rainville
 
P
,
Jackson
 
PL
.
A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on pain empathy: investigating the role of visual information and observer's perspective
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
.
2019
:
14
:
789
813
.

Kay
 
KN
,
Rokem
 
A
,
Winawer
 
J
,
Dougherty
 
RF
,
Wand
 
BA
.
GLMdenoise: a fast, automated technique for denoising task-based fMRI data
.
Front Neurosci
.
2013
:
7
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00247.

Kersting
 
A
,
Ohrmann
 
P
,
Pedersen
 
A
,
Kroker
 
K
,
Samberg
 
D
,
Bauer
 
J
,
Kugel
 
H
,
Koelbeck
 
K
,
Steinhard
 
J
,
Heindel
 
W
, et al.  
Neural activation underlying acute grief in women after the loss of an unborn child
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
2009
:
166
:
1402
1410
.

Keysers
 
C
,
Kaas
 
JH
,
Gazzola
 
V
.
Somatosensation in social perception
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2010
:
11
:
417
428
.

Kriegeskorte
 
N
,
Mur
 
M
,
Bandettini
 
P
.
Representational similarity analysis. Connecting the branches of systems neuroscience
.
Frontiers Sys Neurosci
.
2008
:
2
:
4
. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.06.004.2008.

Kross
 
E
,
Berman
 
MG
,
Mischel
 
W
,
Smith
 
EE
,
Wager
 
TD
.
Social rejection shares somatosensory representations with physical pain
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2011
:
108
:
6270
6275
.

Labek
 
K
,
Berger
 
S
,
Buchheim
 
A
,
Bosch
 
J
,
Spohrs
 
J
,
Dommes
 
L
,
Beschoner
 
P
,
Stingl
 
JC
,
Viviani
 
R
.
The iconography of mourning and its neural correlates: a functional neuroimaging study
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
.
2017
:
12
:
1303
1313
.

Lamm
 
C
,
Decety
 
J
,
Singer
 
T
.
Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain
.
NeuroImage
.
2011
:
54
:
2492
2502
.

Langner
 
R
,
Kellermann
 
T
,
Boers
 
F
,
Sturm
 
W
,
Willmes
 
K
,
Eickhoff
 
SB
.
Modality-specific perceptual expectations selectively modulate baseline activity in auditory, somatosensory, and visual cortices
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2011
:
21
:
2850
2862
.

Lanzoni
 
L
,
Ravasio
 
D
,
Thompson
 
H
,
Vatansever
 
D
,
Margulies
 
D
,
Smallwood
 
J
,
Jefferies
 
E
.
The role of default mode network in semantic cue integration
.
NeuroImage
.
2020
:
219
:
117019
.

Leech
 
R
,
Sharp
 
DJ
.
The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease
.
Brain
.
2014
:
137
:
12
32
.

Leech
 
R
,
Braga
 
R
,
Sharp
 
DJ
.
Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex
.
J Neurosci
.
2012
:
32
:
215
222
.

Levy
 
DJ
,
Glimcher
 
PW
.
The root of all value: a neural common currency for choice
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
.
2012
:
22
:
1027
1038
.

Li
 
S
,
Ostwald
 
D
,
Giese
 
M
,
Kourtzi
 
Z
.
Flexible coding for categorical decisions in the human brain
.
J Neurosci
.
2007
:
27
:
12321
12330
.

Lim
 
SL
,
O'Doherty
 
JP
,
Rangel
 
A
.
The decision value computations in the vMPFC and striatum use a relative value code that is guided by visual attention
.
J Neurosci
.
2011
:
31
:
13124
13223
.

Luyten
 
P
,
Fonagy
 
P
.
The neurobiology of mentalizing
.
Pers Dis Theory Res Treatment
.
2015
:
6
:
366
379
.

Mack
 
ML
,
Preston
 
AR
,
Love
 
AC
.
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex compression during concept learning
.
Nat Commun
.
2020
:
11
:
46
.

Margulies
 
DS
,
Ghosh
 
SS
,
Goulas
 
A
,
Falkiewicz
 
M
,
Huntenburg
 
JM
,
Langs
 
G
,
Bezgin
 
G
,
Eickhoff
 
SB
,
Castellanos
 
FX
,
Petrides
 
M
, et al.  
Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2016
:
113
:
12574
12579
.

Messina
 
I
,
Sambin
 
M
,
Beschoner
 
P
,
Viviani
 
R
.
Changing views of emotion regulation and neurobiological models of the mechanism of action of psychotherapy
.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
.
2016
:
16
:
571
587
.

Mesulam
 
M-M
.
Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory
.
Ann Neurol
.
1990
:
28
:
597
613
.

Murphy
 
C
,
Jefferies
 
E
,
Rueschemeyer
 
SA
,
Sormaz
 
M
,
Wang
 
H
,
Margulies
 
DS
,
Smallwood
 
J
.
Distant from input: evidence of regions within the default mode network supporting perceptually-decoupled and conceputally-guided cognition
.
NeuroImage
.
2018
:
171
:
393
401
.

O'Connor
 
MF
.
Immunological and neuroimaging biomarkers of complicated grief
.
Dialogues Clin Neurosci
.
2012
:
14
:
141
148
.

Pearson
 
JM
,
Heilbronner
 
SR
,
Barack
 
DL
,
Hayden
 
BY
,
Platt
 
ML
.
Posterior cingulate cortex: adapting behavior to a changing world
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2011
:
15
:
143
151
.

Peyron
 
R
,
Fauchon
 
C
.
Functional imaging of pain
.
Rev Neurol (Paris)
.
2019
:
175
:
38
85
.

Ploran
 
EJ
,
Nelson
 
SM
,
Velanova
 
K
,
Donaldson
 
DI
,
Petersen
 
SE
,
Wheeler
 
ME
.
Evidence accumulation and the moment of recognition: dissociating perceptual recognition processes using fMRI
.
J Neurosci
.
2007
:
27
:
11912
11924
.

Raichle
 
ME
,
MacLeod
 
AM
,
Snyder
 
AZ
,
Powers
 
WJ
,
Gusnard
 
DA
,
Shulman
 
GL
.
A default mode of brain function
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2001
:
98
:
676
682
.

Rangel
 
A
,
Clithero
 
JA
. The computation of stimulus values in simple choice. In:
Glimcher
 
PW
,
Fehr
 
E
, editors.
Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain
.
New York
:
Elsevier
;
2014
. pp.
125
148

Rao
 
RPN
,
Ballard
 
DH
.
Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects
.
Nat Neurosci
.
1999
:
2
:
79
87
.

Ripoll
 
LH
,
Snyder
 
R
,
Steele
 
H
,
Siever
 
LJ
.
The neurobiology of empathy in borderline personality disorder
.
Curr Psychiatry Rep
.
2013
:
15
:
344
.

Royer
 
J
,
Paquola
 
C
,
Larivière
 
S
,
Vos de Wael
 
R
,
Tavakol
 
S
,
Lowe
 
AJ
,
Benkarim
 
O
,
Evans
 
AC
,
Bzdok
 
D
,
Smallwood
 
J
, et al.  
Myeloarchitecture gradients in the human insula: histological underpinnings and association to intrinsic functional connectivity
.
NeuroImage
.
2020
:
216
:
116859
.

Rudebeck
 
PH
,
Murray
 
EA
.
The orbitofrontal oracle: cortical mechanisms for the prediction and evaluation of specific behavioral outcomes
.
Neuron
.
2014
:
84
:
1143
1156
.

Rypma
 
B
,
Prabhakaran
 
V
,
Desmond
 
JE
,
Glover
 
GH
,
Gabrieli
 
JDE
.
Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory
.
NeuroImage
.
1999
:
9
:
216
226
.

Satpute
 
AB
,
Lindquist
 
KA
.
The default mode network's role in discrete emotion
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2019
:
23
:
851
864
.

Schilbach
 
L
,
Eickhoff
 
SB
,
Rotarska-Jagiela
 
A
,
Fink
 
GR
,
Vogeley
 
K
.
Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the "default system" of the brain
.
Conscious Cogn
.
2008
:
17
:
457
467
.

Schurz
 
M
,
Radua
 
J
,
Aichhorn
 
M
,
Richlan
 
F
,
Perner
 
J
.
Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2014
:
42
:
9
34
.

Schurz
 
M
,
Radua
 
J
,
Tholen
 
MG
,
Maliske
 
L
,
Margulies
 
DS
,
Mars
 
RB
,
Sallet
 
J
,
Kanske
 
P
.
Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: a meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind
.
Psychol Bull
.
2021
:
147
:
293
327
.

Seger
 
CA
,
Braunlich
 
K
,
Wehe
 
HS
,
Liu
 
Z
.
Generalization in category learning: the roles of representational and decisional uncertainty
.
J Neurosci
.
2015
:
35
:
8802
8812
.

Shadlen
 
MN
,
Kiani
 
R
.
Decision making as a window on cognition
.
Neuron
.
2013
:
30
:
791
806
.

Shulman
 
GL
,
Fiez
 
JA
,
Corbetta
 
M
,
Buckner
 
RL
,
Miezin
 
FM
,
Raichle
 
ME
,
Petersen
 
SE
.
Common blood flow changes across visual tasks II. Decreases in cerebral cortex
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
1997
:
9
:
648
663
.

Shulman
 
GL
,
Astafiev
 
SV
,
McAvoy
 
MP
,
d'Avossa
 
G
,
Corbetta
 
M
.
Right TPJ deactivation during visual search: functional significance and support for a filter hypothesis
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2007
:
17
:
2625
2633
.

Skodol
 
AE
,
Clark
 
LA
,
Bender
 
DS
,
Krueger
 
RF
,
Morey
 
LC
,
Verheul
 
R
,
Alarcon
 
RD
,
Bell
 
CC
,
Seiver
 
LJ
,
Oldham
 
JM
.
Proposed changes in personality and personality disorder assessment and diagnosis for DSM-5 part I: description and rationale
.
Pers Dis Theory Res Treatment
.
2011
:
1
:
4
22
.

Smallwood
 
J
,
Tipper
 
C
,
Brown
 
K
,
Baird
 
B
,
Engen
 
H
,
Michaels
 
JR
,
Grafton
 
S
,
Schooler
 
JW
.
Escaping the here and now: evidence for a role of the default mode network in perceptually decoupled thought
.
NeuroImage
.
2013
:
69
:
120
125
.

Smallwood
 
J
,
Bernhardt
 
BC
,
Leech
 
R
,
Bzdok
 
D
,
Jefferies
 
E
,
Margulies
 
DS
.
The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2021
:
22
:
503
513
.

Sosic-Vasic
 
Z
,
Eberhard
 
J
,
Bosch
 
JE
,
Dommes
 
L
,
Labek
 
K
,
Buchheim
 
A
,
Viviani
 
R
.
Mirror neuron activations in encoding of psychic pain in borderline personality disorder
.
NeuroImage Clin
.
2019
:
22
:
101737
.

Soyman
 
E
,
Bruls
 
R
,
Ioumpa
 
K
,
Müller-Pinzler
 
L
,
Gallo
 
S
,
van
 
Straaten
 
ECW
, et al.  
Intracortical human recordings reveal intensity coding for the pain of others in the insula
 
bioRxiv
.
2021
. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.06.23.449371.

Sporns
 
O
,
Honey
 
CJ
,
Kötter
 
R
.
Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks
.
PLoS One
.
2007
:
2
:
e1049
.

Spreng
 
RN
,
Mar
 
RA
,
Kim
 
ASN
.
The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2008
:
21
:489–510. https://doi.org/10-1162/jocn.2008.21029.

Timmers
 
I
,
Park
 
AL
,
Fischer
 
MD
,
Kronman
 
CA
,
Heathcote
 
LC
,
Hernandez
 
JM
,
Simons
 
LE
.
Is empathy for pain unique in its neural correlates? A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of empathy
.
Frontiers Behav Neurosci
.
2018
:
12
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00289.

van den
 
Heuvel
 
MP
,
Sporns
 
O
.
Network hubs in the human brain
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2013
:
17
:
683
696
.

Viviani
 
R
.
Emotion regulation, attention to emotion, and the ventral attentional network
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2013
:
7
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00746.

Viviani
 
R
.
Neural correlates of emotion regulation in the ventral prefrontal cortex and the encoding of subjective value and economic utility
.
Front Psych
.
2014
:
5
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00123.

Viviani
 
R
.
Overcoming bias in representational similarity analysis
.
2021
:
arXiv, 2102.08931
.

Viviani
 
R
,
Dommes
 
L
,
Bosch
 
J
,
Stingl
 
JC
,
Beschoner
 
P
.
The neural correlates of decisions about sadness in facial expressions
.
J Neurosci Psychol Econ
.
2018
:
11
:
93
105
.

Viviani
 
R
,
Dommes
 
L
,
Bosch
 
J
,
Beschoner
 
P
,
Stingl
 
JC
,
Schnell
 
T
.
Choosing between personal values: the Pavlovian substrates of intrinsic preferences
 
bioRxiv
.
2019
. https://doi.org/10.1101/856294.

Viviani
 
R
,
Dommes
 
L
,
Bosch
 
JE
,
Labek
 
K
.
Segregation, connectivity, and gradients of deactivation in neural correlates of evidence in social decision making
.
NeuroImage
.
2020
:
223
:
117339
.

Wheeler
 
ME
,
Woo
 
SG
,
Ansel
 
T
,
Tremel
 
JJ
,
Collier
 
AL
,
Velanova
 
K
,
Ploran
 
EJ
,
Yang
 
T
.
The strength of gradually accruing probabilistic evidence modulates brain activity during a categorical decision
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2015
:
27
:
705
719
.

Wickham
 
H
.
ggplot2. Elegant graphics for data analysis
.
Heidelberg
:
Springer
;
2009

Wilson
 
RC
,
Takahashi
 
YK
,
Schoenbaum
 
G
,
Niv
 
Y
.
Orbitofrontal cortex as a cognitive map of task space
.
Neuron
.
2014
:
81
:
267
279
.

Woolgar
 
A
,
Thompson
 
R
,
Bor
 
D
,
Duncan
 
J
.
Multi-voxel coding of stimuli, rules, and responses in human frontoparietal cortex
.
NeuroImage
.
2011
:
56
:
744
752
.

Yarkoni
 
T
,
Poldrack
 
RA
,
Nichols
 
TN
,
Van Essen
 
DC
,
Wager
 
TD
.
Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data
.
Nat Methods
.
2011
:
8
:
665
670
.

Yeo
 
BTT
,
Krienen
 
FM
,
Sepulcre
 
J
,
Sabuncu
 
MR
,
Lashkari
 
D
,
Hollinshead
 
M
,
Roffman
 
JL
,
Smoller
 
JW
,
Zöllei
 
L
,
Pilomeni
 
JR
, et al.  
The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity
.
J Neurophysiol
.
2011
:
106
:
1125
1165
.

Yoo
 
SBM
,
Hayden
 
BY
.
Economic choice as an untangling of options into actions
.
Neuron
.
2018
:
99
:
434
447
.

Zhou
 
JZ
,
Gardner
 
MPH
,
Schoenbaum
 
G
.
Is the core function of orbitofrontal cortex to signal values or make predictions?
 
Curr Opin Behav Sci
.
2021
:
41
:
1
9
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)

Supplementary data