Abstract

We present anatomy-based symptom-lesion mapping to assess the association between lesions of tracts in the extreme capsule and aphasia. The study cohort consisted of 123 patients with acute left-hemispheric stroke without a lesion of language-related cortical areas of the Stanford atlas of functional regions of interest. On templates generated through global fibre tractography, lesions of the extreme capsule and of the arcuate fascicle were quantified and correlated with the occurrence of aphasia (n=18) as defined by the Token Test. More than 15% damage of the slice plane through the extreme capsule was a strong independent predictor of aphasia in stroke patients, odds ratio 16.37, 95% confidence interval: 3.11–86.16, P<0.01. In contrast, stroke lesions of >15% in the arcuate fascicle were not associated with aphasia. Our results support the relevance of a ventral pathway in the language network running through the extreme capsule.

Introduction

Relating brain anatomy and neurological symptoms has gained the interest of researchers since the 19th century. Correlating a pathological phenotype to anatomically defined lesions such as in stroke is the oldest approach in this field, first done in post-mortem brains,1 and meanwhile made possible in vivo by the advent of imaging techniques. Today, its voxel-based variant using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to map symptoms onto lesions is a driving force in human brain mapping and thought to identify causal relationships between brain and behaviour.2 However, the established univariate voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) method3 has been challenged because of its topographical bias, resulting in a misplacement towards the centre of major arteries.4,5

The reverse approach, choosing a defined anatomical structure and relating its affection to the occurrence of symptoms, may be called ‘anatomy-based symptom-lesion mapping’ (ASLM) in analogy to VLSM. ASLM has rarely been used and calls for a different procedure than VLSM. ASLM requires several a-priori conditions to achieve a high specificity: (i) a brain function linked to an established ‘anatomical network’ and corresponding neurological syndrome, (ii) within this network a defined anatomical structure of interest and techniques to unambiguously identify this anatomical structure and (iii) the selection of patients without lesions in other parts of the anatomical network to ensure that the clinical syndrome is due to the selected region. Thus, while VLSM relates scores or binary behavioural data to lesions on a voxel-by-voxel basis, ASLM selects an anatomically defined structure and relates its lesioning to the occurrence of a symptom or a syndrome while carefully avoiding affection of other parts of the network. There are other studies relating lesioning of tracts to the occurrence of symptoms.6 We are not aware of a comparable publication following these preconditions vigorously and present a proof-of-concept study to investigate how such a technique can be useful. The three preconditions for our study are explained in the following: (i) Language is a well-studied brain function with a corresponding classical syndrome, aphasia, which is assessed by validated clinical tests. Language involves different domains and overlaps with other cognitive systems that process motor function, sensation, emotion, memory and others7,8 and a widespread, bilateral network with many cortical and subcortical areas is involved in language processing.9 An atlas that comprises these cortical language-relevant regions is the Stanford atlas.10 (ii) The most prominent functio-anatomical model for language processing is the dual-stream language network.11,12 In this model, several temporal areas and inferior parietal cortex are connected with the ventrolateral frontal region and lateral premotor cortex along two major associations tracts.13,14 The ‘dorsal pathway’ in this model is constituted by the arcuate fascicle (AF; syn. fasciculus arcuatus15), traditionally been considered as the most important tract in language system processing.16,17 In the ventral pathway, the situation is less clear. The most prominent ventral tracts, which continue to the frontal cortex are the inferior frontal-occipital fascicle and the uncinate fascicle. The inferior frontal-occipital fascicle by definition does not connect temporal regions,18 the uncinate fascicle may be more related to abstract representations or is seen as part of the limbic system.19 A direct ventral temporo-frontal connection between what may be seen as Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas in humans has unequivocally been demonstrated in nonhuman primates using autoradiographic tracing and termed ‘extreme capsule fascicle’ (ECF).14,20 Similar fibres in the extreme capsule have been identified in vivo in humans using Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI)-based fibre tracking21,22 and stimulated a ‘rethinking of the language circuity’.23 In contrast to the AF, the function of the ventral connection to the frontal lobe for language processing and its importance in the development of aphasia is less obvious.17,24,25 Thus, we decided to study the ECF within the anatomical region ‘extreme capsule’. (3) ASLM requires the exclusion of patients with lesions to the functional network other than the anatomical structure under study, here the ECF. We decided to exclude patients with lesions affecting the language-relevant cortical regions of interests (ROIs) as defined by the Stanford atlas language network10 and took the AF as control area.

We explored the anatomical region ‘extreme capsule’ and its relation to the syndrome ‘aphasia’ as tested by the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT),26 the most widely used aphasia test in German, in patients with acute (<10days) first ever stroke due to a singular ischaemic infarct of the left hemisphere. We asked the simple question whether a lesion of the extreme capsule leads to aphasia, taking the AF as control structure.

Materials and methods

The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the University Medical Centre of Freiburg (281/13). Patients were recruited from the Stroke Unit or the intensive care unit, Department of Neurology at the University Medical Centre of Freiburg, Germany, in the context of a large prospective study (‘Freiburg large scale project’) on stroke-related cognitive deficits.27–29 All patients with acute stroke consecutively admitted to the Stroke Unit or intensive care unit were screened. As ischaemic infarcts and intracranial bleedings have different pathophysiology, different anatomical locations, and different mechanisms to produce symptoms, we decided to only study acute ischaemic infarcts within 10days and exclude hemodynamic infarcts. The inclusion criteria were (i) embolic first-time stroke of the left middle cerebral artery-territory, (ii) age < 90years and (iii) German native speaker. Exclusion criteria included (i) recurrent stroke during the study period, (ii) previous stroke, previous intracerebral bleeding, previous traumatic brain injury, (iii) inability to tolerate MRI examination or clinical testing due to reduced general health status, (iv) hearing or visual deficits, (v) cognitive impairment other than aphasia and (vi) any contraindication to MRI. In the period between February 2011 and December 2018, we screened 7405 patients; 287 with acute ischaemic left-hemispheric stroke met the inclusion and exclusion criteria. In five patients, aphasia testing was not possible within 10days after stroke. To further restrict the 282 patients to those with infarct areas outside the cortical language-zones, overlaps between stroke lesion and the four left-hemispheric functional ROIs of the Stanford atlas language network (Fig. 1) were calculated and all patients with lesions (>0%) in at least one of the four ROIs were excluded (n=150).

Location of language zones. Four left-hemispheric functional regions of interest of the Stanford atlas language network (red: inferior frontal gyrus; lilac: anterior temporal lobe; green: middle temporal gyrus; blue: superior temporal gyrus), modified from Shirer et al.10,30
Figure 1

Location of language zones. Four left-hemispheric functional regions of interest of the Stanford atlas language network (red: inferior frontal gyrus; lilac: anterior temporal lobe; green: middle temporal gyrus; blue: superior temporal gyrus), modified from Shirer et al.10,30

The study outcome was aphasia as a binary variable. The Token Test was found to discriminate particularly well between patients with aphasia and normal controls,31 normal hospitalized adults,32 non-aphasic right-hemisphere-damaged adults33,34 and non-aphasic diffuse- and focal-brain-damaged adults.35 To assess aphasic deficits in our cohort, all patients completed the Token Test of the AAT.26 Aphasia was diagnosed for patients with deficits of at least seven age-corrected error scores according to the allocation of the AAT. In nine patients, categorization was ambiguous. These patients were allocated to the non-aphasia group as they scored <7 error points in the Token Test; however, they scored <78 of 90 points in the AAT subtests for reading and writing. We decided to exclude these nine patients from the study to ensure unharmed language function in the non-aphasia group.

Full written consent was obtained from all subjects. In cases of severe aphasia or paralysis of the right hand, detailed information was given to the patient’s relatives or the legal guardian. The study was approved by the local ethics committee.

MRI acquisition: on a 3-T TIM TRIO scanner with a 32-channel head coil (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) diffusion-sensitive single-shot spin-echo echo-planar imaging sequence was acquired with the following parameters: 61 diffusion encoding gradient directions (b-factor, 1000s/mm2); repetition time, 11 800ms; echo time, 96ms; inversion time, 2300ms; 69 axial slices; matrix size, 104 × 104; field of view, 208mm; voxel size, 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm. Nine additional scans without diffusion weighting (b-factor, 0s/mm2) were equally distributed across the acquisition series, resulting in a total of 70 volumes. For anatomical correlation T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo sequence was acquired with the following parameters: repetition time, 2200ms; echo time, 4.11ms; inversion time, 1100ms; flip angle, 12°; 160 sagittal slices; matrix size, 256 × 256; field of view, 256mm; voxel size, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3.

DTI processing: DTI data were processed using a MATLAB-based in-house toolbox for fibre tracking.36 Parameters for global tracking were a cylinder width of 1mm and a cylinder length of 3mm. The weight of a cylinder segment was set to one-fourth of the brain-averaged anisotropic signal component, resulting in a ‘dense’ reconstruction with an average of 30 cylinders per voxel. Note that the weight parameter is comparable to a fractional anisotropy threshold: for higher weights, the number of streamlines is reduced and a significant amount of streamlines is only revealed for regions with a highly anisotropic diffusion distributions. Conversely, a lower weight leads to high number of streamlines, even in regions with a low fractional anisotropy. Given the low weight selected here, the number of iterations was set to 3 × 108. Finally, the temperature schedule for the cooling phase of the polymerization process was chosen exponentially with a starting temperature of 0.1 to a stop temperature of 0.001.37

Bundle selection: we used the imaging and tracking methodology and selection of AF streamlines as described previously.38 Detailed description of the ROIs used for the ECF streamline selection are shown in Fig. 2. First ROI1 and ROI3 were used to include (‘and’ function) from the temporo-lateral region running through the anterior part of the extreme capsule/external capsule, secondary ROI2 was used to exclude streamlines (‘not’ function) from the temporo-polar region. To determine the amount of disruption of the fibre bundles, we selected the mid portion of the bundles, where most of the fibres can be assumed to run through a very circumscribed region. These regions were cut into slices perpendicular to the main fibre orientation. Figure 3A shows the ROIs and slices in the ECF, Fig. 3B in the AF. For each slice of these ROIs, we calculated the percentage of overlap between the fibre bundle area in that slice and the lesion of each patient. Assuming that association fibres are discontinued as soon as the lesion overlaps with at least one slice of an ROI, we computed the maximum percentage of overlap across the slices of ECF and AF ROIs for each patient.

Segmentation of extreme capsule streamlines. (A, C and F) Coronal reformatted MPRAGE (magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition with gradient echo) images. (E) Sagittal reformatted MPRAGE image. (B and D) Corresponding colour-coded fractional anisotropy maps: the arrows in A show the location of the temporal stem in both hemispheres. The yellow circle in B (ROI1) includes all green voxels in anterior extreme capsule/temporal stem at the level of the anterior commissure including extreme capsule streamlines. The yellow area in C (ROI2) covers the entire cross-section of the anterior temporal lobe. ROI3 (E and F) was placed in a sagittal plane barely cutting the ground of the temporal sulcus to segment cortical projection streamlines of the temporal lobe.
Figure 2

Segmentation of extreme capsule streamlines. (A, C and F) Coronal reformatted MPRAGE (magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition with gradient echo) images. (E) Sagittal reformatted MPRAGE image. (B and D) Corresponding colour-coded fractional anisotropy maps: the arrows in A show the location of the temporal stem in both hemispheres. The yellow circle in B (ROI1) includes all green voxels in anterior extreme capsule/temporal stem at the level of the anterior commissure including extreme capsule streamlines. The yellow area in C (ROI2) covers the entire cross-section of the anterior temporal lobe. ROI3 (E and F) was placed in a sagittal plane barely cutting the ground of the temporal sulcus to segment cortical projection streamlines of the temporal lobe.

Extreme capsule fibre template and arcuate fascicle template. Regions of interest and slices (perpendicular to the main fibre orientation) in the extreme capsule fibre system (A) and arcuate fascicle fibre system (B). Coronal, sagittal and horizontal planes of templates. Fibre system (blue), outline of template (red), layers in template (green).
Figure 3

Extreme capsule fibre template and arcuate fascicle template. Regions of interest and slices (perpendicular to the main fibre orientation) in the extreme capsule fibre system (A) and arcuate fascicle fibre system (B). Coronal, sagittal and horizontal planes of templates. Fibre system (blue), outline of template (red), layers in template (green).

Stroke volume, age and gender were covariates. Total normalized stroke volume was measured voxel-based on diffusion-weighted MRI data and indicated in millilitres. Patients were dichotomized in those with or without aphasia mainly by means of the internationally established Token Test.39 Patients were selected that infarcts did not affect the language-relevant cortical ROIs as defined by the Stanford atlas language network.10,30 The association between ECF lesions and aphasia was calculated with Fisher’s exact test. In the absence of an established parameter for lesions of tracts, we dichotomized ECF lesions arbitrarily into two equally sized groups using the statistical parameter median split. We further split the group with ECF lesions ≥15% into subgroups using the upper quartile threshold of 30%. The reference group consisted of stroke patients with no lesion of the ECF (0%). Patients with AF lesions were dichotomized accordingly by using the median split. We used multivariate logistic regression to estimate the odds ratio (OR) of aphasia, associated with ECF lesions compared with no ECF lesions, adjusting for AF lesions, stroke volume as a continuous variable, age (dichotomous with cut-off at 70years) and gender.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Results

The study cohort consisted of 123 patients with acute left-hemispheric ischaemic infarcts without lesions in the functional ROIs of the language network. Testing for aphasia was performed within 10days after their first-time stroke (mean 3.93 ± 2.28days, range 0–10days). Aphasia was diagnosed in 18 patients with >7 error scores in the Token Test, 105 were qualified as having no aphasia in the Token Test as well as in the subtest reading and writing of the AAT. The mean age was 64.5 ± 14.5years and 63.4% were men. ECF lesions were present in 24.4% (30 of 123 patients), and AF lesions in 37.4% (46 of 123 patients), including 13.8% (17 of 123 patients, 5 of whom with aphasia and 12 without aphasia) with simultaneous ECF lesions. The median split of both ECF and AF lesions was 15%. The mean stroke volume was 6.9 ± 8.0 ml, 6.0 ± 7.4 ml in those without and 11.7 ± 10.0 ml in those with aphasia. Aphasia occurred in 14.6% (18 of 123 patients) (Table 1). There was no difference in the intervals between stroke onset performance of Token Test or MRI scanning between both groups. Individual data of all aphasia patients are shown in Table 2.

Table 1

Demographics of cohort with ischaemic left-hemispheric stroke

Without aphasiaWith aphasiaAll patientsP-valuec
TotalN=105N=18N=123
Gender
 Male66/105 (62.9)12/18 (66.7)78/123 (63.4)1.00
 Female39/105 (37.1)6/18 (33.3)45/123 (36.6)
Age (years)
 Mean (SD)63.4 (14.5)70.7 (13.3)64.5 (14.5)<0.05
 Median (p25, p75)65/105 (57, 75)73 (70, 81)67 (57, 76)
 <7062/105 (59.0)5/18 (27.8)67/123 (54.5)
 ≥7043/105 (41.0)13/18 (72.2)56/123 (45.5)
Stroke volume (ml)
 Mean (SD)6.0 (7.4)11.7 (10.0)6.9 (8.0)<0.05
ECF lesiona,d
 None85/105 (81.0)8/18 (44.4)93/123 (75.6)<0.05
 <15%15/105 (14.3)1/18 (5.6)16/123 (13.0)
 ≥15%5/105 (4.8)9/18 (50.0)14/123 (11.4)
 Mean (SD)b19.3 (24.9)37.3 (21.7)25.3 (25.0)<0.05
AF lesiona,d
 None65/105 (61.9)12/18 (66.7)77/123 (62.6)0.80
 <15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 ≥15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 Mean (SD)b20.6 (21.5)20.4 (17.5)20.6 (20.9)0.80
Without aphasiaWith aphasiaAll patientsP-valuec
TotalN=105N=18N=123
Gender
 Male66/105 (62.9)12/18 (66.7)78/123 (63.4)1.00
 Female39/105 (37.1)6/18 (33.3)45/123 (36.6)
Age (years)
 Mean (SD)63.4 (14.5)70.7 (13.3)64.5 (14.5)<0.05
 Median (p25, p75)65/105 (57, 75)73 (70, 81)67 (57, 76)
 <7062/105 (59.0)5/18 (27.8)67/123 (54.5)
 ≥7043/105 (41.0)13/18 (72.2)56/123 (45.5)
Stroke volume (ml)
 Mean (SD)6.0 (7.4)11.7 (10.0)6.9 (8.0)<0.05
ECF lesiona,d
 None85/105 (81.0)8/18 (44.4)93/123 (75.6)<0.05
 <15%15/105 (14.3)1/18 (5.6)16/123 (13.0)
 ≥15%5/105 (4.8)9/18 (50.0)14/123 (11.4)
 Mean (SD)b19.3 (24.9)37.3 (21.7)25.3 (25.0)<0.05
AF lesiona,d
 None65/105 (61.9)12/18 (66.7)77/123 (62.6)0.80
 <15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 ≥15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 Mean (SD)b20.6 (21.5)20.4 (17.5)20.6 (20.9)0.80

p = percentile; SD = standard deviation.

a

Lesions dichotomized according to their median split.

b

Of those with a lesion.

c

Comparing patients with and without aphasia using Fisher’s exact test for categorical variables and Student’s t-test for continuous variables.

d

Including 17 patients with simultaneous ECF and AF lesions, 5 of whom with aphasia and 12 without aphasia.

Table 1

Demographics of cohort with ischaemic left-hemispheric stroke

Without aphasiaWith aphasiaAll patientsP-valuec
TotalN=105N=18N=123
Gender
 Male66/105 (62.9)12/18 (66.7)78/123 (63.4)1.00
 Female39/105 (37.1)6/18 (33.3)45/123 (36.6)
Age (years)
 Mean (SD)63.4 (14.5)70.7 (13.3)64.5 (14.5)<0.05
 Median (p25, p75)65/105 (57, 75)73 (70, 81)67 (57, 76)
 <7062/105 (59.0)5/18 (27.8)67/123 (54.5)
 ≥7043/105 (41.0)13/18 (72.2)56/123 (45.5)
Stroke volume (ml)
 Mean (SD)6.0 (7.4)11.7 (10.0)6.9 (8.0)<0.05
ECF lesiona,d
 None85/105 (81.0)8/18 (44.4)93/123 (75.6)<0.05
 <15%15/105 (14.3)1/18 (5.6)16/123 (13.0)
 ≥15%5/105 (4.8)9/18 (50.0)14/123 (11.4)
 Mean (SD)b19.3 (24.9)37.3 (21.7)25.3 (25.0)<0.05
AF lesiona,d
 None65/105 (61.9)12/18 (66.7)77/123 (62.6)0.80
 <15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 ≥15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 Mean (SD)b20.6 (21.5)20.4 (17.5)20.6 (20.9)0.80
Without aphasiaWith aphasiaAll patientsP-valuec
TotalN=105N=18N=123
Gender
 Male66/105 (62.9)12/18 (66.7)78/123 (63.4)1.00
 Female39/105 (37.1)6/18 (33.3)45/123 (36.6)
Age (years)
 Mean (SD)63.4 (14.5)70.7 (13.3)64.5 (14.5)<0.05
 Median (p25, p75)65/105 (57, 75)73 (70, 81)67 (57, 76)
 <7062/105 (59.0)5/18 (27.8)67/123 (54.5)
 ≥7043/105 (41.0)13/18 (72.2)56/123 (45.5)
Stroke volume (ml)
 Mean (SD)6.0 (7.4)11.7 (10.0)6.9 (8.0)<0.05
ECF lesiona,d
 None85/105 (81.0)8/18 (44.4)93/123 (75.6)<0.05
 <15%15/105 (14.3)1/18 (5.6)16/123 (13.0)
 ≥15%5/105 (4.8)9/18 (50.0)14/123 (11.4)
 Mean (SD)b19.3 (24.9)37.3 (21.7)25.3 (25.0)<0.05
AF lesiona,d
 None65/105 (61.9)12/18 (66.7)77/123 (62.6)0.80
 <15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 ≥15%20/105 (19.0)3/18 (16.7)23/123 (18.7)
 Mean (SD)b20.6 (21.5)20.4 (17.5)20.6 (20.9)0.80

p = percentile; SD = standard deviation.

a

Lesions dichotomized according to their median split.

b

Of those with a lesion.

c

Comparing patients with and without aphasia using Fisher’s exact test for categorical variables and Student’s t-test for continuous variables.

d

Including 17 patients with simultaneous ECF and AF lesions, 5 of whom with aphasia and 12 without aphasia.

Table 2

Individual data of all aphasia patients

PatientToken Test error pointsa (percentile rank)Token Test subtest score of total (%)
AAT subtests percentile rank
ECF lesionbAF lesionbLesion site
12345Written languageRepetitionNamingCompre hension
19 (83)10090804040657059653141Basal ganglia
211 (79)1009080303097929455Thalamocapsular area
310 (81)1007070505095849194Frontoopercular area
49 (83)1009080206072834425192Striatocapsular area
550 (2)00000526270Lateral sulcus
636 (36)70200001660163Frontoinsular area
77 (89)908080606060776053Precentral area
810 (81)801008030507089997613Corona radiata
99 (83)10090703060999999705024Basal ganglia
1010 (81)1001009070408483778865Basal ganglia
1111 (79)10090805070997282792040Striatocapsular area
1218 (68)10080500307397627021Lenticulostriatal area
1311 (79)100100607004168643311Basal ganglia
1421 (63)7070500407972663927Lenticulostriatal area
1538 (31)3020100011221411592Basal ganglia
1613 (76)9010050505066949859Inferior frontal gyrus
17c10 (81)10080904030709494Semioval centre
187 (89)100905070601009298100Precentral area
Mean70857659343767756854
SD23283029262330263131
PatientToken Test error pointsa (percentile rank)Token Test subtest score of total (%)
AAT subtests percentile rank
ECF lesionbAF lesionbLesion site
12345Written languageRepetitionNamingCompre hension
19 (83)10090804040657059653141Basal ganglia
211 (79)1009080303097929455Thalamocapsular area
310 (81)1007070505095849194Frontoopercular area
49 (83)1009080206072834425192Striatocapsular area
550 (2)00000526270Lateral sulcus
636 (36)70200001660163Frontoinsular area
77 (89)908080606060776053Precentral area
810 (81)801008030507089997613Corona radiata
99 (83)10090703060999999705024Basal ganglia
1010 (81)1001009070408483778865Basal ganglia
1111 (79)10090805070997282792040Striatocapsular area
1218 (68)10080500307397627021Lenticulostriatal area
1311 (79)100100607004168643311Basal ganglia
1421 (63)7070500407972663927Lenticulostriatal area
1538 (31)3020100011221411592Basal ganglia
1613 (76)9010050505066949859Inferior frontal gyrus
17c10 (81)10080904030709494Semioval centre
187 (89)100905070601009298100Precentral area
Mean70857659343767756854
SD23283029262330263131

SD = standard deviation.

a

Of a total of 50 points.

b

Maximum percentage of overlap across the slices of the ECF and AF region of interest.

c

Patient was unable to perform the comprehension subtest due to a recurrent stroke.

Table 2

Individual data of all aphasia patients

PatientToken Test error pointsa (percentile rank)Token Test subtest score of total (%)
AAT subtests percentile rank
ECF lesionbAF lesionbLesion site
12345Written languageRepetitionNamingCompre hension
19 (83)10090804040657059653141Basal ganglia
211 (79)1009080303097929455Thalamocapsular area
310 (81)1007070505095849194Frontoopercular area
49 (83)1009080206072834425192Striatocapsular area
550 (2)00000526270Lateral sulcus
636 (36)70200001660163Frontoinsular area
77 (89)908080606060776053Precentral area
810 (81)801008030507089997613Corona radiata
99 (83)10090703060999999705024Basal ganglia
1010 (81)1001009070408483778865Basal ganglia
1111 (79)10090805070997282792040Striatocapsular area
1218 (68)10080500307397627021Lenticulostriatal area
1311 (79)100100607004168643311Basal ganglia
1421 (63)7070500407972663927Lenticulostriatal area
1538 (31)3020100011221411592Basal ganglia
1613 (76)9010050505066949859Inferior frontal gyrus
17c10 (81)10080904030709494Semioval centre
187 (89)100905070601009298100Precentral area
Mean70857659343767756854
SD23283029262330263131
PatientToken Test error pointsa (percentile rank)Token Test subtest score of total (%)
AAT subtests percentile rank
ECF lesionbAF lesionbLesion site
12345Written languageRepetitionNamingCompre hension
19 (83)10090804040657059653141Basal ganglia
211 (79)1009080303097929455Thalamocapsular area
310 (81)1007070505095849194Frontoopercular area
49 (83)1009080206072834425192Striatocapsular area
550 (2)00000526270Lateral sulcus
636 (36)70200001660163Frontoinsular area
77 (89)908080606060776053Precentral area
810 (81)801008030507089997613Corona radiata
99 (83)10090703060999999705024Basal ganglia
1010 (81)1001009070408483778865Basal ganglia
1111 (79)10090805070997282792040Striatocapsular area
1218 (68)10080500307397627021Lenticulostriatal area
1311 (79)100100607004168643311Basal ganglia
1421 (63)7070500407972663927Lenticulostriatal area
1538 (31)3020100011221411592Basal ganglia
1613 (76)9010050505066949859Inferior frontal gyrus
17c10 (81)10080904030709494Semioval centre
187 (89)100905070601009298100Precentral area
Mean70857659343767756854
SD23283029262330263131

SD = standard deviation.

a

Of a total of 50 points.

b

Maximum percentage of overlap across the slices of the ECF and AF region of interest.

c

Patient was unable to perform the comprehension subtest due to a recurrent stroke.

Aphasia was significantly more frequent in patients with lesions of the ECF (P<0.05), but not with lesions of the AF (P=0.80). The multivariate logistic regression analysis showed that stroke patients with >15% ECF lesion were at greater odds to be affected with aphasia than those without ECF lesions (OR=16.37, 95% confidence interval: 3.11–86.16, P<0.01). The subgroups with 15–30% ECF lesion (OR=18.31, 95% confidence interval: 1.86–180.77, P=0.013) as well as patients with >30% ECF lesion (OR=15.32, 95% confidence interval: 2.31–101.57, P=0.005) showed the same risk of aphasia. AF lesions of more than 15% were not associated with aphasia (OR=0.30, 95% confidence interval: 0.04–2.35, P=0.25) (Table 3). There was no interaction between ECF and AF lesions (P=0.71). Lesion overlaps of all 123 patients are displayed in Fig. 4.

Lesion overlap of all patients and subgroups with extreme capsule fibre and arcuate fascicle lesions. Equidistant slices from z = −33 to z = 64.
Figure 4

Lesion overlap of all patients and subgroups with extreme capsule fibre and arcuate fascicle lesions. Equidistant slices from z = −33 to z=64.

Table 3

Predictors of aphasia in 123 patients with left-hemispheric stroke

With aphasia, n (%)Without aphasia, n (%)Crude OR (0.95-CI)Adjusted ORa (0.95-CI)P-Valueb
Total18 (100.0)105 (100.0)
ECF lesionc
 None8 (44.44)85 (80.95)11
 <15%1 (5.56)15 (14.29)0.71 (0.08–6.08)0.72 (0.07–7.08)0.78
 ≥15%9 (50.00)5 (4.76)19.12 (5.15–71.00)16.37 (3.11–86.16)<0.01
 ≥15% < 30%4 (22.22)2 (1.90)21.25 (3.36–134.56)18.31 (1.86–180.77)0.01
 >30%5 (27.78)3 (2.86)17.71 (3.56–88.10)15.32 (2.31–101.57)<0.01
AF lesionc
 one12 (66.67)65 (61.90)11
 15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.33 (0.05–2.25)0.26
 ≥15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.30 (0.04–2.35)0.25
Other factors
 Female gender6 (33.33)39 (37.14)0.85 (0.29–2.43)0.70 (0.19–2.56)0.59
 Age ≥70years13 (72.22)43 (40.95)3.75 (1.25–11.29)3.97 (1.02–15.46)0.05
 Stroke volume (ml)d11.686.041.07 (1.01–1.12)1.06 (0.98–1.14)0.13
With aphasia, n (%)Without aphasia, n (%)Crude OR (0.95-CI)Adjusted ORa (0.95-CI)P-Valueb
Total18 (100.0)105 (100.0)
ECF lesionc
 None8 (44.44)85 (80.95)11
 <15%1 (5.56)15 (14.29)0.71 (0.08–6.08)0.72 (0.07–7.08)0.78
 ≥15%9 (50.00)5 (4.76)19.12 (5.15–71.00)16.37 (3.11–86.16)<0.01
 ≥15% < 30%4 (22.22)2 (1.90)21.25 (3.36–134.56)18.31 (1.86–180.77)0.01
 >30%5 (27.78)3 (2.86)17.71 (3.56–88.10)15.32 (2.31–101.57)<0.01
AF lesionc
 one12 (66.67)65 (61.90)11
 15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.33 (0.05–2.25)0.26
 ≥15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.30 (0.04–2.35)0.25
Other factors
 Female gender6 (33.33)39 (37.14)0.85 (0.29–2.43)0.70 (0.19–2.56)0.59
 Age ≥70years13 (72.22)43 (40.95)3.75 (1.25–11.29)3.97 (1.02–15.46)0.05
 Stroke volume (ml)d11.686.041.07 (1.01–1.12)1.06 (0.98–1.14)0.13

CI: confidence interval; OR: odds ratio.

a

Adjusted for ECF lesion, AF lesion, gender, age and stroke volume.

b

Two-sided Wald-test derived from logistic regression.

c

Lesions dichotomized according to their median split.

d

Mean volume in ml presented instead of n (%) and used as continuous variable in the logistic regression models.

Table 3

Predictors of aphasia in 123 patients with left-hemispheric stroke

With aphasia, n (%)Without aphasia, n (%)Crude OR (0.95-CI)Adjusted ORa (0.95-CI)P-Valueb
Total18 (100.0)105 (100.0)
ECF lesionc
 None8 (44.44)85 (80.95)11
 <15%1 (5.56)15 (14.29)0.71 (0.08–6.08)0.72 (0.07–7.08)0.78
 ≥15%9 (50.00)5 (4.76)19.12 (5.15–71.00)16.37 (3.11–86.16)<0.01
 ≥15% < 30%4 (22.22)2 (1.90)21.25 (3.36–134.56)18.31 (1.86–180.77)0.01
 >30%5 (27.78)3 (2.86)17.71 (3.56–88.10)15.32 (2.31–101.57)<0.01
AF lesionc
 one12 (66.67)65 (61.90)11
 15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.33 (0.05–2.25)0.26
 ≥15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.30 (0.04–2.35)0.25
Other factors
 Female gender6 (33.33)39 (37.14)0.85 (0.29–2.43)0.70 (0.19–2.56)0.59
 Age ≥70years13 (72.22)43 (40.95)3.75 (1.25–11.29)3.97 (1.02–15.46)0.05
 Stroke volume (ml)d11.686.041.07 (1.01–1.12)1.06 (0.98–1.14)0.13
With aphasia, n (%)Without aphasia, n (%)Crude OR (0.95-CI)Adjusted ORa (0.95-CI)P-Valueb
Total18 (100.0)105 (100.0)
ECF lesionc
 None8 (44.44)85 (80.95)11
 <15%1 (5.56)15 (14.29)0.71 (0.08–6.08)0.72 (0.07–7.08)0.78
 ≥15%9 (50.00)5 (4.76)19.12 (5.15–71.00)16.37 (3.11–86.16)<0.01
 ≥15% < 30%4 (22.22)2 (1.90)21.25 (3.36–134.56)18.31 (1.86–180.77)0.01
 >30%5 (27.78)3 (2.86)17.71 (3.56–88.10)15.32 (2.31–101.57)<0.01
AF lesionc
 one12 (66.67)65 (61.90)11
 15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.33 (0.05–2.25)0.26
 ≥15%3 (16.67)20 (19.05)0.81 (0.21–3.17)0.30 (0.04–2.35)0.25
Other factors
 Female gender6 (33.33)39 (37.14)0.85 (0.29–2.43)0.70 (0.19–2.56)0.59
 Age ≥70years13 (72.22)43 (40.95)3.75 (1.25–11.29)3.97 (1.02–15.46)0.05
 Stroke volume (ml)d11.686.041.07 (1.01–1.12)1.06 (0.98–1.14)0.13

CI: confidence interval; OR: odds ratio.

a

Adjusted for ECF lesion, AF lesion, gender, age and stroke volume.

b

Two-sided Wald-test derived from logistic regression.

c

Lesions dichotomized according to their median split.

d

Mean volume in ml presented instead of n (%) and used as continuous variable in the logistic regression models.

Discussion

We present a proof-of-concept study relating the lesion of an anatomically defined structure to the occurrence of a neurological syndrome. Our findings show a significant association between lesions in the extreme capsule and aphasia in patients with acute stroke without cortical infarction of the language network regions. Patients with lesions of >15% of the slice plane of the ECF tract have an over 16 times higher OR of presenting with aphasia after stroke than patients without a lesion in this area, irrespective of any lesion of the AF. Lesions in the AF were not associated with aphasia as defined by the Token Test. Stroke volume, gender and age were not independently associated with a higher risk of aphasia.

This study introduces a new technique we name ASLM. ASLM is based on defining a brain ROI, in this study an association tract, and correlating it with behavioural dysfunction, here the presence of aphasia, in patients with intact cortical language processing regions. We are not aware of another study adopting this approach. There are VLSM studies relating aphasic symptoms to lesions of tracts.24,25,28,40 Others assess white matter integrity of established language association tracts outside the stroke lesion itself without controlling for involvement of the cortical language zones.

We found a strong and statistically significant association between ECF lesion and aphasia. There is an ongoing discussion about the correct naming of the language-related ventral tracts running through the extreme capsule, consisting of uncinate fascicle, inferior frontal-occipital fascicle and ECF.14,41 We took great care to separate the ECF but are well aware that the three tracts anatomically overlap within the extreme capsule,42 and thus may be not really differentiable conceptually. Thus, we studied the bulk of fibres running through the extreme capsule and each of the tracts may be associated with aphasia, not that lesions of either ECF, inferior frontal-occipital fascicle or uncinate fascicle are mainly responsible. Investigating the ECF lesions by using a DTI fibre tracking template and calculating the damage of plane slice in percent has enabled us to include qualitative and quantitative information on the damage of the ventral pathway. In the absence of an established parameter for defining ECF lesion cut-off, we arbitrarily set it at 15% using the median split. By further splitting the group with ECF lesions ≥15% into subgroups using the upper quartile threshold of 30%, we showed that the selection of this cut-off did not change the main study finding (Table 2).

We investigated acute stroke patients within 10days without any other pathology on MRI to ensure that clinical testing was not influenced by reorganization of the brain.43,44 We specified language regions with the Stanford atlas of functional ROIs and only included patients whose lesions did not overlap with such defined regions. The vigorous selection of patients increases specificity, the downside is the need for relatively large number of patients to select from. The size of the study cohort of 123 stroke patients including 18 (14.6%) with aphasia may be considered as a study limitation. However, we found a strong and statistically significant association between ECF and aphasia. The external validity of this strong association has to be proven in further studies. A larger study cohort with more aphasia cases could have yielded more precise OR estimates. Multicentre studies or cloud-based stroke depositories may present a solution to increase study size.

The Stanford atlas does not include all brain regions potentially involved in language processing. Consequently, we did not examine and can therefore not exclude that patients with ≥15% affection of the extreme capsule cluster have lesions of other regions that contribute to aphasia.

The Token Test of the German version of the AAT26 is generally accepted as a reliable instrument for distinguishing between aphasia and no aphasia.45,46 In this study, we defined aphasia by an impaired Token Test performance only. To guarantee a comparison group definitely without language problems we excluded patients of the non-aphasia group as defined by Token Test criteria, who, however, were affected in the subtest for reading and writing. Thus, both groups, aphasia and no aphasia, are reliably separated. We considered aphasia as dichotomous variable and did not specify subgroups.

The neuroscientific question we asked, does lesioning of the ventral language pathway lead to aphasia independent from cortical lesions or lesions of the AF can clearly be answered with yes. While language is mainly processed in the cortex,9 aphasia after subcortical pathology is well studied.47,48 Various mechanisms are discussed as participation of subcortical structures in language processing beyond articulation, diaschisis, cortical hypoperfusion with undetected affection of cortical neurons on imaging or disconnection phenomena.49–55

For decades, the AF was regarded as the major fibre tract for language processing connecting Broca’s with Wernicke’s area16,17 and reports about lesions of AF leading to aphasia are frequent.56,57 The discovery of a ventral pathway in the auditory system58 and tracing experiments in primates20 led to the formulation of a dual-stream model for language processing.11,12,59,60 The model is similar to the original formulation of a ‘direct’ and an ‘indirect’ pathway by Wernicke (although he correlated the ‘direct’ pathway anatomically with extreme capsule fibres17) and re-discovered with today’s DTI techniques.22 In the dual-loop model of language processing, temporal lobe, including Wernicke’s area, and inferior frontal cortex, including Broca’s area, are connected via two association tracts, allowing processing of language along two pathways with different computational abilities.61 A dorsal pathway through the AF and the superior longitudinal fascicle fibre system is used for sensorimotor mapping for correct speech production (‘mapping sound onto articulation’), storage and retrieval of sequences, whereas a ventral pathway running through the extreme capsule mainly serves ‘mapping sound onto meaning’.8,11,13,21,22,62 The ventral stream allows categorization of items based on their structure,17,63 which is just what the Token Test tests.64

Disruptive intraoperative electrostimulation of the ECF leads to semantic paraphasia.65 Damage of the ventral pathway is known to cause a deficit in language comprehension, while damage of the dorsal pathway impairs speech production.28,66,67 Thus, while the dorsal pathway is needed for correct speech production as well as processing of chunks,68 the ventral pathway is necessary for comprehension. The Token Test examines language comprehension in addition to attention and other cognitive functions, not however overt language production.64 The Token Test may thus be especially suited to test ‘ventral’ stream functions.

Conclusions

The ASLM is a new and promising method. Our results support the relevance of a ventral pathway in the language network running through the extreme capsule. Extreme capsule lesions are an independent and strong predictor for aphasia after acute stroke, other than lesions in the AF.

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr Tobias Bormann from the Department of Neurology and Clinical Neurosciences, Freiburg, for critical reading of the manuscript. The authors gratefully acknowledge Stephan Rietbrock and Christopher Wallenhorst for their valuable statistical support during the preparation of this manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Brain-Links-BrainTools Cluster of Excellence funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG, grant number EXC 1086).

Competing interests

The authors report no competing interests.

References

1

Broca
P.
Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d’une observation d’aphémie (perte de la parole)
.
Bull. et mém. Soc. anat. de Paris
.
1861
;
6
:
330
357
.

2

Rorden
C
,
Karnath
HO.
Using human brain lesions to infer function: A relic from a past era in the fMRI age?
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2004
;
5
(
10
):
813
819
.

3

Bates
E
,
Wilson
SM
,
Saygin
AP
, et al.
Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping
.
Nat Neurosci
.
2003
;
6
(
5
):
448
450
.

4

Mah
YH
,
Husain
M
,
Rees
G
,
Nachev
P.
Human brain lesion-deficit inference remapped
.
Brain
.
2014
;
137
(
Pt 9
):
2522
2531
.

5

Sperber
C.
Rethinking causality and data complexity in brain lesion-behaviour inference and its implications for lesion-behaviour modelling
.
Cortex
.
2020
;
126
:
49
62
.

6

McKinnon
ET
,
Fridriksson
J
,
Basilakos
A
, et al.
Types of naming errors in chronic post-stroke aphasia are dissociated by dual stream axonal loss
.
Sci Rep
.
2018
;
8
(
1
):
14352
.

7

Geranmayeh
F
,
Chau
TW
,
Wise
RJS
,
Leech
R
,
Hampshire
A.
Domain-general subregions of the medial prefrontal cortex contribute to recovery of language after stroke
.
Brain
.
2017
;
140
(
7
):
1947
1958
.

8

Hagoort
P.
MUC (memory, unification, control): A model on the neurobiology of language beyond single word processing. In:
Hickok
G
,
Small
SL
, eds.
Neurobiology of language
.
Academic Press
;
2016
:
339
347
,
Chapter 28
.

9

Price
CJ.
The anatomy of language: A review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
.
2010
;
1191
:
62
88
.

10

Shirer
WR
,
Ryali
S
,
Rykhlevskaia
E
,
Menon
V
,
Greicius
MD.
Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2012
;
22
(
1
):
158
165
.

11

Hickok
G
,
Poeppel
D.
The cortical organization of speech processing
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2007
;
8
(
5
):
393
402
.

12

Rauschecker
JP
,
Scott
SK.
Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: Nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing
.
Nat Neurosci
.
2009
;
12
(
6
):
718
724
.

13

Dick
AS
,
Tremblay
P.
Beyond the arcuate fasciculus: Consensus and controversy in the connectional anatomy of language
.
Brain
.
2012
;
135
(
Pt 12
):
3529
3550
.

14

Petrides
M.
Neuroanatomy of Language Regions of the Human Brain
, 1st ed.
Academic Press
;
2014
.

15

FIPAT
. Terminologia Neuroanatomica. FIPAT.library.dal.ca. Federative International Programme for Anatomical Terminology;
2017
.

16

Catani
M
,
Mesulam
M.
The arcuate fasciculus and the disconnection theme in language and aphasia: History and current state
.
Cortex
.
2008
;
44
(
8
):
953
961
.

17

Weiller
C
,
Bormann
T
,
Saur
D
,
Musso
M
,
Rijntjes
M.
How the ventral pathway got lost: And what its recovery might mean
.
Brain Lang
.
2011
;
118
(
1-2
):
29
39
.

18

Catani
M
,
Howard
RJ
,
Pajevic
S
,
Jones
DK.
Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain
.
Neuroimage
.
2002
;
17
(
1
):
77
94
.

19

Schmahmann
JD
,
Pandya
D.
Fiber pathways of the brain
.
Oxford University Press
;
2006
.

20

Petrides
M
,
Pandya
DN.
Association fiber pathways to the frontal cortex from the superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey
.
J Comp Neurol
.
1988
;
273
(
1
):
52
66
.

21

Frey
S
,
Campbell
JS
,
Pike
GB
,
Petrides
M.
Dissociating the human language pathways with high angular resolution diffusion fiber tractography
.
J Neurosci
.
2008
;
28
(
45
):
11435
11444
.

22

Saur
D
,
Kreher
BW
,
Schnell
S
, et al.
Ventral and dorsal pathways for language
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2008
;
105
(
46
):
18035
18040
.

23

Makris
N
,
Pandya
DN.
The extreme capsule in humans and rethinking of the language circuitry
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
2009
;
213
(
3
):
343
358
.

24

Efthymiopoulou
E
,
Kasselimis
DS
,
Ghika
A
, et al.
The effect of cortical and subcortical lesions on spontaneous expression of memory-encoded and emotionally infused information: Evidence for a role of the ventral stream
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2017
;
101
:
115
120
.

25

Forkel
SJ
,
Catani
M.
Lesion mapping in acute stroke aphasia and its implications for recovery
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2018
;
115
:
88
100
.

26

Huber
W
,
Poeck
K
,
Willmes
K.
The Aachen Aphasia Test
.
Adv Neurol
.
1984
;
42
:
291
303
.

27

Beume
LA
,
Martin
M
,
Kaller
CP
, et al.
Visual neglect after left-hemispheric lesions: A voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study in 121 acute stroke patients
.
Exp Brain Res
.
2017
;
235
(
1
):
83
95
.

28

Kummerer
D
,
Hartwigsen
G
,
Kellmeyer
P
, et al.
Damage to ventral and dorsal language pathways in acute aphasia
.
Brain
.
2013
;
136
(
Pt 2
):
619
629
.

29

Martin
M
,
Nitschke
K
,
Beume
L
, et al.
Brain activity underlying tool-related and imitative skills after major left hemisphere stroke
.
Brain
.
2016
;
139
(
Pt 5
):
1497
1516
.

30

Stanford University
. Atlas of functional ROIs. Language Network;
2012
. https://findlab.stanford.edu/functional_ROIs.html Accessed 21 March 2021.

31

Morley
GK
,
Lundgren
S
,
Haxby
J.
Comparison and clinical applicability of auditory comprehension scores on the behavioral Neurology deficit evaluation, Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, porch index of communicative ability and token test
.
J Clin Neuropsychol
.
1979
;
1
(
3
):
249
258
.

32

De Renzi
E
,
Faglioni
P
,
Ferrari
P.
The influence of sex and age on the incidence and type of aphasia
.
Cortex
.
1980
;
16
(
4
):
627
630
.

33

Boller
F
,
Vignolo
LA.
Latent sensory aphasia in hemisphere-damaged patients: An experimental study with the Token Test
.
Brain
.
1966
;
89
(
4
):
815
830
.

34

Swisher
LP
,
Sarno
MT.
Token test scores of three matched patient groups: Left brain-damaged with aphasia; right brain-damaged without aphasia; non-brain-damaged
.
Cortex
.
1969
;
5
(
3
):
264
273
.

35

Orgass
B
,
Poeck
K.
A new aphasia-test for the diagnosis of speech comprehension disorders
.
Der Nervenarzt
.
1966
;
37
(
3
):
124
126
.

36

Department of Diagnostic Radiology MP
. University of Freiburg. Experimental soft-ware package “DTI&FiberTools”. http://www.uniklinik-freiburg.de/mr-en/research-groups/diffperf/fibertools.html Accessed 23 October 2020.

37

Reisert
M
,
Mader
I
,
Anastasopoulos
C
,
Weigel
M
,
Schnell
S
,
Kiselev
V.
Global fiber reconstruction becomes practical
.
Neuroimage
.
2011
;
54
(
2
):
955
962
.

38

Egger
K
,
Yang
S
,
Reisert
M
, et al.
Tractography of association fibers associated with language processing
.
Clin Neuroradiol
.
2015
;
25
(
Suppl 2
):
231
236
.

39

Orgass
B.
Eine Revision des Token-Tests. II. Validitätsnachweis. Normierung und Standardisierung
.
Diagnostica
.
1976
;
22
:
141
156
.

40

Fridriksson
J
,
Guo
D
,
Fillmore
P
,
Holland
A
,
Rorden
C.
Damage to the anterior arcuate fasciculus predicts non-fluent speech production in aphasia
.
Brain
.
2013
;
136
(
Pt 11
):
3451
3460
.

41

Nakae
T
,
Matsumoto
R
,
Kunieda
T
, et al.
Connectivity gradient in the human left inferior frontal gyrus: intraoperative cortico-cortical evoked potential study
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2020
;
30
(
8
):
4633
4650
.

42

Meynert
T.
Neue studien über die associations-bündel des hirnmantels
.
Mathnat Sci Class
.
1892
;
101
:
361
380
.

43

Karnath
HO
,
Rorden
C.
The anatomy of spatial neglect
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2012
;
50
(
6
):
1010
1017
.

44

Rijntjes
M.
Mechanisms of recovery in stroke patients with hemiparesis or aphasia: New insights, old questions and the meaning of therapies
.
Curr Opin Neurol
.
2006
;
19
(
1
):
76
83
.

45

Spellacy
FJ
,
Spreen
O.
A short form of the token test
.
Cortex
.
1969
;
5
(
4
):
390
397
.

46

van Dongen
HR
,
van Harskamp
F.
The token test. A preliminary evaluation of a method to detect aphasia
.
Psychiatr Neurol Neurochir
.
1972
;
75
(
2
):
129
134
.

47

Kirk
A
,
Kertesz
A.
Cortical and subcortical aphasias compared
.
Aphasiology
.
1994
;
8
(
1
):
65
82
.

48

Wallesch
C-W.
Symptomatology of subcortical aphasia
.
J Neurolinguistics
.
1997
;
10
(
4
):
267
275
.

49

Cappa
SF
,
Cavallotti
G
,
Guidotti
M
,
Papagno
C
,
Vignolo
LA.
Subcortical aphasia: Two clinical-CT scan correlation studies
.
Cortex
.
1983
;
19
(
2
):
227
241
.

50

Crosson
B.
Subcortical functions in language: A working model
.
Brain Lang
.
1985
;
25
(
2
):
257
292
.

51

Hillis
AE
,
Wityk
RJ
,
Barker
PB
, et al.
Subcortical aphasia and neglect in acute stroke: The role of cortical hypoperfusion
.
Brain
.
2002
;
125
(
5
):
1094
1104
.

52

Perani
D
,
Vallar
G
,
Cappa
S
,
Messa
C
,
Fazio
F.
Aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke. A clinical/cerebral perfusion correlation study
.
Brain
.
1987
;
110
(
5
):
1211
1229
.

53

Thiebaut de Schotten
M
,
Foulon
C
,
Nachev
P.
Brain disconnections link structural connectivity with function and behaviour
.
Nat Commun
.
2020
;
11
(
1
):
5094
.

54

Weiller
C
,
Ringelstein
EB
,
Reiche
W
,
Thron
A
,
Buell
U.
The large striatocapsular infarct. A clinical and pathophysiological entity
.
Arch Neurol
.
1990
;
47
(
10
):
1085
1091
.

55

Weiller
C
,
Willmes
K
,
Reiche
W
, et al.
The case of aphasia or neglect after striatocapsular infarction
.
Brain
.
1993
;
116
(
6
):
1509
1525
.

56

Berthier
ML
,
Lambon Ralph
MA.
Dissecting the function of networks underpinning language repetition
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2014
;
8
:
727
.

57

Ardila
A.
A review of conduction aphasia
.
Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep
.
2010
;
10
(
6
):
499
503
.

58

Romanski
LM
,
Tian
B
,
Fritz
J
,
Mishkin
M
,
Goldman-Rakic
PS
,
Rauschecker
JP.
Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex
.
Nat Neurosci
.
1999
;
2
(
12
):
1131
1136
.

59

Pulvermüller
F.
Brain mechanisms linking language and action
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2005
;
6
(
7
):
576
582
.

60

Wise
RJ.
Language systems in normal and aphasic human subjects: Functional imaging studies and inferences from animal studies
.
Br Med Bull
.
2003
;
65
:
95
119
.

61

Weiller
C
,
Vry
MS
,
Saur
D
,
Umarova
R
,
Rijntjes
M.
Remote dysfunction;
2015
.

62

Rauschecker
JP
,
Scott
SK.
Pathways and streams in the auditory cortex. In:
Hickok
G
,
Small
SL
, eds.
Neurobiology of language
.
Academic Press
;
2016
.
287
298
.

63

Weiller
C
,
Musso
M
,
Rijntjes
M
,
Saur
D.
Please don't underestimate the ventral pathway in language
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2009
;
13
(
9
):
369
370
,
370
1
.

64

De Renzi
E
,
Vignolo
LA.
The token test: A sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics
.
Brain
.
1962
;
85
:
665
678
.

65

Duffau
H
,
Gatignol
P
,
Mandonnet
E
,
Peruzzi
P
,
Tzourio-Mazoyer
N
,
Capelle
L.
New insights into the anatomo-functional connectivity of the semantic system: A study using cortico-subcortical electrostimulations
.
Brain
.
2005
;
128
(
Pt 4
):
797
810
.

66

Fridriksson
J
,
den Ouden
DB
,
Hillis
AE
, et al.
Anatomy of aphasia revisited
.
Brain
.
2018
;
141
(
3
):
848
862
.

67

Fridriksson
J
,
Yourganov
G
,
Bonilha
L
,
Basilakos
A
,
Den Ouden
DB
,
Rorden
C.
Revealing the dual streams of speech processing
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2016
;
113
(
52
):
15108
15113
.

68

Blumenthal-Dramé
A
,
Glauche
V
,
Bormann
T
,
Weiller
C
,
Musso
M
,
Kortmann
B.
Frequency and chunking in derived words: A parametric fMRI study
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2017
;
29
(
7
):
1162
1177
.

Abbreviations

    Abbreviations
     
  • AAT =

    Aachen Aphasia Test

  •  
  • AF =

    arcuate fascicle

  •  
  • ASLM =

    anatomy-based symptom-lesion mapping

  •  
  • DTI =

    Diffusion Tensor Imaging

  •  
  • ECF =

    extreme capsule fibre

  •  
  • MRI =

    magnetic resonance imaging

  •  
  • OR =

    odds ratio

  •  
  • ROI =

    region of interest

  •  
  • VLSM =

    voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.