Abstract

Objective

Inadequate supervision has been linked to children’s injuries. Parental injury prevention beliefs may play a role in supervision, yet little theory has examined the origins of such beliefs. This study examined whether mothers who perpetrated child neglect, who as a group provide inadequate supervision, have more maladaptive beliefs. Then, it tested a social information processing (SIP) model for explaining these beliefs.

Methods

SIP and injury prevention beliefs were assessed in disadvantaged mothers of preschoolers (N  =  145), half with child neglect histories.

Results

The neglect group exhibited significantly more maladaptive injury prevention beliefs than comparisons. As predicted, SIP was linked to beliefs that may increase injury risk, even after accounting for relevant sociodemographic variables.

Conclusions

Findings support the link of beliefs to injury risk and suggest that specific cognitive problems may underlie these beliefs. Future work should further validate this model, which may inform enhancements to prevention efforts.

Unintentional injuries are a leading cause of child death and disability (CDC, 2013; Corso, Finkelstein, Miller, Fiebelkorn, & Zaloshnja, 2006). Approximately 10% of preschool-aged children experience a nonfatal unintentional injury resulting in emergency department treatment each year, and costs of nonfatal injuries in this age group alone total more than $11.4 billion (CDC, 2013). Rates of injuries not requiring medical attention are likely substantially higher. Children in low-income, single-parent families and those in disadvantaged neighborhoods are at especially high risk (Haynes, Reading, & Gale, 2003; Laflamme et al., 2010), and understanding risk within this group is crucial for prevention efforts. Parental supervision is one factor thought to be crucial to injury prevention (Morrongiello, 2005; Morrongiello & Schell, 2010; Saluja et al., 2004), especially for preschoolers, who are most often injured at home (Morrongiello, Midgett, & Shields, 2001). Inadequate supervision may constitute child neglect (Coohey, 2003); indeed, it is the most frequent reason for referrals (Nelson, Saunders, & Landsman, 1993), and is estimated as a factor in 44% of neglect cases (Ruiz-Casares, Trocmé, & Fallon, 2012). In one study of preschoolers in foster care, almost 90% had experienced supervisory neglect (Pears, Kim, & Fisher, 2008). Death record studies show inadequate supervision as a factor in almost half of maltreatment fatalities (44%; Damashek, Drass, & Bonner, 2014) and child injury fatalities (43%; Landen, Bauer, & Kohn, 2003).

Given its consequences (Coohey, 2003; Morrongiello & Schell, 2010; Peterson & Brown, 1994), research is needed to identify factors that increase risk for providing inadequate supervision. Parents’ beliefs are argued to reflect different supervision styles and, although supervision is determined by many factors, provide the basis for parents’ decisions about supervision across situations (Morrongiello & House, 2004; Morrongiello & Schell, 2010). Beliefs have been shown to be associated with parents’ behavior in naturalistic observations, as well as children’s injuries, suggesting beliefs may be a valid marker for supervision and injury risk (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2006). Given that instruments assessing beliefs are a low-cost way to assess risk, research linking existing measures to risk for lack of supervision is needed. One aim of this study is to do this. In addition, theory-based research that sheds light on the origins of such beliefs is needed to inform prevention efforts. This study sought to extend a cognitive theory, designed to explain parenting risk and shown to be useful in understanding risk for neglect, to childhood injury risk by examining links between social cognitive and neurocognitive problems and parental injury prevention beliefs.

Parental Social Information Processing and Injury Risk

Research on childhood injury is typically descriptive, with a relative paucity of theory-driven studies (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008). Social cognitive models have been suggested as useful for understanding how parents act (or fail to act) to prevent injuries (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008), and there is considerable overlap between antecedents of child injury and maltreatment (Peterson & Brown, 1994). Indeed, social information processing (SIP) models of parenting risk that have been used to explain risk for abuse (Azar, 1986; Azar, Reitz, & Goslin, 2008; Milner, 2003) are also being applied to neglect (Azar, Stevenson, & Johnson, 2012; Crittenden, 1993), and SIP theory is hypothesized as relevant for understanding parents’ injury prevention-related beliefs and behaviors (Azar & Weinzierl, 2005).

The SIP model of parenting posits that deficits in parents’ social cognitive and neurocognitive capacities increase risk for maladaptive parenting behaviors by changing the ways in which they attend to, take in, and act on information from the social environment. SIP capacities include schemas (e.g., expectations of children’s capacities), executive functioning (EF) and problem-solving abilities, and attributions (Azar, Robinson, Hekimian, & Twentyman, 1984; Azar et al., 2012; Milner, 2003). Greater SIP deficits have been shown to characterize parents who maltreat their child compared with control samples (Azar et al., 1984, 2012; Azar & Rohrbeck, 1986; Bugental & Happaney, 2004; Haskett, Smith Scott, Grant, Ward, & Robinson, 2003; Milner, 2003). Azar and Weinzierl (2005) argue that parental SIP deficits are also relevant to children’s injury risk, although less empirical work has examined this question. Here we provide a brief overview of how parental SIP capacities are hypothesized to link to children’s injury risk; for a more in-depth discussion, see Azar and Weinzierl (2005).

Parents’ schemas (or expectations) of children must be flexible and complex enough to adjust to their developmental changes (e.g., mobility, reach) and individual differences (e.g., risk-taking) (Azar & Weinzierl, 2005). Parents often overestimate children’s abilities (e.g., for self-care) and knowledge (e.g., of safety rules) (Morrongiello et al., 2001; Peterson, Mori, & Scissors, 1986; Schwebel & Bounds, 2003), and this is likely to be especially pronounced when parents view children as “little adults,” resulting in children being placed in risky situations more often. Indeed, beliefs that children learn from injuries are common and place the responsibility on the child to avoid future injuries; not surprisingly, parents are then less likely to act to minimize risk (Lewis, DiLillo, & Peterson, 2004; Morrongiello & Dayler, 1996; Simpson, Turnbull, Ardagh, & Richardson, 2009). Parents often believe childhood injuries are an inevitable part of development (Morrongiello & Dayler, 1996; Simpson et al., 2009; Whitehead & Owens, 2012) and may also have positive beliefs regarding the impact of being injured on children (e.g., children are toughened by injuries) that contribute to their lack of preventative actions (Lewis et al., 2004). Indeed, unrealistic expectations for children have been linked to both neglect and injury (Azar, 1986; Azar et al., 1984, 2012; Bugental & Happaney, 2004; Morrongiello & House, 2004).

Parents’ EF capacities provide a foundation for higher-order cognitive skills, such as problem-solving, and are essential to learning (Azar et al., 2012; Azar & Weinzierl, 2005; Barrett & Fleming, 2011). Good EF and problem-solving skills are needed to respond to changing environmental cues and child behaviors as children grow and develop. Parents with EF deficits are likely to have difficulty planning and organizing activities, anticipating and identifying safety risks, monitoring children and the environment, and adjusting their responses (e.g., vigilance level) accordingly, leading to inadequate supervision and greater injury risk (Azar & Weinzierl, 2005; Johnston, Mash, Miller, & Ninowski, 2012). For example, poor inhibition may contribute to difficulty maintaining attention to child cues in the face of competing, more rewarding stimuli (e.g., leaving an enjoyable TV show to follow a child into another room) (Johnston et al., 2012). Indeed, parents who have perpetrated maltreatment exhibit poorer problem-solving than comparison ones (Azar et al., 1984, 2012; Hansen, Pallotta, Tishelman, Conaway, & MacMillan, 1989; Milner, 2003).

Although less work has examined more specific EF capacities, there is some evidence that mothers who have maltreated children are less cognitively flexible (e.g., have difficulty shifting set, responding to feedback) than comparison mothers (Azar et al., 2012; Fontaine & Nolin, 2012; Nayak & Milner, 1998), and this predicts their parenting even after considering general intellectual functioning (Azar et al., 2012; Fontaine & Nolin, 2012). More research is needed to link parents’ EF with child injury risk; however, EF in older children has been linked to their attendance to safety cues and behavior in a potential risky situation (i.e., road crossing) (Barton & Morrongiello, 2011), supporting the hypothesis that parents’ EF contributes to the ability to attend to relevant environmental cues. Parents with poorer EF may fail to learn from their children’s injuries and, thus, may develop and maintain more maladaptive beliefs about supervision and the benefits of injuries.

Finally, negative and maladaptive appraisals of child behavior may result from unrealistic expectations and poor EF and problem-solving, further increasing injury risk (Azar & Weinzierl, 2005; Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008). Simplistic, unrealistic schemas are likely to be violated, and poor EF limits generation of response strategies and more benign appraisals, resulting in more hostile attributions, leading to harsher responses, as well as more self-blaming attributions, leading to disengagement from child care (Azar & Weinzierl, 2005). Hostile attributions have been associated with more lax discipline (Leung & Slep, 2006) and child neglect (Azar et al., 2012). Previous research has shown that parents who attribute injuries to external factors are less likely to take action (e.g., teaching, changing the environment) following an injury (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008; Peterson, Bartelstone, Kern, & Gillies, 1995; Peterson & Brown, 1994), but the idea of a global negative bias toward the child has not received as much attention. Hostile attributions may be most relevant in disadvantaged contexts where environmental stress is high and parents’ own needs are not met, as attributing injuries to child characteristics allows parents to preserve parenting self-efficacy (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008). Hostile attributions for risky behavior (e.g., “she knows better and is just acting that way to annoy me”) are less likely to lead to preventative action than more adaptive attributions (e.g., “she doesn’t realize how dangerous her behavior is”) and may lead parents to have beliefs that injuries are inevitable and due to fate or luck.

Current Study

Because parents’ supervision beliefs are seen as closely linked to supervision and some evidence supporting this contention is available (e.g., research examining maternal monitoring on the playground, actual child injuries; Morrongiello & Corbett, 2006; Morrongiello & House, 2004), examining SIP predictors of injury prevention beliefs seems worthy of consideration. The study used a sample of mothers of preschoolers who were sociodemographically disadvantaged and therefore are at high risk for unintentional injuries (Laflamme et al., 2010). Furthermore, half of the sample had a history of child neglect, indicating especially high risk for inadequate supervision. Hypotheses were as follows:

Hypothesis 1. Mothers who perpetrated neglect will have more maladaptive and less adaptive injury prevention beliefs than comparisons, further validating the importance of beliefs to risk for inadequate supervision.

Hypothesis 2. SIP deficits will be associated with more maladaptive and less adaptive injury prevention beliefs, after considering IQ and other demographic risk factors, supporting the validity of the SIP model for injury risk.

Methods

Participants

Low-socioeconomic status mothers of 3–5-year-old children (N  =  145) were recruited for a study of parenting from agencies serving disadvantaged families (e.g., Head Start, low-income day cares) and parents involved with child protective services (CPS). Recruitment took place in the poorest neighborhoods of Philadelphia, and the project was described to participants as a study of parenting. With mothers’ permission, all were screened for records of CPS involvement. The study sample includes 69 mothers with CPS histories for neglect and 76 comparison mothers with no CPS histories and no evidence of risk for physical abuse using the Brief Child Abuse Potential Inventory (Ondersma, Chaffin, Mullins, & LeBreton, 2005). This study had university institutional review board approval.

Procedure

Data collection was completed over three home visits of 2.5 hours each. Breaks were given as needed to prevent fatigue. In the first session, participants provided informed consent, consent for CPS record review, and demographic information. To control for literacy difficulties, consent forms and all instruments were read to mothers. Care was taken to ensure that participants understood the use of Likert scales. Participants received $150 for participation.

Measures

IQ

Four subscales of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Arithmetic, Matrix Reasoning, Information, and Coding; Wechsler, 2008) were administered, providing a reliable estimate of IQ (r  =  .93; Sattler & Ryan, 2009).

Parent Opinion Questionnaire

The Parent Opinion Questionnaire (POQ) assesses unrealistic expectations for children (Azar et al., 1984). It has 80 agree–disagree items (e.g., “Usually, a 2-year-old can sit and play quietly alone in a room for several hours”). Higher scores indicate more unrealistic expectations. It has good test–retest reliability (r  =  .85; Haskett, Scott, Willoughby, Ahern, & Nears, 2006) and internal consistency (Cronbach’s α  =  .79), and has been shown to differentiate maltreating mothers from comparisons (Azar et al., 1984, 2012; Azar & Rohrbeck, 1986).

Parent Problem Solving Inventory

The Parent Problem Solving Inventory (PPSI) was used to assess problem-solving abilities in parenting situations. Ten typical childrearing problems were presented in story form (Wasik & Bryant, 1994). Participants were read the beginning of each story (e.g., a child begins to tantrum in a grocery store and the mother is embarrassed) and the story end, where the problem is resolved (e.g., the child is calm), and must provide the middle of the story (i.e., solution[s] to the problem). Responses were recorded by interviewers and scored later by independent raters. For this study, the number of different types of solutions provided across stories was used in analyses. Interrater agreement was 90%. In prior studies, the PPSI has distinguished maltreating from non-maltreating parents (Azar et al., 1984, 2012).

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is a neuropsychological test of “set-shifting,” that is, the ability to display flexibility in the face of changing stimuli and rules (Heaton, 1981). For this study, the number of perseverative errors (i.e., the number of times a participant continued to use a rule that was no longer correct) was used. More perseverative errors indicate less cognitive flexibility and more difficulty “set-shifting.”

Alternate Uses Test

Cognitive flexibility was also assessed using the Alternate Uses Test (AUT; Guilford, Christiansen, Merrifield, & Wilson, 1978). Participants were asked to produce as many uses as possible, other than the typical use, for six familiar objects within a set amount of time. This type of task assesses divergent thinking, and production of new responses is seen as reflecting underlying EF capacities (Gilhooly, Fioratou, Anthony, & Wynn, 2007). In this study, a higher number of unique and acceptable responses indicated greater flexibility. Interrater agreement for a randomly selected subsample (20 cases) was 93%.

Child Vignettes

The Child Vignettes (CV) assesses hostile attributions. Participants were read 18 vignettes of hypothetical aversive child behavior (Plotkin, 1983), asked to imagine that the child is their own, and rated how much they think the child did the behavior to annoy them on a 9-point scale from “not at all” to “very much.” Higher scores indicate more hostile attributions. Cronbach’s α was .87 in this sample. This measure has distinguished mothers with maltreatment histories from control mothers (Azar et al., 2012; Haskett et al., 2006; Plotkin, 1983).

Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire

The Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire (PSAPQ) is a 29-item questionnaire measuring supervision beliefs thought to underlie situation-specific supervision behaviors (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2006; Morrongiello & House, 2004). Participants were asked to think about being on a playground with their child and rated how often each item is true on a 5-point scale from “never” to “all of the time.” Factor analysis has identified four subscales. Items in the Protectiveness (e.g., “I warn him/her about things that could be dangerous”) and Supervision (e.g., “I have my child within arm’s reach at all times”) subscales assess beliefs about active supervision and harm reduction. Items in the Risk Tolerance subscale (e.g., “I let him/her learn from his/her own mishaps”) assess acceptance of children’s risk-taking behavior, and those in the Belief in Fate subscale (e.g., “When my child gets injured it is due to bad luck”) assess beliefs that children’s injuries are due to fate. The PSAPQ has good test–retest and internal reliability (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2006); Cronbach’s α was .76 in this sample. It has been linked to child injury and parents’ observed supervision (Morrongiello & House, 2004).

Injury Attitudes Questionnaire

The Injury Attitudes Questionnaire (IAQ) is a 30-item measure assessing perceived benefits of child injuries (Lewis et al., 2004). Participants indicated their agreement with each statement (e.g., “After being injured, my child usually learns not to do the same thing again”) on a 7-point scale from “very strongly disagree” to “very strongly agree.” Previous research has found that items load onto two factors—beliefs that children learn from injuries and beliefs that children are toughened by injuries (Lewis et al., 2004). The IAQ has good test–retest reliability and internal consistency (Lewis et al., 2004); Cronbach’s α was .83 in this sample.

Analyses

Given considerable theoretical overlap between supervision beliefs and injury beliefs, a factor analysis of the PSAPQ and IAQ subscales was conducted. The six subscales loaded onto two factors that were used in analyses (Table I). The first factor consisted of the risk tolerance and belief in fate PSAPQ subscales and the toughening and learning IAQ subscales, and was named “Maladaptive Injury Prevention Beliefs.” The second consisted of the Protectiveness and Supervision PSAPQ subscales, and was named “Adaptive Injury Prevention Beliefs.”

Table I.

Factor Analysis of PSAPQ and IAQ Subscales

VariablesFactor 1 loadings maladaptive injury prevention beliefsFactor 2 loadings adaptive injury prevention beliefs
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance.740.206
PSAPQ: Belief in fate.551.385
IAQ: Toughening.768.164
IAQ: Learning.737.070
PSAPQ: Protectiveness−.229.841
PSAPQ: Supervision−.486.720
VariablesFactor 1 loadings maladaptive injury prevention beliefsFactor 2 loadings adaptive injury prevention beliefs
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance.740.206
PSAPQ: Belief in fate.551.385
IAQ: Toughening.768.164
IAQ: Learning.737.070
PSAPQ: Protectiveness−.229.841
PSAPQ: Supervision−.486.720

Note. PSAPQ = Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire; IAQ = Injury Attitudes Questionnaire. Highest loadings for each subscale are bolded.

Table I.

Factor Analysis of PSAPQ and IAQ Subscales

VariablesFactor 1 loadings maladaptive injury prevention beliefsFactor 2 loadings adaptive injury prevention beliefs
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance.740.206
PSAPQ: Belief in fate.551.385
IAQ: Toughening.768.164
IAQ: Learning.737.070
PSAPQ: Protectiveness−.229.841
PSAPQ: Supervision−.486.720
VariablesFactor 1 loadings maladaptive injury prevention beliefsFactor 2 loadings adaptive injury prevention beliefs
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance.740.206
PSAPQ: Belief in fate.551.385
IAQ: Toughening.768.164
IAQ: Learning.737.070
PSAPQ: Protectiveness−.229.841
PSAPQ: Supervision−.486.720

Note. PSAPQ = Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire; IAQ = Injury Attitudes Questionnaire. Highest loadings for each subscale are bolded.

Results

Demographics

On average, participants had less than a high school education (M  =  11.49 years, SD  =  1.15) and almost all were single parents (89.0%) with incomes below the federal poverty line (90.3%). The sample was primarily African American (79.3%). They had an average of 2.83 children (SD  =  1.41), with 57% of the preschool children being female. Compared with the control group, the neglect group was slightly older (M  =  28.25 vs. M  =  26.19, t  = −2.32, p  =  .02), had lower IQs (M  =  77.86 vs. M  =  81.59, t  =  2.03, p  =  .04), and had more children (M  =  3.36 vs. M  =  2.36, t  = −4.59, p  <  .01). Mother age and IQ were controlled for in all analyses, as were child age and gender, which have been associated with parental supervision beliefs (Damashek, Borduin, & Ronis, 2014; Morrongiello & Dawber, 1999; Morrongiello, Walpole, & McArthur, 2009).

Hypothesis 1: Injury prevention beliefs of the neglect group mothers were compared with those of comparison mothers using analysis of covariance, with mother age and IQ, and child age and gender as covariates (Table II). The neglect group endorsed significantly greater beliefs in fate as the cause of injuries and showed a trend toward stronger beliefs that injuries toughen children. They also had significantly higher scores than comparison mothers on the overall maladaptive beliefs factor score, supporting the link of maladaptive injury prevention beliefs to parenting risk. No significant group differences were found in adaptive injury prevention beliefs.

Table II.

CPS Neglect Status: Group Differences in Supervision and Injury Beliefs

VariablesObserved mean (SD)Adjusted mean (SE)SS/MSF(1, 141)
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance0.340.71
Neglect (n = 69)3.02 (0.72)3.04 (0.09)
Comparison (n = 76)2.96 (0.66)2.94 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Belief in fatea5.0710.23**
Neglect2.03 (0.77)2.00 (0.09)
Comparison1.58 (0.66)1.61 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Protectiveness0.120.63
Neglect4.15 (0.44)4.15 (0.05)
Comparison4.21 (0.44)4.21 (0.05)
PSAPQ: Supervisionb0.000.00
Neglect3.99 (0.43)3.99 (0.06)
Comparison3.99 (0.49)3.99 (0.05)
IAQ: Tougheningc4.392.79+
Neglect3.25 (1.31)3.33 (0.15)
Comparison3.03 (1.27)2.97 (0.15)
IAQ: Learningc,d1.071.07
Neglect4.77 (0.89)4.82 (0.12)
Comparison4.69 (1.15)4.65 (0.12)
Maladaptive Supervision Beliefs3.814.04*
Neglect0.14 (0.96)0.18 (0.12)
Comparison−0.13 (1.02)−0.16 (0.11)
Adaptive Supervision Beliefs0.500.52
Neglect0.06 (0.99)0.06 (0.12)
Comparison−0.06 (1.02)−0.06 (0.12)
VariablesObserved mean (SD)Adjusted mean (SE)SS/MSF(1, 141)
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance0.340.71
Neglect (n = 69)3.02 (0.72)3.04 (0.09)
Comparison (n = 76)2.96 (0.66)2.94 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Belief in fatea5.0710.23**
Neglect2.03 (0.77)2.00 (0.09)
Comparison1.58 (0.66)1.61 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Protectiveness0.120.63
Neglect4.15 (0.44)4.15 (0.05)
Comparison4.21 (0.44)4.21 (0.05)
PSAPQ: Supervisionb0.000.00
Neglect3.99 (0.43)3.99 (0.06)
Comparison3.99 (0.49)3.99 (0.05)
IAQ: Tougheningc4.392.79+
Neglect3.25 (1.31)3.33 (0.15)
Comparison3.03 (1.27)2.97 (0.15)
IAQ: Learningc,d1.071.07
Neglect4.77 (0.89)4.82 (0.12)
Comparison4.69 (1.15)4.65 (0.12)
Maladaptive Supervision Beliefs3.814.04*
Neglect0.14 (0.96)0.18 (0.12)
Comparison−0.13 (1.02)−0.16 (0.11)
Adaptive Supervision Beliefs0.500.52
Neglect0.06 (0.99)0.06 (0.12)
Comparison−0.06 (1.02)−0.06 (0.12)

Note. CPS = Child Protective Services; PSAPQ = Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire; IAQ = Injury Attitudes Questionnaire; SS = Type III Sum of Square; MS = Mean Square.

Mother IQ, mother age, index child age, and index child gender are included as covariates.

aNegative association with mother IQ.

bHigher scores for mothers of girls.

cNegative association with mother age.

dPositive association with index child age.

+

p  <  .10, *p  <  .05 ** p  <  .01.

Table II.

CPS Neglect Status: Group Differences in Supervision and Injury Beliefs

VariablesObserved mean (SD)Adjusted mean (SE)SS/MSF(1, 141)
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance0.340.71
Neglect (n = 69)3.02 (0.72)3.04 (0.09)
Comparison (n = 76)2.96 (0.66)2.94 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Belief in fatea5.0710.23**
Neglect2.03 (0.77)2.00 (0.09)
Comparison1.58 (0.66)1.61 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Protectiveness0.120.63
Neglect4.15 (0.44)4.15 (0.05)
Comparison4.21 (0.44)4.21 (0.05)
PSAPQ: Supervisionb0.000.00
Neglect3.99 (0.43)3.99 (0.06)
Comparison3.99 (0.49)3.99 (0.05)
IAQ: Tougheningc4.392.79+
Neglect3.25 (1.31)3.33 (0.15)
Comparison3.03 (1.27)2.97 (0.15)
IAQ: Learningc,d1.071.07
Neglect4.77 (0.89)4.82 (0.12)
Comparison4.69 (1.15)4.65 (0.12)
Maladaptive Supervision Beliefs3.814.04*
Neglect0.14 (0.96)0.18 (0.12)
Comparison−0.13 (1.02)−0.16 (0.11)
Adaptive Supervision Beliefs0.500.52
Neglect0.06 (0.99)0.06 (0.12)
Comparison−0.06 (1.02)−0.06 (0.12)
VariablesObserved mean (SD)Adjusted mean (SE)SS/MSF(1, 141)
PSAPQ: Risk tolerance0.340.71
Neglect (n = 69)3.02 (0.72)3.04 (0.09)
Comparison (n = 76)2.96 (0.66)2.94 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Belief in fatea5.0710.23**
Neglect2.03 (0.77)2.00 (0.09)
Comparison1.58 (0.66)1.61 (0.08)
PSAPQ: Protectiveness0.120.63
Neglect4.15 (0.44)4.15 (0.05)
Comparison4.21 (0.44)4.21 (0.05)
PSAPQ: Supervisionb0.000.00
Neglect3.99 (0.43)3.99 (0.06)
Comparison3.99 (0.49)3.99 (0.05)
IAQ: Tougheningc4.392.79+
Neglect3.25 (1.31)3.33 (0.15)
Comparison3.03 (1.27)2.97 (0.15)
IAQ: Learningc,d1.071.07
Neglect4.77 (0.89)4.82 (0.12)
Comparison4.69 (1.15)4.65 (0.12)
Maladaptive Supervision Beliefs3.814.04*
Neglect0.14 (0.96)0.18 (0.12)
Comparison−0.13 (1.02)−0.16 (0.11)
Adaptive Supervision Beliefs0.500.52
Neglect0.06 (0.99)0.06 (0.12)
Comparison−0.06 (1.02)−0.06 (0.12)

Note. CPS = Child Protective Services; PSAPQ = Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire; IAQ = Injury Attitudes Questionnaire; SS = Type III Sum of Square; MS = Mean Square.

Mother IQ, mother age, index child age, and index child gender are included as covariates.

aNegative association with mother IQ.

bHigher scores for mothers of girls.

cNegative association with mother age.

dPositive association with index child age.

+

p  <  .10, *p  <  .05 ** p  <  .01.

Hypothesis 2: Two regressions were run to test the contributions of SIP deficits to maladaptive and adaptive injury prevention belief factors. The first examined maladaptive injury prevention beliefs, with mother age and IQ, child age and gender, and neglect status entered in the first block and SIP variables in the second block (Table III). SIP variables significantly improved model fit, and neglect status was no longer a significant predictor when considered along with SIP variables. In the final model, mother age and IQ, child gender, unrealistic expectations, hostile attributions, and perseverative errors were significant individual predictors. Along with control variables, SIP factors accounted for approximately 31% of the variance in maladaptive injury prevention beliefs. The second regression examined adaptive injury prevention beliefs. Covariates were entered in the first block, and SIP variables were entered in the second block. After accounting for covariates, there was a trend indicating that SIP variables as a group added some predictive power (R2  = 11%, F(10, 134)  =  1.71, p  = .09), although no control or SIP variable alone contributed significantly (full results available from authors).

Table III.

Social Information Processing Deficits Predicting Maladaptive Supervision Beliefs

Independent variablesR2Adjusted R2F for change in R2BSEβ
Model 1.090.0572.76*
 Mother age−.029.016−.158+
 Mother IQ.009.008.101
 Index child age.176.094.154+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.288.172−.143+
 Neglect.337.168.169*
Model 2.307.2555.94**
 Mother age−.033.014−.179*
 Mother IQ.021.009.230*
 Index child age.151.085.132+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.337.155−.168*
 Neglect.114.156.057
 Unrealistic expectations.057.015.293**
 Problem-solving.025.021.093
 Hostile attributions.012.004.265**
 Perseverative errors.014.006.196*
 Alternate uses.020.017.096
Independent variablesR2Adjusted R2F for change in R2BSEβ
Model 1.090.0572.76*
 Mother age−.029.016−.158+
 Mother IQ.009.008.101
 Index child age.176.094.154+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.288.172−.143+
 Neglect.337.168.169*
Model 2.307.2555.94**
 Mother age−.033.014−.179*
 Mother IQ.021.009.230*
 Index child age.151.085.132+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.337.155−.168*
 Neglect.114.156.057
 Unrealistic expectations.057.015.293**
 Problem-solving.025.021.093
 Hostile attributions.012.004.265**
 Perseverative errors.014.006.196*
 Alternate uses.020.017.096
+

p  <  .10, *p  <  .05, **p  <  .01.

Table III.

Social Information Processing Deficits Predicting Maladaptive Supervision Beliefs

Independent variablesR2Adjusted R2F for change in R2BSEβ
Model 1.090.0572.76*
 Mother age−.029.016−.158+
 Mother IQ.009.008.101
 Index child age.176.094.154+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.288.172−.143+
 Neglect.337.168.169*
Model 2.307.2555.94**
 Mother age−.033.014−.179*
 Mother IQ.021.009.230*
 Index child age.151.085.132+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.337.155−.168*
 Neglect.114.156.057
 Unrealistic expectations.057.015.293**
 Problem-solving.025.021.093
 Hostile attributions.012.004.265**
 Perseverative errors.014.006.196*
 Alternate uses.020.017.096
Independent variablesR2Adjusted R2F for change in R2BSEβ
Model 1.090.0572.76*
 Mother age−.029.016−.158+
 Mother IQ.009.008.101
 Index child age.176.094.154+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.288.172−.143+
 Neglect.337.168.169*
Model 2.307.2555.94**
 Mother age−.033.014−.179*
 Mother IQ.021.009.230*
 Index child age.151.085.132+
 Index child gender (female = 1)−.337.155−.168*
 Neglect.114.156.057
 Unrealistic expectations.057.015.293**
 Problem-solving.025.021.093
 Hostile attributions.012.004.265**
 Perseverative errors.014.006.196*
 Alternate uses.020.017.096
+

p  <  .10, *p  <  .05, **p  <  .01.

Discussion

Childhood injury risk is clearly multidetermined by an array of contextual, parental, and child factors. This study focused on risk indicators for parental inadequate supervision. It had two goals. First, it examined the idea that supervision beliefs may be a valuable alternative to costly observational approaches (Morrongiello & House, 2004; Morrongiello & Schell, 2010), using two belief instruments. As predicted, neglect group mothers reported more maladaptive injury prevention beliefs. The second goal of this study was to test whether social cognitive and neurocognitive factors are linked to injury prevention beliefs. SIP deficits, specifically unrealistic expectations, EF, and hostile attributions were strongly linked to maladaptive injury prevention beliefs, over and above maternal IQ and other demographic risk factors (e.g., child gender, age). Adaptive injury prevention beliefs, however, did not differ between groups, and there was only limited support for links between SIP deficits and adaptive beliefs.

As beliefs have mostly been studied in populations at lower risk for unintentional injuries, finding neglect/non-neglect group differences in this disadvantaged sample is notable given the high sociodemographic risk in the comparison group. This finding suggests markers may exist within populations most at risk and allow targeted prevention. Preventing unintentional childhood injury requires well-validated etiological models (Azar et al., 2008; Morrongiello & Schwebel, 2008; Peterson & Brown, 1994; Slack et al., 2011). Injury research has already identified social cognitive problems as playing a role in injury (e.g., attributions, expectations; Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008; Peterson & Brown, 1994). Work in maltreatment has also focused in on a specific set of parental social cognitive and neurocognitive determinants, including expectations of children, problem-solving and EF, and attributions, and documented a similar role in parenting risk for neglect (Azar et al., 2012). The findings of this study extend the validity of this global model of parenting risk and elaborate on parental cognitive factors that may negatively affect parental decision-making and actions, and potentially increase the occurrence of childhood injuries. This builds on injury-specific models posited by others (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008; Peterson & Brown, 1994) that argued for the importance of expectancies and attributions, and also enhances these models by including more basic neurocognitive deficits that affect learning and judgments in the moment.

Findings should be considered with some caution, as direct measures of supervision and childhood injuries were not used. Prospective longitudinal research linking SIP directly to supervision behaviors and injuries would strengthen interpretations. Our sample was entirely disadvantaged urban mothers with children in a particular age group (3–5-year-olds), and along with collecting injury data, a larger sample that is more diverse in social class, context, and child age is needed to assess the broader worth of this risk model and the validity of injury prevention beliefs. For example, adaptive injury prevention beliefs may be more relevant to injury risk in lower-risk populations. Validation of the IAQ with regard to injury is especially needed.

Although we chose in this study to focus on parental determinants of injury risk, parental supervision is just one of many factors that explain childhood injury, and future work should explore how SIP factors may interact with child and contextual factors to affect injury risk. For example, parental SIP problems may be transmitted across generations through both genetic and socialization mechanisms (Friedman et al., 2008; MacBrayer, Milich, & Hundley, 2003), and children’s own SIP deficits may increase their risk for injuries (Barton & Morrongiello, 2011). Environmental hazards are associated with parents’ supervision beliefs (Damashek, Borduin, et al., 2014), and SIP factors may affect how parents perceive or respond to hazards in the environment. Future studies might consider more complex pathways and interactions among parent, child, and contextual factors.

Cognitive techniques have been successfully used as adjuncts to behavioral skills training to reduce maltreatment, and following more validation work, the addition of cognitive strategies targeting specific beliefs (e.g., distorted views on the value of injuries and their attributions to fate) may be useful adjuncts to behavioral injury prevention interventions (Edwards & Lutzker, 2008). Specific efforts to change parents’ attributions regarding child negative intent and cognitive restructuring of expectations and schema about injuries may improve parental motivation and increase the likelihood of behavioral changes (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2008). In addition, given the role of EF in monitoring (Azar & Weinzierl, 2005; Johnston et al., 2012), efforts targeting components of EF (e.g., attentional processes, working memory, set shifting) are particularly worth of exploration.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Philadelphia Department of Human Services and the community agencies that made this research possible. We would also like to thank the editor and reviewers of our manuscript for their thoughtful comments.

Funding

This work was supported by funding from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (#5R01HD53713), the PSU Child, Youth and Family Consortium and Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

Conflicts of interest: None declared.

References

Azar
S. T.
(
1986
).
A framework for understanding child maltreatment: An integration of cognitive behavioural and developmental perspectives
.
Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement
,
18
,
340
355
.

Azar
S. T.
,
Reitz
E. B.
,
Goslin
M. C.
(
2008
).
Mothering: Thinking is part of the job description: Application of cognitive views to understanding maladaptive parenting and doing intervention and prevention work
.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
,
29
,
295
304
.

Azar
S. T.
,
Robinson
D. R.
,
Hekimian
E.
,
Twentyman
C. T.
(
1984
).
Unrealistic expectations and problem-solving ability in maltreating and comparison mothers
.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
,
52
,
687
691
.

Azar
S. T.
,
Rohrbeck
C. A.
(
1986
).
Child abuse and unrealistic expectations: Further validation of the Parent Opinion Questionnaire
.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
,
54
,
867
868
. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.54.6.867

Azar
S. T.
,
Stevenson
M. T.
,
Johnson
D. R.
(
2012
).
Intellectual disabilities and neglectful parenting: Preliminary findings on the role of cognition in parenting risk
.
Journal of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities
,
5
,
94
129
.

Azar
S. T.
,
Weinzierl
K. M.
(
2005
).
Child maltreatment and childhood injury research: A cognitive behavioral approach
.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology
,
30
,
598
614
. http://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsi046

Barrett
J.
,
Fleming
A. S.
(
2011
).
Annual research review: All mothers are not created equal: Neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences
.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
,
52
,
368
397
.

Barton
B. K.
,
Morrongiello
B. A.
(
2011
).
Examining the impact of traffic environment and executive functioning on children’s pedestrian behaviors
.
Developmental Psychology
,
47
,
182
191
. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0021308

Bugental
D. B.
,
Happaney
K.
(
2004
).
Predicting infant maltreatment in low-income families: The interactive effects of maternal attributions and child status at birth
.
Developmental Psychology
,
40
,
234
243
. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.234

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC)
. (
2013
). Web-based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS). National Center for Injury Prevention and Control (producer). www.cdc.gov/ncipc/wisqars. Accessed September 3, 2015.

Coohey
C.
(
2003
).
Defining and classifying supervisory neglect
.
Child Maltreatment
,
8
,
145
156
. http://doi.org/10.1177/1077559502250786

Corso
P.
,
Finkelstein
E.
,
Miller
T.
,
Fiebelkorn
I.
,
Zaloshnja
E.
(
2006
).
Incidence and lifetime costs of injuries in the United States
.
Injury Prevention
,
12
,
212
218
.

Crittenden
P. M.
(
1993
).
An information-processing perspective on the behavior of neglectful parents
.
Criminal Justice and Behavior
,
20
,
27
48
.

Damashek
A.
,
Borduin
C.
,
Ronis
S.
(
2014
).
The role of environmental hazard in mothers’ beliefs about appropriate supervision
.
Journal of Early Childhood Research
,
12
,
50
63
.

Damashek
A.
,
Drass
S.
,
Bonner
B. L.
(
2014
).
Child maltreatment fatalities related to inadequate caregiver supervision
.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence
,
29
,
1987
2001
.

Edwards
A.
,
Lutzker
J. R.
(
2008
).
Iterations of the SafeCare model: An evidence-based child maltreatment prevention program
.
Behavior Modification
,
32
,
736
756
.

Fontaine
D.
,
Nolin
P.
(
2012
).
Study of “hot” executive functions in a sample of parents who have been accused of physical abuse or neglect
.
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma
,
21
,
1
18
. http://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2012.632391

Friedman
N. P.
,
Miyake
A.
,
Young
S. E.
,
DeFries
J. C.
,
Corley
R. P.
,
Hewitt
J. K.
(
2008
).
Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin
.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
,
137
,
201
225
. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201

Gilhooly
K. J.
,
Fioratou
E.
,
Anthony
S. H.
,
Wynn
V.
(
2007
).
Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects
.
British Journal of Psychology
,
98
,
611
625
. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2007.tb00467.x

Guilford
J. P.
,
Christiansen
P. R.
,
Merrifield
P. R.
,
Wilson
R. C.
(
1978
).
Alternate uses: Manual of instructions and interpretations
.
Orange, CA
:
Seridan Psychological Services
.

Hansen
D. J.
,
Pallotta
G. M.
,
Tishelman
A. C.
,
Conaway
L. P.
,
MacMillan
V. M.
(
1989
).
Parental problem-solving skills and child behavior problems: A comparison of physically abusive, neglectful, clinic, and community families
.
Journal of Family Violence
,
4
,
353
368
. http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00978576

Haskett
M. E.
,
Scott
S.
,
Willoughby
M.
,
Ahern
L.
,
Nears
K.
(
2006
).
The Parent Opinion Questionnaire and Child Vignettes for use with abusive parents: Assessment of psychometric properties
.
Journal of Family Violence
,
21
,
137
151
.

Haskett
M. E.
,
Smith Scott
S.
,
Grant
R.
,
Ward
C. S.
,
Robinson
C.
(
2003
).
Child-related cognitions and affective functioning of physically abusive and comparison parents
.
Child Abuse and Neglect
,
27
,
663
686
. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(03)00103-0

Haynes
R.
,
Reading
R.
,
Gale
S.
(
2003
).
Household and neighbourhood risks for injury to 5–14 year old children
.
Social Science & Medicine
,
57
,
625
636
. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00446-X

Heaton
R. K.
(
1981
).
The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual
.
Odessa
:
Psychological Assessment Resources Inc
.

Johnston
C.
,
Mash
E. J.
,
Miller
N.
,
Ninowski
J. E.
(
2012
).
Parenting in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
.
Clinical Psychology Review
,
32
,
215
228
. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.007

Laflamme
L.
,
Hasselberg
M.
,
Burrows
S.
,
Laflamme
L.
,
Hasselberg
M.
,
Burrows
S.
(
2010
).
20 years of research on socioeconomic inequality and children’s—Unintentional injuries understanding the cause-specific evidence at hand
.
International Journal of Pediatrics
,
e819687
.

Landen
M. G.
,
Bauer
U.
,
Kohn
M.
(
2003
).
Inadequate supervision as a cause of injury deaths among young children in Alaska and Louisiana
.
Pediatrics
,
111
,
328
331
. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.111.2.328

Leung
D. W.
,
Slep
A. M. S.
(
2006
).
Predicting inept discipline: The role of parental depressive symptoms, anger, and attributions
.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
,
74
,
524
534
. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.524

Lewis
T.
,
DiLillo
D.
,
Peterson
L.
(
2004
).
Parental beliefs regarding developmental benefits of childhood injuries
.
American Journal of Health Behavior
,
28
,
S61
S68
.

MacBrayer
E. K.
,
Milich
R.
,
Hundley
M.
(
2003
).
Attributional biases in aggressive children and their mothers
.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
112
,
698
708
. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.598

Milner
J. S.
(
2003
).
Social information processing in high-risk and physically abusive parents
.
Child Abuse and Neglect
,
27
,
7
20
. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00506-9

Morrongiello
B. A.
(
2005
).
Caregiver supervision and child-injury risk: I. Issues in defining and measuring supervision; II. Findings and directions for future research
.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology
,
30
,
536
552
.

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Corbett
M.
(
2006
).
The Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire: A measure of supervision relevant to children’s risk of unintentional injury
.
Injury Prevention
,
12
,
19
23
. http://doi.org/10.1136/ip.2005.008862

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Corbett
M. R.
(
2008
).
Elaborating a conceptual model of young children’s risk of unintentional injury and implications for prevention strategies
.
Health Psychology Review
,
2
,
191
205
. http://doi.org/10.1080/17437190902777594

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Dawber
T.
(
1999
).
Parental influences on toddlers’ injury-risk behaviors: Are sons and daughters socialized differently?
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
,
20
,
227
251
. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(99)00015-5

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Dayler
L.
(
1996
).
A community-based study of parents’ knowledge, attitudes and beliefs related to childhood injuries
.
Canadian Journal of Public Health/Revue Canadienne De Sante’e Publique
,
87
,
383
388
.

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
House
K.
(
2004
).
Measuring parent attributes and supervision behaviors relevant to child injury risk: Examining the usefulness of questionnaire measures
.
Injury Prevention
,
10
,
114
118
. http://doi.org/10.1136/ip.2003.003459

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Midgett
C.
,
Shields
R.
(
2001
).
Don’t run with scissors: Young children’s knowledge of home safety rules
.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology
,
26
,
105
115
. http://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/26.2.105

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Schell
S. L.
(
2010
).
Child injury: The role of supervision in prevention
.
American Journal of Lifestyle Medicine
,
4
,
65
74
.

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Schwebel
D. C.
(
2008
).
Gaps in childhood injury research and prevention: What can developmental scientists contribute?
Child Development Perspectives
,
2
,
78
84
. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00046.x

Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Walpole
B.
,
McArthur
B. A.
(
2009
).
Brief report: Young children’s risk of unintentional injury: A comparison of mothers’ and fathers’ supervision beliefs and reported practices
.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology
,
34
,
1063
1068
.

Nayak
M. B.
,
Milner
J. S.
(
1998
).
Neuropsychological functioning: Comparison of mothers at high- and low-risk for child physical abuse
.
Child Abuse and Neglect
,
22
,
687
703
.

Nelson
K. E.
,
Saunders
E. J.
,
Landsman
M. J.
(
1993
).
Chronic child neglect in perspective
.
Social Work
,
38
,
661
671
. http://doi.org/10.1093/sw/38.6.661

Ondersma
S. J.
,
Chaffin
M. J.
,
Mullins
S. M.
,
LeBreton
J. M.
(
2005
).
A brief form of the Child Abuse Potential Inventory: Development and validation
.
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology
,
34
,
301
311
. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3402_9

Pears
K. C.
,
Kim
H. K.
,
Fisher
P. A.
(
2008
).
Psychosocial and cognitive functioning of children with specific profiles of maltreatment
.
Child Abuse and Neglect
,
32
,
958
971
.

Peterson
L.
,
Bartelstone
J.
,
Kern
T.
,
Gillies
R.
(
1995
).
Parents’ socialization of children’s injury prevention: Description and some initial parameters
.
Child Development
,
66
,
224
235
. http://doi.org/10.2307/1131202

Peterson
L.
,
Brown
D.
(
1994
).
Integrating child injury and abuse-neglect research: Common histories, etiologies, and solutions
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
116
,
293
315
.

Peterson
L.
,
Mori
L.
,
Scissors
C.
(
1986
).
Mom or dad says I shouldn’t: Supervised and unsupervised children’s knowledge of their parents’ rules for home safety
.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology
,
11
,
177
188
. http://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/11.2.177

Plotkin
R. C.
(
1983
). Cognitive mediation in disciplinary actions among mothers who have abused or neglected their children: Dispositional and environmental factors (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester.

Ruiz-Casares
M.
,
Trocmé
N.
,
Fallon
B.
(
2012
).
Supervisory neglect and risk of harm. Evidence from the Canadian Child Welfare System
.
Child Abuse and Neglect
,
36
,
471
480
. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.03.005

Saluja
G.
,
Brenner
R.
,
Morrongiello
B. A.
,
Haynie
D.
,
Rivera
M.
,
Cheng
T. L.
(
2004
).
The role of supervision in child injury risk: Definition, conceptual and measurement issues
.
Injury Control and Safety Promotion
,
11
,
17
22
.

Sattler
J. M.
,
Ryan
J. J.
(
2009
).
Assessment with the WAIS-IV
. San Diego: Jerome M Sattler, Publisher, Inc.

Schwebel
D. C.
,
Bounds
M. L.
(
2003
).
The role of parents and temperament on children’s estimation of physical ability: Links to unintentional injury prevention
.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology
,
28
,
505
516
. http://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsg041

Slack
K. S.
,
Berger
L. M.
,
DuMont
K.
,
Yang
M. Y.
,
Kim
B.
,
Ehrhard-Dietzel
S.
,
Holl
J. L.
(
2011
).
Risk and protective factors for child neglect during early childhood: A cross-study comparison
.
Children and Youth Services Review
,
33
,
1354
1363
.

Simpson
J. C.
,
Turnbull
B. L.
,
Ardagh
M.
,
Richardson
S.
(
2009
).
Child home injury prevention: Understanding the context of unintentional injuries to preschool children
.
International Journal of Injury Control and Safety Promotion
,
16
,
159
167
.

Wasik
B. H.
,
Bryant
D. M.
(
1994
).
Three parent and adult problem-solving instruments
.
Chapel Hill, NC
:
University of North Carolina
. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED379539

Wechsler
D.
(
2008
).
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition
.
San Antonio, TX
:
Pearson
.

Whitehead
E.
,
Owens
D.
(
2012
). Parental perceptions of unintentional injury risks to children.
International Journal of Health Promotion and Education
,
50
,
20
27
. http://doi.org/10.1080/14635240.2012.661953