Abstract

Drawing on theories of digital media (non-)use and well-being, this study examines how voluntary disconnection relates to subjective well-being and what role digital skills play in this relationship. We rely on mobile experience sampling methods to link nuanced disconnection practices throughout the day (e.g., putting screen devices away and muting notifications) with momentary experiences of well-being. We collected 4,028 responses from 105 mobile media users over the course of one week. Multilevel regression analyses revealed neither significant within-person effects of disconnection on affective well-being, social connectedness, or life satisfaction, nor a significant moderation effect of digital skills. Exploratory analyses, however, show that effects of disconnection on well-being vary greatly across participants, and that effects are dependent on whether one disconnects in the physical copresence of others. Our study offers a refined perspective on the consequences, or lack thereof, of deliberate non-use of technology in the digital age.

Lay Summary

This study looked at whether taking breaks from digital media throughout the day has an impact on people’s own perceptions of their well-being. Over the course of one week, the researchers asked 105 participants to answer six questionnaires each day through a mobile application, which resulted in 4,028 filled-out questionnaires. The results show that, on average, taking a break from digital media does not affect how people feel (positively or negatively), or how socially connected they feel immediately after their disconnection. It also does not affect how satisfied they are with their life considering the past day. However, upon further analysis, the results show that people’s reactions to breaks from technology vary greatly. Some people experience no or negative effects of disconnection on their well-being, while others experience positive effects. The study also finds that when people take a break from digital media while being with others, this has short-term positive effects on their well-being. With this, the study shows a nuanced picture of the benefits—or lack of these—of what taking breaks from digital media can do for people’s well-being.

In today’s “permanently online, permanently connected” world (Vorderer et al., 2017), people increasingly make purposeful efforts to disconnect, such as by limiting their connectivity or time spent on digital media (Nguyen et al., 2022). For instance, individuals might take “digital detoxes” and use tech-based solutions such as apps and device features to unplug (Nguyen, 2021). Societal norms toward disconnection might also develop, as social groups and organizations implement guidelines and policies around digital media use (Vanden Abeele & Nguyen, 2023). Research reveals that well-being and health considerations are one of the central reasons why people decide to place limits on their digital media use or to “disconnect” (Jorge, 2019; Nguyen, 2023). Yet, in current scholarship on digital media use and its impact on subjective well-being, the role of everyday digital media practices that are geared toward limiting connectivity remains understudied. Exceptions to this are intervention studies where people are asked to take breaks from social media or mobile devices, but these do not reflect people’s everyday lived experiences (e.g., Hall et al., 2021; for a systematic review, see Radtke et al., 2021). As such, research has not yet been able to address to what extent disconnection is able to restore people’s sense of well-being, which is perceived to be threatened by digital media use (Nguyen, 2023). Note that ample research has been conducted on the relationship between digital media use and subjective well-being, finding mostly none to small effects (for a review, see Meier & Reinecke, 2020; for a discussion, see Orben, 2020), but individual perceptions that digital media affects well-being negatively still remain (e.g., Comparis, 2018; Kantar, 2018). Since well-being considerations are one of the key motivations to disconnect (Nguyen, 2023), it is important to examine if disconnection—that is, deliberately limiting the use of digital media—is an effective strategy to restore such perceptions of reduced well-being.

Another important question is to what extent digital inequalities shape the relationship between digital disconnection and subjective well-being. In recent years, scholars have voiced concerns about how digital inequalities may extend to the realm of digital disconnection, putting forward the notion that some might be better able to manage the abundant digital information and communication environment than others (e.g., Gui & Büchi, 2021; Hargittai & Micheli, 2019; Nguyen, 2021; Nguyen & Hargittai, 2023). In a world where connection is the default, disconnection may be reserved for the more digitally and socioeconomically privileged, who are able to “afford” going offline (Beattie & Cassidy, 2020; Büchi et al., 2019). With many disconnective features being built into device and app interfaces (e.g., Apple Screen Time, “Focus” mode), it is likely that these will be more accessible to those who are digitally skilled enough to utilize them (Nguyen, 2021). Overall, digital inequality research has shown that more skilled digital media users are more likely to experience benefits from their digital media uses, for instance, in terms of increased social capital or well-being (Hofer et al., 2019; Nguyen et al., 2020). As such, an important question is whether and how digital skills play a moderating role in the relationship between everyday digital disconnection practices and subjective well-being.

In this preregistered study, we examine the implications of people’s everyday disconnection practices for their sense of well-being (i.e., affective, social, and cognitive well-being), and how digital skills play a role in this. We aim to explore people’s broader digital media repertoire instead of only focusing on specific devices or platforms, as digital media use and henceforth disconnection likely does not happen in an isolated context (e.g., Nguyen et al., 2021). Regarding digital disconnection, we aim to capture the everyday practices in which people engage while maintaining connectivity—but without going completely offline for longer periods of time, such as in the case of digital detoxes where users take longer breaks (e.g., weeks and months) from mobile or social media (Baym et al., 2020; Franks et al., 2018; Radtke et al., 2021). To capture these everyday disconnection practices in their natural setting—as opposed to intervention studies that “force” participants to disconnect—this study employs an experience sampling method (ESM) design where digital media users are surveyed six times throughout the day over the course of a week to capture their possible disconnection practices as they occur naturally. The unique advantage of ESM in the context of this study is that it can capture (a) the nuanced disconnection practices in which people engage throughout the day (e.g., putting the phone with the screen down for several minutes; making a mental note not to visit certain apps or websites) and (b) the momentary, likely short-lived, effects of such nuanced disconnection practices on subjective well-being.

Theorizing the relationship between disconnection and subjective well-being

Digital media play an essential role in organizing everyday life, maintaining social relationships, and fulfilling leisure time, and as such may be important for nourishing affective, cognitive, and social well-being outcomes (Meier & Reinecke, 2020). It is not surprising that decades of research have been concerned with the question of how digital media use affects people’s subjective well-being (for a review, see Meier & Reinecke, 2020). With respect to the impact of digital disconnection on various dimensions of subjective well-being, there are several hypotheses to consider. As a starting point, we first discuss existing theories about the relationship between digital media use and well-being, and then elaborate on what this would mean for potential effects of disconnection on different well-being outcomes. In this study, we define digital disconnection as nuanced acts of deliberately limiting one’s digital media use (e.g., putting devices away, making mental notes not to look at devices, turning off notifications or the internet). Overall, it is important to note that digital disconnection and its potential consequences for well-being might differ depending on geographic and cultural context (Bozan & Treré, 2023; Treré, 2021). Much of the disconnection literature so far (for a review, see Nassen et al., 2023), as this study, has been focused on Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic countries (Henrich et al., 2010), where dis/connectivity for many is likely a choice rather than a necessity (also see the Discussion and Conclusion section).

On the one hand, scholarship has theorized digital media use to have a positive effect on well-being. For instance, digital media such as smartphones can help young people develop a sense of autonomy (Schnauber-Stockmann et al., 2021) and can support older adults in living an autonomous life (Abascal & Civit, 2000), which are fundamental human needs that contribute to overall well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2001). With respect to digital communication, researchers have suggested that both the quantity and quality of human communication can be improved through use of technology, which in turn contributes to greater social connection and overall well-being (Dienlin et al., 2017; Valkenburg & Peter, 2007). If we think of digital media use, that is, connection, as having positive effects on subjective well-being, then one hypothesis could be that disconnection interferes with this relationship and thus negatively impacts well-being.

On the other hand, digital media use may also have negative consequences for subjective well-being. For instance, a well-known hypothesis is that of “displacement” (Dienlin et al., 2017; Kraut et al., 1998; Valkenburg & Peter, 2007), whereby time spent on digital media detracts from time that could be spent on other potentially more meaningful activities, including in-person social ones. For instance, in the case of problematic social media use—when people feel a loss of control over their social media use or experience “overconnection”—research has shown that it can lead to increased loneliness (Marttila et al., 2021) and is thus detrimental to one’s well-being. Digital media have also been experienced as distracting, leading to unwanted effects such as procrastination (Hinsch & Sheldon, 2013; Meier, 2021). Disconnection, then, could be a solution to such perceived negative effects and allow people to spend their free time on other activities that are perceived to be more meaningful, all the while regaining a sense of control over their digital media use (Aranda & Baig, 2018; Franks et al., 2018; Nguyen, 2021).

So far, in theorizing the relationship between digital disconnection and subjective well-being, we have given examples in which we think of disconnection as the reverse of connection, and likewise the effects it may have on subjective well-being. While this might seem logical and straightforward, we argue that effects of digital disconnection on well-being are not necessarily the opposite of those of digital media use. Unlike digital media use, which is often described as automatic, habitual (Bayer et al., 2022; Giannakos et al., 2013; Wohn, 2012) and perhaps even as mindless (Baym et al., 2020; Schellewald, 2021), when it comes to disconnection practices, people engage in them more consciously and in a goal-oriented manner (Aranda & Baig, 2018; Franks et al., 2018; Nguyen, 2021). Through disconnecting, people often hope to achieve something specific, such as being more present in offline activities, not being distracted when wanting to concentrate on an activity, or escaping the social pressure to be online (Nguyen, 2023). These observations are in line with Communication Bond Belong theory, which posits that the motivation to engage in social interactions decreases when the need to belong is fulfilled, and vice versa, because social interactions come at the expense of social energy, which people need to preserve (Hall & Davis, 2017). Recently, scholars have linked this theory to the concept of digital solitude (Campbell & Ross, 2022), suggesting that when people actively decide to disconnect, this can help them regain their social energy (Ross et al., 2023). From the various points of view discussed, it would make sense to expect that, overall, people’s disconnection practices have a positive impact on well-being in general. Nonetheless, it is important to note that digital media use itself (rather than disconnection) can also be practiced as a moment of digital solitude, for instance when someone uses social media by oneself for a moment of relaxation or to cope with offline stress (Keessen, 2023; Wolfers & Utz, 2022), and so the relationship of disconnection to well-being may not be that straightforward.

What does existing empirical scholarship on digital disconnection have to say about its effects on subjective well-being? Research on the relationship between disconnection and subjective well-being has so far mostly been of experimental nature, where participants are instructed to abstain from social media or their smartphones for a certain time period. Overall, the results of such studies are mixed: Some find positive effects on affective, cognitive, and social well-being outcomes, while others find no such effects, or even negative ones (for a systematic review, see Radtke et al., 2021). In one of the most comprehensive studies to date, Hall et al. (2021) randomly assigned participants to one of five experimental conditions in which they were either asked to make no changes to their social media use or take a break from social media ranging from 1 to 4 weeks. Relying on daily diary assessments, the authors found no effect of abstention nor the duration of abstinence on affective, social, or cognitive dimensions of subjective well-being.

Aside from intervention studies, there is also research that relies on participants’ recall of their disconnection experiences. For instance, in a study where researchers collected open survey responses, Facebook users reported that after a time of deactivation, they returned to be more mindful users of the platform (Baym et al., 2020). Cross-sectional survey research has shown that intended unavailability to communicate with others (either in person or digitally) can increase overall well-being, but only for users who are more digitally connected than others (Ross et al., 2023). An interview study found that taking breaks from social media could benefit people’s sense of health and well-being, although this was not the case for all interviewed participants (Nguyen, 2021). On the whole, while experimental studies show mixed effects of forced disconnection from social media/mobile phones on subjective well-being (e.g., Radtke et al., 2021), interview studies that ask about voluntary disconnection experiences in retrospect report overall benefits for perceived well-being (Aranda & Baig, 2018; Baym et al., 2020; Franks et al., 2018; Nguyen et al., 2020). This leaves the question of whether method of analysis may influence how disconnection and well-being are linked.

Observational studies that focus on the effects of disconnection strategies implemented in day-to-day life on subjective well-being (unlike forced “detoxes” where people opt out of social media for a certain time period due to researcher prompting) are less present in the literature. Yet, such everyday disconnection practices are quite common and, as such, are ripe for investigation. For instance, research from Switzerland indicates that two-thirds of digital media users engage in at least one strategy to disconnect (e.g., turning off notifications, using do-not-disturb-functions, or putting digital devices away), with health and well-being among the most prominent reasons listed for doing so (Nguyen et al., 2022). In an Austrian study of people ages 18–35, almost half used screen time apps on their smartphone (Schmuck, 2020). Moreover, among people who do not use screen time apps, social network site use was positively associated with problematic smartphone use, and consequently, lower subjective well-being, while there was no such link among screen time app users (Schmuck, 2020). As such, it could be that engaging in efforts to regulate one’s media use can mitigate potential negative effects of digital media use on subjective well-being. While these studies are helpful as a first step, more research is needed to deepen our understanding of the possible effectiveness of everyday disconnection practices for subjective well-being.

Momentary effects of disconnection on subjective well-being

Given that people often engage in disconnection practices consciously and with a specific goal in mind (e.g., being more present in offline activities; Aranda & Baig, 2018; Franks et al., 2018; Nguyen, 2021), we expect that the effects on affective and cognitive well-being overall will be positive. However, when it comes to social well-being, existing empirical work seems to suggest that in the short-term, disconnection can lead to diminished feelings of connectedness. Nguyen (2021) found this to be the case in an interview study with 30 adults who had taken a break from social media at least once. People’s overall, long-term reflections were positive and reflected increased well-being, whereas short-term, they had reported negative effects on social well-being, such as fear of missing out and restlessness when they decided to deactivate Facebook or put their mobile phone away (Nguyen, 2021).

When comparing experimental studies in which people were asked to disconnect from social media, a one-week intervention yielded an increase in social connectedness (Brown & Kuss, 2020), while another study with a shorter two-day intervention led to lower connectedness (Sheldon et al., 2011), although the latter finding likely does not reflect momentary effects. A comprehensive study which compared different lengths of social media breaks (ranging from 0 to 4 weeks) found no effects of the duration of abstinence on loneliness (Hall et al., 2021). Research using the ESM to examine communication between close ties (e.g., couples) has shown that connected availability—the perception that a partner is continuously available digitally—benefits subjective well-being (Taylor & Bazarova, 2021). Theoretically, disconnection then could be detrimental to feelings of social connectedness—at least in the short term, as it interferes with this state of “connected presence” (Christensen, 2009; Licoppe, 2004) to others at all times. In the context of in-person social interactions, however, phubbing—interrupting a social situation through smartphone use—can lead to detrimental well-being (Nuñez & Radtke, 2023). Disconnection, then, could increase especially perceived social well-being. As an observational study showed, people experienced more intimate conversations when smartphones were used less (Vanden Abeele et al., 2019).

This study aims to examine the momentary effects of people’s disconnection practices throughout the day on three dimensions of subjective well-being: affective (i.e., positive/negative mood), cognitive (i.e., life satisfaction), and social (i.e., social connectedness), as these can be considered the core elements of general well-being (Andrews & McKennell, 1980; Diener et al., 2009). In the case of such everyday disconnection practices (e.g., putting the phone away or turning off the internet for a short time period), effects on subjective well-being indicators may be short-lived and limited to how one feels in the moment, thus requiring momentary assessment methods to capture. Our aim is to include people’s broader digital media repertoire, thus focusing on devices that grant access to the internet, such as a smartphone, laptop/computer, or tablet, and the apps and programs that one can use through them. Inspired by current theorizing and empirical work in the area of digital media use, digital disconnection, and well-being, we expect that engaging in disconnection practices will benefit affective and cognitive well-being in particular. However, for social well-being, we expect negative effects in the short-term. As such, we formulate the following hypotheses:1

H1: Everyday disconnection practices are related to higher perceived affective well-being.

H2: Everyday disconnection practices are related to higher perceived cognitive well-being.

H3: Everyday disconnection practices are related to lower perceived social well-being.

Digital inequalities in disconnection: the role of digital skills

Scholars have suggested that socio-digital inequalities exist around people’s disconnection practices and have highlighted the importance of digital skills—that is, the ability to use technology effectively and efficiently (Hargittai & Hsieh, 2012)—in managing the abundant information and communication that people are exposed to through digital media (Gui et al., 2017; Hargittai et al., 2012; Hargittai & Micheli, 2019). A survey of American internet users revealed that people with greater digital skills were more likely to use strategies to limit their digital media use during the COVID-19 pandemic (Nguyen & Hargittai, 2023). Qualitative interview studies have also reported that digital skills are important for being able to use features of technology to limit connectivity (Nguyen, 2021; Nguyen et al., 2021). Examples of such strategies include the use of screen time apps to track and set limits to one’s digital media use, as well as the use of app and device settings to manage one’s online availability or exposure to online information and communication. Overall, since evidence of an association between digital skills and disconnection behaviors is still scarce, a first question that we will explore in this article is whether people’s digital skills predict the extent to which they engage in nuanced disconnection practices throughout the day.

While previous research has established that digital inequalities in people’s disconnection behaviors exist, the extended question of whether this results in differentiated benefits derived from such deliberate non-use remains unanswered to date. This focus on disparities in the benefits that people are able to derive from technology use is an important aspect of digital inequality (Hargittai, 2008): Digital inequality research suggests that in order for people to reap the benefits from using digital media, people need to possess the skills needed to capitalize on such use (DiMaggio et al., 2004). For instance, research on digital media use and social and cognitive well-being in older adults reveals a positive relationship, which was stronger for those with greater digital skills (Hofer et al., 2019; Nguyen et al., 2020), indicating that digital skills are an important moderator of media effects. This suggests that people with adequate digital skills can mobilize technology use for greater benefits—meaning that more digitally privileged people might disproportionally profit from engaging in digital activities. Given the importance of digital skills for engaging in disconnection practices (Nguyen & Hargittai, 2023), the question of whether such differences also extend to the benefits that people potentially derive from such disconnection is a highly relevant one. In this study, we also explore the role of digital skills for the benefits that people reap from digital disconnection in terms of their subjective well-being. Here, we ask the following research questions:

RQ1: Do digital skills predict people’s engagement in disconnection practices?2

RQ2: Do digital skills moderate the relationships between digital disconnection and (a) affective, (b) cognitive, and (c) social well-being?

Methods

In this preregistered study, we use ESM (for more information about this method, see Myin-Germeys & Kuppens, 2022) to examine the effects of everyday disconnection practices on three different dimensions of subjective well-being: affective, cognitive, and social. We also examine how these relationships are dependent on people’s digital skills. ESM is an appropriate method to examine the occurrence of everyday disconnection practices that happen throughout the day, as well as the immediate, momentary, short-lived effects that it may have on subjective well-being. The hypotheses, research questions, design, and analytical plan were preregistered (https://osf.io/6kw9c).

Design and sampling plan

We recruited Dutch participants 18 years and older through a market research company in March and April 2022. We aimed for a somewhat equal distribution of gender, age, and education levels, which the research company took into account in sending out invitations. Prospective participants received an invitation e-mail from the research company with the study information letter and a link to the informed consent form and intake questionnaire. In the study information letter, we explained the study procedure and that people needed to possess an Android smartphone and install the mobile experience sampling app (movisensXS) to participate. On day 1, participants received an intake questionnaire to measure sociodemographics and general digital media use. Through this intake survey, participants also received step-by-step instructions to install the movisensXS app. We also gaveinstructions to change device settings to receive questionnaire notifications from the app, even in “do-not-disturb” mode (this is a default option of the app, which is needed so that it can function properly).

On days 2–8 (i.e., 7 consecutive days), participants received six questionnaires each day through movisensXS at semi-random time intervals between 8:30 am and 9:00 pm (see exact sampling schedule in preregistration). Given that our questions (see “Daily ESM Questionnaire”) asked about the preceding 2-hr time window for several questions, we ensured that all questionnaire prompts were at least 2hr apart to avoid overlap in the 2-hr time windows. In total, participants received 42 questionnaires through the app. For most participants (99 out of 105) we needed to extend the ESM part of the study (by 1day n =64; 2days n =12; 3days n =10; 4days n =6; 5days n =5; 6days n =2), either because they had missed questionnaires on the first days due to unforeseen technical issues or because of personal reasons such as illness (see under the Analyses section that the day of measurement did not influence the results). For those willing to continue their participation in the study in spite of this, we sampled until they filled out at least 33 questionnaires (which was one of our inclusion criteria in the preregistration based on power calculations). On the 9th day, participants received a final outtake questionnaire (these data are not part of this article). Participants received a financial compensation of 25 Euros upon completion of the study if they filled out the intake and final questionnaire, and if they filled out at least 80% of the ESM questionnaires (i.e., a minimum of 33), with at least one each day.

Participants were instructed to fill out the questionnaire as soon as possible after receiving a notification, but it stayed available for 30min after the first prompt. Alternatively, participants could delay the notification up until 25min later, or they could skip the questionnaire. Participants received four reminder notifications if they had not filled out the questionnaire in those 30min. Given this study’s focus on disconnection practices, it is important to note that the mobile experience app also worked offline (i.e., when there was no internet connection). The app would then upload the results to the server once the participant again had an active internet connection.

The irony of using mobile media to study disconnection practices is not lost on the authors. We nonetheless believe that this is still an appropriate method to study this behavior, as we do not expect to include participants who disconnect from their phone for longer periods of time (i.e., days, weeks). Instead, we focus on the brief, nuanced disconnection practices that active digital media users engage in throughout the day. Extended disconnection is quite rare, and likely limited to a select group of people who are extremely serious about disconnecting (Nguyen, 2021).

Participants

In total, 151 people signed through the experience sampling app, of which n =14 never filled out any questionnaires, and n =32 ended their participation early. Our final sample consisted of 105 participants, of which two-thirds were female (68.6%). The average age of participants was 40.10years (SD =13.10) and ranged from 19 to 75years. The sample included both lower educated (50.5%) and higher educated participants who had earned at least a Bachelor’s degree (49.5%).

In total, we sent 5055 ESM questionnaires to our 105 participants, of which they filled out 4028 (79.7%). On average, participants completed 38.4 ESM questionnaires (range 32–42). Note that we had an inclusion criterion of filling out at least 33 ESM questionnaires to be included in the final sample of our study (for more detailed information on the rationale behind this decision, see our preregistration). However, as two participants started, but did not finish the 33rd questionnaire, we ended up including all participants who completed 32 questionnaires.

Baseline questionnaire

Sociodemographics

We measured age by asking for people’s birth year and subtracting that from 2022. We asked about gender (female, male, other), which we recoded into a female category (vs. other). We measured education level through nine categories, ranging from no formal education to having obtained an academic degree, which we recoded into a dichotomous variable reflecting higher education level (i.e., university versus all lower education categories).

General digital media use

We asked about people’s use of the internet and their smartphones separately, distinguishing between weekdays and the weekend, which resulted in four items. The question was: “On an average [weekday/Saturday or Sunday], how often do you use [the internet, either on a computer, tablet, or smartphone/your smartphone]?”. We calculated the average use per day by taking the sum of the weekday answer * 5days and the weekend answer * 2days, and dividing this by 7days, resulting in two variables separately reflecting average internet use and smartphone use per day. On average, participants used the internet for 4.20hr per day (SD =1.43), and their smartphone for 3.35hr per day (SD =1.61).

Digital skills

For digital skills, we used an established and validated index to measure people’s know-how about the internet and social media (Hargittai, 2020; Hargittai & Hsieh, 2012). This self-reported index has partly been validated against people’s actual performance of skills (Hargittai, 2005). Respondents reported their understanding of 12 internet and social media-related items on a 1–5 point scale ranging from no understanding to full understanding (e.g., “PDF,” “cache,” “followers,” “tagging”). We took the average of the items as the digital skills score (Cronbach’s α =0.87). The average level of digital skills was relatively high, but people with lower levels of skills were also represented in the sample (M =3.88, SD =0.78, range 1–5).

Daily ESM questionnaire

Disconnection

We pretested the question and answer options for measuring disconnection (n =9) for clarity, comprehensibility, and completeness during a pilot of the study procedure and made several adjustments before finalizing the version presented here. To measure disconnection practices in everyday life, we asked people: “In the past two hours, have you done any of the following deliberately to take a break from digital media? Check all that apply.” We communicated to participants that digital media entailed “devices that grant access to the internet, such as a smartphone, laptop/computer, or tablet.” The nine answer options were: (a) put my mobile phone or other digital media away; (b) put my mobile phone or other digital media with the screen facing down; (c) told myself not to look at my mobile phone or other digital media; (d) told myself not to look at certain websites or apps (for instance, email, news, and social media); (e) closed programs, apps, and websites (for instance, email, news, and social media); f) turned off notifications (for instance from email, news, and social media); (g) turned off the internet (for instance, flight mode, “do-not-disturb” function); (h) took a break from digital media in another way; and (i) limited digital media use without any special approach. At the end of the list, we included a 10th option for people to report that they had not disconnected in the past 2hr: “I have NOT taken a break from digital media in the past two hours.”

Confirmatory factor analyses (using the R package lavaan; Rosseel et al., 2023) relying on tetrachoric correlations suitable for dichotomous variables showed an acceptable model fit (χ2(27, N =3972) =308.92, P <.001, CFI=0.800, RMSEA=0.051; Hooper et al., 2008), justifying our decision to combine the nine items for measuring disconnection into one index. Thus, for our analyses, we created a dichotomous variable reflecting whether people had disconnected in the past 2hr (versus not). If people indicated that they had used one of the disconnection strategies while simultaneously reporting that they had not disconnected in the past two hours, we recoded their responses as missing since those answers are logically misaligned.

If people had indicated that they had disconnected, we also collected data on whom they were with when they disconnected. Answer options were: alone, partner, child(ren), other family, friend(s), colleague(s), and other; people could select multiple answer options. For the analyses, we created a dichotomous variable for each type of social tie. We also created another dichotomous variable to reflect whether people had disconnected in the copresence of other(s) (1) versus while alone (0).

Subjective well-being

We focused on three dimensions of subjective well-being, namely affective, cognitive, and social well-being. We measured affective and social well-being at every occasion, that is, six times per day. Given that we expected less variability during the day in cognitive well-being than other dimensions of subjective well-being (de Vries et al., 2021), we asked this only once: in the last questionnaire of each day. We used one-item measures for these constructs, which is common in experience sampling research to lower participant burden.

We conceptualized affective well-being as people’s experience of negative and positive emotions or mood. To capture affective well-being, we asked: “How do you feel at this moment?”, with answer options ranging from 1 “Very negative” to 7 “Very positive” (de Vries et al., 2021). We conceptualized cognitive well-being as one’s general satisfaction with life, and asked: “When you look at the past day, how satisfied are you with your life at this moment?” Participants could answer on a slider ranging from 1 “Very unsatisfied” to 100 “Very satisfied” (de Vries et al., 2021). We used a slider with a larger range, as this has been shown to be a reliable and valid method to assess constructs related to overall quality of life, such as life satisfaction (Benrud-Larson et al., 2005; de Boer et al., 2004). Finally, we conceptualized social well-being as people’s sense of social connectedness, where we asked: “How socially connected do you feel at this moment?,” with answer options ranging from 1 “Not at all” to 7 “Very much” (Arjmand et al., 2021).

Analyses

We first conducted exploratory analyses to look into the prevalence of disconnection practices across our sample, as well as how sociodemographics, digital experiences, and digital skills (RQ1) related to disconnection practices. Here, we used mixed effects logistic regression to model binary outcome variables, with a random intercept varying by participant. The outcome variables were disconnection behavior in the past two hours (yes/no), and whom they were with when they disconnected (dichotomous variable for each type of social company: alone, partner, child(ren), other family, friend(s), colleague(s), and other).

Next, we tested the hypotheses with multilevel linear regression modeling to account for the nested structure of the data. We estimated two-level models in which repeated measurements of digital disconnection (level 1) are nested within individuals (level 2). To test H1, H2, and H3, we estimated separate models for each dependent variable (affective, cognitive, and social well-being). For affective and social well-being as outcomes, which were measured at six times daily, we estimated a fixed-effects model with disconnection as predictor. For cognitive well-being, which was measured once per day, we estimated a fixed-effects model with the sum score of total disconnection during the day as predictor. We conducted additional exploratory analyses to examine if the within-person effects of digital disconnection on subjective well-being (H1–H3) differ across individuals. To do so, we extended each model with a random slope and tested if the model fit improved when doing so.

To answer RQ2, we extended each of the aforementioned models with an interaction term between digital skills and disconnection. We also conducted all analyses with age, gender, education level, and general digital media use as control variables. Since these were not significant, we report the analyses without them. We also considered the measurement day type (weekday versus weekend) and whether it was the 1st–6th questionnaire of the day, but given that these showed no effect on the dependent variables, we excluded them from further analyses. The time-level variables (level 1) were person-mean centered as we are interested in the within-subject effects. The individual-level variables of digital skills and general media use (level 2) were grand-mean centered. All analyses were conducted in R, using package lme4 (Bates et al., 2022) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2020).

Results

Disconnection practices (exploratory analyses and RQ1)

Table 1 displays the prevalence of disconnection practices. In 39.5% (n =1,567) of completed ESM questionnaires, participants indicated that they had used at least one of the listed disconnection strategies in the preceding two hours. Recall that the question asked whether participants had engaged in these actions “deliberately to take a break from digital media.” The most commonly used strategies were “Put my mobile phone or other digital media away” (23.4%, n =928), “Put my mobile phone or other digital media with the screen facing down” (7.4%, n =295), and “Limited digital media use without any special approach” (7.6%, n =303). Participants, when they had disconnected, used on average 1.36 strategies (SD =0.77) with a range of 1–8 strategies. Specifically, in 75.4% (n =1,182) of filled-out ESM questionnaires where people said they had disconnected, only one strategy was used, followed by 17.5% (n =274) with two strategies, and 4.9% (n =76) with three strategies.

Table 1.

Prevalence of disconnection practices during 7-day ESM period

%n
Any disconnection practice39.51,567
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media away23.4928
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media with the screen facing down7.4295
 Told myself not to look at my mobile phone or other digital media4.7187
 Told myself not to look on certain websites or apps (e.g., email, news, social media)1.353
 Closed programs, apps and websites (e.g., email, news, social media)1.559
 Turned off notifications (e.g., from email, news, and social media)2.7108
 Turned off the internet (e.g., flight mode, “do-not-disturb” function)1.976
 Took a break from digital media in another way3.0118
 Limited digital media use without any special approach7.6303
I have NOT taken a break from digital media in the past 2hr60.52,405
%n
Any disconnection practice39.51,567
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media away23.4928
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media with the screen facing down7.4295
 Told myself not to look at my mobile phone or other digital media4.7187
 Told myself not to look on certain websites or apps (e.g., email, news, social media)1.353
 Closed programs, apps and websites (e.g., email, news, social media)1.559
 Turned off notifications (e.g., from email, news, and social media)2.7108
 Turned off the internet (e.g., flight mode, “do-not-disturb” function)1.976
 Took a break from digital media in another way3.0118
 Limited digital media use without any special approach7.6303
I have NOT taken a break from digital media in the past 2hr60.52,405

Note: Data from 105 participants with 3,972 valid responses to the question about disconnection.

Table 1.

Prevalence of disconnection practices during 7-day ESM period

%n
Any disconnection practice39.51,567
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media away23.4928
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media with the screen facing down7.4295
 Told myself not to look at my mobile phone or other digital media4.7187
 Told myself not to look on certain websites or apps (e.g., email, news, social media)1.353
 Closed programs, apps and websites (e.g., email, news, social media)1.559
 Turned off notifications (e.g., from email, news, and social media)2.7108
 Turned off the internet (e.g., flight mode, “do-not-disturb” function)1.976
 Took a break from digital media in another way3.0118
 Limited digital media use without any special approach7.6303
I have NOT taken a break from digital media in the past 2hr60.52,405
%n
Any disconnection practice39.51,567
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media away23.4928
 Put my mobile phone or other digital media with the screen facing down7.4295
 Told myself not to look at my mobile phone or other digital media4.7187
 Told myself not to look on certain websites or apps (e.g., email, news, social media)1.353
 Closed programs, apps and websites (e.g., email, news, social media)1.559
 Turned off notifications (e.g., from email, news, and social media)2.7108
 Turned off the internet (e.g., flight mode, “do-not-disturb” function)1.976
 Took a break from digital media in another way3.0118
 Limited digital media use without any special approach7.6303
I have NOT taken a break from digital media in the past 2hr60.52,405

Note: Data from 105 participants with 3,972 valid responses to the question about disconnection.

Next, we looked into the role of sociodemographics and digital skills in disconnection behavior, and the social context of disconnecting (Table 2). Mixed-effects logistic regression analysis revealed that gender, age, and education level did not relate to disconnection behavior over the course of one week. Digital skills (RQ1) were also not related to people’s engagement in disconnection practices throughout the day. When looking into the social context in which people disconnected, we found significant age and gender patterns. Specifically, older age was related to greater odds of disconnecting around one’s partner. We found no significant relationships between sociodemographics and disconnecting around children. On the other hand, older age was related to lower odds of disconnecting around other family members (besides one’s partner and children), friends, and colleagues—suggesting that younger people are more likely to do so in these social contexts. Female participants also had lower odds of disconnecting while being with friends, as compared to male participants.

Table 2.

Sociodemographics, digital skills, and disconnection

Disconnection behavior (n =3972)
Social context: Alone (n =1559)
Social context: Partner (n =1559)
Social context: Child(ren) (n =1559)
BOR[95% CI]BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI
Intercept−0.070.93[0.21–4.19]0.842.32[0.78–6.88]−4.94***0.01[0.00–0.05]−7.30**0.00[0.00–0.06]
Female−0.250.77[0.35–1.71]−0.440.65[0.36–1.15]0.531.71[0.64–4.541.763.96[0.50–31.56]
Age−0.010.99[0.96–1.02]−0.010.99[0.97–1.01]0.06***1.07[1.03–1.10]0.041.04[0.96–1.13]
Higher educated0.191.21[0.59–2.50]−0.130.87[0.52–1.48]0.411.51[0.64–3.57]−0.140.87[0.13–5.91]
Digital skills (RQ1)−0.190.83[0.52–1.33]
 ICC0.490.270.500.84


Social context: Other family (n =1559)

Social context: Friend(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Colleague(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Other (n =1559)




BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI

Intercept−1.020.36[0.05–2.38]−0.180.84[0.20–3.47]−0.680.51[0.06–4.40]−5.18***0.01[0.00–0.07]
Female0.051.05[0.39–2.86]−0.85*0.43[0.20–0.91]0.241.27[0.40–4.03]0.211.23[0.33–4.52]
Age−0.07**0.94[0.90–0.98]−0.06***0.94[0.91–0.97]−0.08**0.92[0.88–0.97]0.011.01[0.97–1.06]
Higher educated−0.820.44[0.18–1.06]0.121.13[0.57–2.21]−0.320.73[0.26–2.01]0.441.56[0.48–5.05]
 ICC0.340.260.500.58
Disconnection behavior (n =3972)
Social context: Alone (n =1559)
Social context: Partner (n =1559)
Social context: Child(ren) (n =1559)
BOR[95% CI]BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI
Intercept−0.070.93[0.21–4.19]0.842.32[0.78–6.88]−4.94***0.01[0.00–0.05]−7.30**0.00[0.00–0.06]
Female−0.250.77[0.35–1.71]−0.440.65[0.36–1.15]0.531.71[0.64–4.541.763.96[0.50–31.56]
Age−0.010.99[0.96–1.02]−0.010.99[0.97–1.01]0.06***1.07[1.03–1.10]0.041.04[0.96–1.13]
Higher educated0.191.21[0.59–2.50]−0.130.87[0.52–1.48]0.411.51[0.64–3.57]−0.140.87[0.13–5.91]
Digital skills (RQ1)−0.190.83[0.52–1.33]
 ICC0.490.270.500.84


Social context: Other family (n =1559)

Social context: Friend(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Colleague(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Other (n =1559)




BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI

Intercept−1.020.36[0.05–2.38]−0.180.84[0.20–3.47]−0.680.51[0.06–4.40]−5.18***0.01[0.00–0.07]
Female0.051.05[0.39–2.86]−0.85*0.43[0.20–0.91]0.241.27[0.40–4.03]0.211.23[0.33–4.52]
Age−0.07**0.94[0.90–0.98]−0.06***0.94[0.91–0.97]−0.08**0.92[0.88–0.97]0.011.01[0.97–1.06]
Higher educated−0.820.44[0.18–1.06]0.121.13[0.57–2.21]−0.320.73[0.26–2.01]0.441.56[0.48–5.05]
 ICC0.340.260.500.58

Notes: N =105 participants. Analyses for the role of social context were done in a subsample of events in which people disconnected. B = unstandardized coefficient, OR = odds ratio; CI = OR confidence interval; ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

*

P <.05,

**

P <.01,

***

P <.001.

Table 2.

Sociodemographics, digital skills, and disconnection

Disconnection behavior (n =3972)
Social context: Alone (n =1559)
Social context: Partner (n =1559)
Social context: Child(ren) (n =1559)
BOR[95% CI]BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI
Intercept−0.070.93[0.21–4.19]0.842.32[0.78–6.88]−4.94***0.01[0.00–0.05]−7.30**0.00[0.00–0.06]
Female−0.250.77[0.35–1.71]−0.440.65[0.36–1.15]0.531.71[0.64–4.541.763.96[0.50–31.56]
Age−0.010.99[0.96–1.02]−0.010.99[0.97–1.01]0.06***1.07[1.03–1.10]0.041.04[0.96–1.13]
Higher educated0.191.21[0.59–2.50]−0.130.87[0.52–1.48]0.411.51[0.64–3.57]−0.140.87[0.13–5.91]
Digital skills (RQ1)−0.190.83[0.52–1.33]
 ICC0.490.270.500.84


Social context: Other family (n =1559)

Social context: Friend(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Colleague(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Other (n =1559)




BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI

Intercept−1.020.36[0.05–2.38]−0.180.84[0.20–3.47]−0.680.51[0.06–4.40]−5.18***0.01[0.00–0.07]
Female0.051.05[0.39–2.86]−0.85*0.43[0.20–0.91]0.241.27[0.40–4.03]0.211.23[0.33–4.52]
Age−0.07**0.94[0.90–0.98]−0.06***0.94[0.91–0.97]−0.08**0.92[0.88–0.97]0.011.01[0.97–1.06]
Higher educated−0.820.44[0.18–1.06]0.121.13[0.57–2.21]−0.320.73[0.26–2.01]0.441.56[0.48–5.05]
 ICC0.340.260.500.58
Disconnection behavior (n =3972)
Social context: Alone (n =1559)
Social context: Partner (n =1559)
Social context: Child(ren) (n =1559)
BOR[95% CI]BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI
Intercept−0.070.93[0.21–4.19]0.842.32[0.78–6.88]−4.94***0.01[0.00–0.05]−7.30**0.00[0.00–0.06]
Female−0.250.77[0.35–1.71]−0.440.65[0.36–1.15]0.531.71[0.64–4.541.763.96[0.50–31.56]
Age−0.010.99[0.96–1.02]−0.010.99[0.97–1.01]0.06***1.07[1.03–1.10]0.041.04[0.96–1.13]
Higher educated0.191.21[0.59–2.50]−0.130.87[0.52–1.48]0.411.51[0.64–3.57]−0.140.87[0.13–5.91]
Digital skills (RQ1)−0.190.83[0.52–1.33]
 ICC0.490.270.500.84


Social context: Other family (n =1559)

Social context: Friend(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Colleague(s) (n =1559)

Social context: Other (n =1559)




BOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CIBOR95% CI

Intercept−1.020.36[0.05–2.38]−0.180.84[0.20–3.47]−0.680.51[0.06–4.40]−5.18***0.01[0.00–0.07]
Female0.051.05[0.39–2.86]−0.85*0.43[0.20–0.91]0.241.27[0.40–4.03]0.211.23[0.33–4.52]
Age−0.07**0.94[0.90–0.98]−0.06***0.94[0.91–0.97]−0.08**0.92[0.88–0.97]0.011.01[0.97–1.06]
Higher educated−0.820.44[0.18–1.06]0.121.13[0.57–2.21]−0.320.73[0.26–2.01]0.441.56[0.48–5.05]
 ICC0.340.260.500.58

Notes: N =105 participants. Analyses for the role of social context were done in a subsample of events in which people disconnected. B = unstandardized coefficient, OR = odds ratio; CI = OR confidence interval; ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

*

P <.05,

**

P <.01,

***

P <.001.

Disconnection practices and subjective well-being (H1–H3)

The results of the multilevel linear regression models examining the relationship between disconnection and well-being are detailed in Tables 35. We first estimated intercept-only models, where we find intra-class correlations (ICC) of r =0.39 for affective well-being, r =0.48 for life satisfaction, and r =0.41 for social connectedness. In general, the larger the ICC, the lower the variability within a person (and thus momentary assessments become irrelevant), and the higher the variability between the persons. In this case, our ICCs mean that over half of the variability in the outcome variables is due to momentary fluctuations within persons. As such, by using multilevel models, we can better model the variation in the outcome variables by allowing for individual differences in comparison to a model that does not take into account the multilevel structure of the data. Next, we included disconnection behavior as a predictor in the models to test the hypotheses (Tables 35, Model 1). Here, we found no significant effects of disconnection behavior on affective well-being, life satisfaction, or social connectedness. Thus, when a participant disconnected more or less often than they would do on average, this did not change their level of subjective well-being. As such, our hypotheses are rejected.

Table 3.

Disconnection and affective well-being

Model 1: Fixed effects (H1)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBΒ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept5.14***5.14***5.14***5.14***5.09***
 Disconnection0.050.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.050.01[−0.01, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Digital skills−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.030.01[−0.02, 0.03]0.030.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.29***0.11[0.07–0.15]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.041.041.031.030.88
 Intercept0.680.640.680.640.67
 Disconnection0.110.11
ICC0.390.380.400.390.43
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100
Model 1: Fixed effects (H1)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBΒ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept5.14***5.14***5.14***5.14***5.09***
 Disconnection0.050.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.050.01[−0.01, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Digital skills−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.030.01[−0.02, 0.03]0.030.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.29***0.11[0.07–0.15]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.041.041.031.030.88
 Intercept0.680.640.680.640.67
 Disconnection0.110.11
ICC0.390.380.400.390.43
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100

Note: B = unstandardized coefficient, β = standardized coefficient, CI = confidence interval; σ2 = variance, ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

**

P <.01,

***

P <.001.

Table 3.

Disconnection and affective well-being

Model 1: Fixed effects (H1)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBΒ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept5.14***5.14***5.14***5.14***5.09***
 Disconnection0.050.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.050.01[−0.01, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Digital skills−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.030.01[−0.02, 0.03]0.030.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.29***0.11[0.07–0.15]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.041.041.031.030.88
 Intercept0.680.640.680.640.67
 Disconnection0.110.11
ICC0.390.380.400.390.43
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100
Model 1: Fixed effects (H1)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBΒ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept5.14***5.14***5.14***5.14***5.09***
 Disconnection0.050.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.050.01[−0.01, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]0.040.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Digital skills−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]−0.27**−0.16[−0.28, −0.04]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.030.01[−0.02, 0.03]0.030.01[−0.02, 0.04]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.29***0.11[0.07–0.15]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.041.041.031.030.88
 Intercept0.680.640.680.640.67
 Disconnection0.110.11
ICC0.390.380.400.390.43
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100

Note: B = unstandardized coefficient, β = standardized coefficient, CI = confidence interval; σ2 = variance, ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

**

P <.01,

***

P <.001.

Table 4.

Disconnection and social connectedness

Model 1: Fixed effects (H3)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept4.05***4.05***4.05***4.05***3.75***
 Disconnection−0.08−0.02[−0.04, 0.01]−0.09−0.02[−0.04, 0.00]−0.09−0.02[−0.05, 0.02]−0.09−0.02[−0.06, 0.02]
 Digital skills−0.28−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]−0.028−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.100.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.090.02[−0.02, 0.05]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.92***0.25[0.21–0.29]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.971.971.911.911.69
 Intercept1.361.331.361.331.32
 Disconnection0.340.34
ICC0.410.400.430.420.44
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100
Model 1: Fixed effects (H3)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept4.05***4.05***4.05***4.05***3.75***
 Disconnection−0.08−0.02[−0.04, 0.01]−0.09−0.02[−0.04, 0.00]−0.09−0.02[−0.05, 0.02]−0.09−0.02[−0.06, 0.02]
 Digital skills−0.28−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]−0.028−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.100.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.090.02[−0.02, 0.05]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.92***0.25[0.21–0.29]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.971.971.911.911.69
 Intercept1.361.331.361.331.32
 Disconnection0.340.34
ICC0.410.400.430.420.44
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100

Note. B = unstandardized coefficient, β = standardized coefficient, CI = confidence interval; σ2 = variance, ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

***

P <.001.

Table 4.

Disconnection and social connectedness

Model 1: Fixed effects (H3)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept4.05***4.05***4.05***4.05***3.75***
 Disconnection−0.08−0.02[−0.04, 0.01]−0.09−0.02[−0.04, 0.00]−0.09−0.02[−0.05, 0.02]−0.09−0.02[−0.06, 0.02]
 Digital skills−0.28−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]−0.028−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.100.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.090.02[−0.02, 0.05]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.92***0.25[0.21–0.29]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.971.971.911.911.69
 Intercept1.361.331.361.331.32
 Disconnection0.340.34
ICC0.410.400.430.420.44
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100
Model 1: Fixed effects (H3)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept4.05***4.05***4.05***4.05***3.75***
 Disconnection−0.08−0.02[−0.04, 0.01]−0.09−0.02[−0.04, 0.00]−0.09−0.02[−0.05, 0.02]−0.09−0.02[−0.06, 0.02]
 Digital skills−0.28−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]−0.028−0.12[−0.24, 0.00]
 Disconnection × Digital skills0.100.02[−0.01, 0.04]0.090.02[−0.02, 0.05]
 Disconnection with copresent others0.92***0.25[0.21–0.29]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual1.971.971.911.911.69
 Intercept1.361.331.361.331.32
 Disconnection0.340.34
ICC0.410.400.430.420.44
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100

Note. B = unstandardized coefficient, β = standardized coefficient, CI = confidence interval; σ2 = variance, ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

***

P <.001.

Table 5.

Disconnection and life satisfaction

Model 1: Fixed effects (H2)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept72.07***72.08***72.08***72.08***71.76***
 Disconnection0.310.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.260.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.220.01[−0.05, 0.07]0.180.01[−0.05, 0.07]
 Digital skills0.030.00[−0.14, 0.15]0.040.00[−0.14, 0.15]
 Disconnection × Digital skills−0.67−0.03[−0.08, 0.02]−0.62−0.03[−0.08, 0.03]
 Disconnection with copresent others4.83*0.14[0.02–0.25]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual161.18161.07157.77157.77150.75
 Intercept149.83151.53149.64149.64109.39
 Disconnection2.762.76
ICC0.480.490.490.500.42
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100
Model 1: Fixed effects (H2)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept72.07***72.08***72.08***72.08***71.76***
 Disconnection0.310.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.260.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.220.01[−0.05, 0.07]0.180.01[−0.05, 0.07]
 Digital skills0.030.00[−0.14, 0.15]0.040.00[−0.14, 0.15]
 Disconnection × Digital skills−0.67−0.03[−0.08, 0.02]−0.62−0.03[−0.08, 0.03]
 Disconnection with copresent others4.83*0.14[0.02–0.25]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual161.18161.07157.77157.77150.75
 Intercept149.83151.53149.64149.64109.39
 Disconnection2.762.76
ICC0.480.490.490.500.42
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100

Note: B = unstandardized coefficient, β = standardized coefficient, CI = confidence interval; σ2 = variance, ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

*

p <.05,

***

p <.001.

Table 5.

Disconnection and life satisfaction

Model 1: Fixed effects (H2)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept72.07***72.08***72.08***72.08***71.76***
 Disconnection0.310.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.260.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.220.01[−0.05, 0.07]0.180.01[−0.05, 0.07]
 Digital skills0.030.00[−0.14, 0.15]0.040.00[−0.14, 0.15]
 Disconnection × Digital skills−0.67−0.03[−0.08, 0.02]−0.62−0.03[−0.08, 0.03]
 Disconnection with copresent others4.83*0.14[0.02–0.25]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual161.18161.07157.77157.77150.75
 Intercept149.83151.53149.64149.64109.39
 Disconnection2.762.76
ICC0.480.490.490.500.42
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100
Model 1: Fixed effects (H2)
Model 2: Fixed effects with interaction (RQ2)
Model 3: Random effects (Exploratory)
Model 4: Random effects with interaction (Exploratory)
Model 5: Fixed effects (Exploratory)
Bβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CIBβ95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept72.07***72.08***72.08***72.08***71.76***
 Disconnection0.310.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.260.02[−0.04, 0.07]0.220.01[−0.05, 0.07]0.180.01[−0.05, 0.07]
 Digital skills0.030.00[−0.14, 0.15]0.040.00[−0.14, 0.15]
 Disconnection × Digital skills−0.67−0.03[−0.08, 0.02]−0.62−0.03[−0.08, 0.03]
 Disconnection with copresent others4.83*0.14[0.02–0.25]
Random effects
σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2

σ2
 Residual161.18161.07157.77157.77150.75
 Intercept149.83151.53149.64149.64109.39
 Disconnection2.762.76
ICC0.480.490.490.500.42
Observations3,9723,9723,9723,9721,559
N105105105105100

Note: B = unstandardized coefficient, β = standardized coefficient, CI = confidence interval; σ2 = variance, ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

*

p <.05,

***

p <.001.

Additionally, we ran exploratory models with disconnection as a dichotomous variable (i.e., in its uncentered form). This allowed us to test whether a situation in which someone disconnected (yes/no)—rather than whether someone disconnected more on less than they normally would have—led to a change in their well-being. Here, we also find no significant effects of disconnection behavior on well-being outcomes. Finally, we also explored the possibility that there could be lagged effects of disconnection at one time point, and subjective well-being at the next time point (only for affective well-being and social connectedness, as life satisfaction was only assessed at the end of the day). These results also did not yield additional insights.

Individual differences in the disconnection—subjective-well-being relationship (exploratory analyses)

Inspired by previous work in the domain of social media use and well-being (Valkenburg et al., 2021), we conducted exploratory analyses in which we accounted for random slopes to examine whether there are between-person differences in the within-person relationship on disconnection practices and subjective well-being. Extending the models for H1–H3 with random slopes (Tables 35, Model 3) significantly improved the model fit for predictions of affective well-being (X2 (2) =17.97, P <.001) and social connectedness (X2 (2) =44.91, P <.001), but not for life satisfaction (X2 (2) =2.84, P =.242). Figure 1 displays the random effects for the relationship between disconnection and (a) affective well-being and (b) social connectedness. For the relationship between disconnection and affective well-being (Figure 1a), β ranged from –.18 (negative) through .18 (positive), with 11 participants (10.5%) experiencing effects of β = –.05 and lower, 73 people (69.5%) between β = –.05 and .05, and 21 participants (20.0%) of β = .05 and higher. For the relationship between disconnection and social connectedness (Figure 1b) β ranged from –0.40 (negative) through 0.29 (positive), with 32 participants (30.5%) experiencing effects of β = –.05 and lower, 57 people (54.3%) between β = –0.05 and .05, and 16 participants (15.2%) of β = .05 and higher. Although the effects are small, our analyses reveal that the within-person relationship between disconnection and affective well-being, as well as disconnection and social connectedness, differs across people.

Random effects (slopes) of disconnection practices on subjective well-being. (a) Affective well-being. (b) Social connectedness.
Figure 1.

Random effects (slopes) of disconnection practices on subjective well-being. (a) Affective well-being. (b) Social connectedness.

The moderating role of digital skills (RQ2)

To examine the possible moderating effect of digital skills on the relationship between disconnection and subjective well-being, we added a cross-level interaction term to the fixed-effects regression models (Tables 35, Model 2). We found no significant moderation effects, meaning that the within-person relationship between disconnection and subjective well-being did not vary depending on people’s level of digital skills. As exploratory analyses (Tables 35, Model 4), we also ran models with cross-level interaction effects between disconnection and digital skills to examine if the varying slopes in the random-effects model (i.e., differences in effects of disconnection on well-being across people) could be explained by their different levels of digital skills. These analyses did not yield additional insights.

The role of social context of disconnection (exploratory analyses)

Finally, we also explored the role of the social context in which people are disconnecting (Tables 35, Model 5). One reason for disconnection might be the copresence of others and therefore an immediate desire to disconnect from technology. For these analyses, we focused on the subset of observations in which disconnection took place and used a dichotomous variable reflecting the physical copresence of others during a disconnection attempt (1 versus 0 being alone; see “Daily ESM Questionnaires” in the Methods section). Similar to H1–H3, we ran multilevel linear regression models examining the relationship between disconnection in the physical copresence of others (predictor) and subjective well-being (outcome). The analyses revealed significant main effects indicating that when people disconnected while around others, their affective well-being, social connectedness, and life satisfaction momentarily improved.

Discussion and conclusion

This preregistered study aimed to examine the effects of everyday disconnection practices on people’s affective, cognitive, and social well-being, as well as the role of digital skills in this relationship. By employing mobile ESM, we were able to capture people’s nuanced disconnection practices throughout the day (e.g., putting the phone away or with the screen down; deciding not to use certain apps or websites), as well as give insight into the momentary, short-lived effects of such deliberate non-use on subjective well-being. By applying this methodological approach to addressing this relationship, our work offers a refined and nuanced perspective on the implications, or lack thereof, of deliberate non-use of technology in a digital world. With our work, we contribute to the currently small, but rapidly growing, body of literature on digital disconnection and well-being.

While well-being considerations are often central to people’s decision to disconnect (Franks et al., 2018; Nguyen et al., 2022), our study found no within-person relationship between people’s disconnection behaviors and subjective well-being outcomes overall. Our findings align with intervention (i.e., experimental) research that finds mixed effects of digital disconnection (in particular mobile phone and social media) on subjective well-being outcomes (Radtke et al., 2021). It also aligns with broader research on media use and subjective well-being, showing that effects of digital media use remain ambiguous. In the most comprehensive review on digital media use and well-being to date, Meier and Reinecke (2020) have disentangled the relationships between digital media use and well-being by distinguishing between different levels of analysis of digital communication channels and types of well-being indicators. Overall, scholarship on digital disconnection, digital media use, and subjective well-being so far suggests that the non-use and use of digital media can have both positive and negative associations with well-being, that the associations—if any—are often small in size, and that associations are dependent on the specific outcomes investigated, as well as on moderators and mediators (Meier & Reinecke, 2020). Our work shows that momentary deliberate disconnective acts—at least in the short-term—have no clear positive or negative effects on subjective well-being outcomes.

One possible explanation for the lack of effects is that the nuanced disconnection practices examined in this study (e.g., putting digital devices with their screen down, telling oneself not to look on certain websites and apps) are not impactful enough to bring about change in one’s subjective well-being throughout the day. Furthermore, in our study we treated any disconnection in the past two hours as a dichotomous variable, and unfortunately, we have no insight into the length of people's disconnection attempts (i.e., was it a matter of minutes or a full hour?). It could be, nonetheless, that increased engagement in these practices over time has beneficial effects on one’s sense of well-being. This would align with qualitative research where in-depth interviews reveal that people’s retrospective, global experiences of digital connection are positive and considered to be beneficial to people’s well-being (Franks et al., 2018; Nguyen, 2021). Alternatively, it may be that a period of a week was not long enough to detect any changes in subjective well-being. While our power calculations—as outlined in our preregistration—demonstrate that we should have enough measurements over the course of seven days to detect small effects, it could be worthwhile to examine these within-person relationships over extended periods of time, for instance using measurement burst designs (Hofer, 2020).

Our exploratory analyses showed that people vary vastly in the effects that disconnection has on their subjective well-being, ranging from negative to positive. Therefore, a second explanation for the lack of main effects is that this individual variation evens out the main effects. An important follow-up question for future research is what individual factors explain who experiences benefits from disconnecting and who experiences drawbacks from doing so. It could be that the impact of disconnection practices on subjective well-being outcomes is linked to one’s motivations to disconnect. In-depth interviews with people who had taken an extended break from social media revealed that there were different motivations for doing so, and that if people had not been bothered by the impact of digital media use on their well-being to begin with, they also did not feel that their well-being improved after disconnecting (Nguyen, 2021). Related to people’s motivations to disconnect, this study did not consider in what activity people engaged while they were disconnected. As reasons for disconnection can be diverse, one can imagine that some activities undertaken while disconnected could be draining (e.g., working, studying) and thus detrimental for well-being, while others could be relaxing and beneficial (e.g., reading a book, exercising, and spending time with friends). There are also numerous other reasons for disconnecting such as disinterest, privacy concerns, political activism, and health considerations (e.g., Baumer et al., 2013; Hesselberth, 2018; Nguyen et al., 2021; Nguyen, 2023). Furthermore, it is important to consider that momentary digital disconnection could be more influential on immediate outcomes such as distraction and procrastination than on overall well-being per se (Valkenburg, 2022). Future theoretical models of disconnection effects and empirical research could account for how motivations and behaviors shape the consequences that disconnection has for various outcomes, including both momentary and longer-term subjective well-being.

Our study also revealed no moderating effect of digital skills in the relationship between digital disconnection and subjective well-being. We also did not find a relationship between digital skills and disconnection behavior, which is in contrast with earlier research suggesting the existence of socio-digital inequalities in disconnection practices (Gui & Büchi, 2021; Nguyen, 2021; Nguyen & Hargittai, 2023). It could be that digital skills are particularly important for disconnection strategies that are highly technical, such as adjusting device and app settings (Nguyen, 2021). In our study, many of the strategies listed—and especially the most used strategies (e.g., putting screen devices away/with the screen down)—do not require advanced digital skills but perhaps rather high levels of self-regulation. For instance, research shows that people with lower self-control are more likely to respond to incoming notifications immediately (Berger et al., 2018), which suggests they find it more difficult to ignore them. As such, a valid question is to what extent one’s self-control about remaining disconnected and not checking in on digital media is important for the success of one’s disconnection attempt and the benefits that people reap from it in terms of their well-being (Vanden Abeele et al., 2022). Digital skills, then, likely matter more when it concerns more technologically advanced ways of disconnecting. Alternatively, it could be that our measure of digital skills (which asked about internet- and social media-related topics) does not reflect the skills needed to adjust device/app settings, although this would only be relevant for a few of the disconnection strategies listed (i.e., turn off notifications, turn off the internet), which were generally used less often. Moving forward, it could be fruitful to differentiate disconnection by device, platform, and type of social interaction (e.g., following the classification by Meier & Reinecke, 2020) and measure corresponding digital skills, as this could provide more precise insight into how people’s digital know-how interacts with disconnection practices.

Finally, we conducted additional exploratory analyses that revealed insight into the social context in which disconnection takes place. In particular, female participants were less likely to disconnect while being with friends, compared to their male counterparts, which is in line with research showing that phubbing occurs more often among women (Chotpitayasunondh & Douglas, 2016). A potential explanation could be that women generally use digital media more to connect with family and friends (Jackson et al., 2001), making it more challenging for them to disconnect. However, more research into gender roles and related inequalities in disconnection practices is warranted.

We also found that older people (age range was from 19 to 75 years) were more likely to disconnect around their partner. It could be that older people are more likely to have a partner, and that this explains our finding. However, it could also be that relationship quality and emotional closeness with one’s partner become increasingly important as one ages (Birditt et al., 2009; Carstensen, 1992), resulting in a higher need to disconnect from digital media around one’s spouse. Moreover, research on disconnection in older adults has suggested that, unlike younger generations, older adults are less likely to experience conflicts between digital media use and social expectations and responsibilities—and therefore find it less difficult to disconnect (Nguyen et al., 2021). Conversely, we found that younger people were more likely to disconnect around other family members (besides one’s children and spouse), friends, and colleagues. A possible explanation for this is that social norms around disconnection are more prevalent among younger generations (Nguyen et al., 2022), and therefore may be more influential on disconnection behaviors among this age group. For future research, it could be fruitful to examine how social norms around disconnection, as well as generational differences in such normative perceptions, relate to the social contexts in which people disconnect. Research on the social context of disconnection may also consider whether people are living alone or are cohabiting with others (e.g., partner and children; Nguyen & Hargittai, 2023).

Our exploratory analyses revealed that when people disconnected in the copresence of others (versus when they did so while alone), their subjective well-being improved momentarily. This suggests that the situational, social context in which people disconnect is important for the perceived benefits derived from such intended technology disengagement. This makes sense, as the use of one’s smartphone at the expense of focusing on copresent others (i.e., phubbing) is generally found to be disturbing (Vanden Abeele, 2020), which could motivate people to disconnect deliberately during social interactions. Overall, these findings support the hypothesis of “displacement” (Dienlin et al., 2017; Kraut et al., 1998; Valkenburg & Peter, 2007), which implies that the use of digital media may be detrimental to well-being when its use goes at the expense of other meaningful (social) activities. The results are also in line with notions from Communicate Bond Belong theory (Hall & Davis, 2017) and related conceptualizations of digital solitude (Campbell & Ross, 2022), which suggest that people invest their social energy in social interactions that benefit their well-being (be it digitally or in-person). In this case, intended disconnection from technology when one is physically co-present could be done to prioritize the in-person social interaction, which might foster connectedness and sense of well-being overall. Conversely, it is also worth considering that some might use digital media as a way of practicing solitude and disconnecting from one’s immediate social environment (Keessens, 2023), and, for such people, disconnection then might be less beneficial for well-being. In this study, we only collected data on social context for situations in which people disconnected, so we are unable to test whether individuals who are more often in social contexts and disconnect show different effects compared to those who are more often alone. This could be an important avenue for future research.

Limitations

Several limitations and caveats to this study have been mentioned throughout the article already. Nonetheless, it is important to recap these and note other caveats. First, we used mobile application software to send participants six questionnaires each day over the course of one week. These prompts, as well as reminders when participants did not respond, may have interrupted their moments of disconnection—especially as disconnection is often done from one’s mobile phone (Nguyen, 2021). In studying explicit disconnection, other methods of momentary assessments that do not involve people’s personal devices, such as a beeper with paper and pencil questionnaires or a separate device just for study purposes, might be preferable.

It is important to note that the experience sampling software we used was only available to Android users. Since iOS users tend to have higher incomes, the sample may be biased toward lower income users (Slickdeals, 2018). Furthermore, the use of an experience sampling app (from installation to responding to questions on it) means that participants needed a fair bit of digital skills to make their participation work, which could lead to potential biases as well. This is reflected in participants’ relatively high average level of skill in this study, even though we had lower-skilled participants as well.

Our study was conducted in The Netherlands, which is a relatively high-income nation with a good digital infrastructure. As such, it is important to realize that the findings might not generalize to other geographic and cultural contexts. Recent research has pointed to digital inequalities in disconnection practices and has called for more scholarship in countries that are less often the focus of inquiry (Bozan & Treré, 2023; Treré, 2021). We encourage future research to do so, as well, and to embark on cross-national studies to put findings into a comparative perspective.

Conclusion

On the whole, this study adds much needed empirical evidence to important debates about how the deliberate non-use of digital media relates to subjective well-being. As we find no main effects of everyday disconnection practices on subjective well-being, our research suggests that this relationship might be more complex than previous work has been able to consider. Additional exploratory analyses reveal that people vary greatly in the effect disconnection has on their well-being, ranging from negative to positive outcomes. Moreover, disconnecting when others are physically copresent related to greater well-being as opposed to when people disconnected while they were alone, suggesting that social context plays an important role in how people experience disconnection. Given the complexity of the relationship, whether disconnection practices affect individual well-being may be dependent on moderating factors such as people’s motivations for and self-control in disconnecting. While ESM studies are complex and require more resources than many other methods, they are able to capture people’s everyday disconnection experiences in their natural setting in ways that are difficult with other approaches. To this end, it is important for scholars to keep innovating methodologically in the realm of disconnection research and continue collecting such nuanced measures in the future.

Data availability statement

The data and analysis code are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/9chb6/).

Funding

This project has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement “Disconnect2Reconnect” No. 891281, awarded to Minh Hao Nguyen when she was working at the Department of Communication and Media Research (IKMZ), University of Zurich.

Conflicts of interest statement

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Notes

1

In the preregistration materials the hypotheses are listed in a different order, but on substance are the same.

2

In the original preregistration, we planned to examine digital skills as a moderator of the relationship between disconnection and subjective well-being. Upon revisiting this, we decided to also include an exploratory research question on the relationship between digital skills and disconnection.

References

Abascal
J.
,
Civit
A.
(
2000
). Mobile communication for people with disabilities and older people: New opportunities for autonomous life. Proceedings of the 6th ERCIM Workshop (pp.
255
268
).

Andrews
F. M.
,
McKennell
A. C.
(
1980
).
Measures of self-reported well-being: Their affective, cognitive, and other components
.
Social Indicators Research
,
8
,
127
155
. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00286474

Aranda
J. H.
,
Baig
S.
(
2018
). Toward “JOMO”: The joy of missing out and the freedom of disconnecting. Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services (Vol. 19, pp. 1–19:8). https://doi.org/10.1145/3229434.3229468

Arjmand
H. A.
,
Seabrook
E.
,
Bakker
D.
,
Rickard
N.
(
2021
).
Mental health consequences of adversity in Australia: National bushfires associated with increased depressive symptoms, while COVID-19 pandemic associated with increased symptoms of anxiety
.
Frontiers in Psychology
,
12
, 1–7. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635158

Bates
D.
,
Maechler
M.
,
Bolker
B.
,
Walker
S.
,
Christensen
R. H. B.
,
Singmann
H.
,
Dai
B.
,
Scheipl
F.
,
Grothendieck
G.
,
Green
P.
,
Fox
J.
,
Bauer
A.
,
Krivitsky
P. N.
(
2022
). Lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models using “Eigen” and S4 (1.1-29) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4

Baumer
E. P. S.
,
Adams
P.
,
Khovanskaya
V. D.
,
Liao
T. C.
,
Smith
M. E.
,
Schwanda Sosik
V.
,
Williams
K.
(
2013
). Limiting, leaving, and (re)lapsing: An exploration of Facebook non-use practices and experiences. SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp.
3257
3266
). https://doi.org/10.1145/2470654.2466446

Bayer
J. B.
,
Anderson
I. A.
,
Tokunaga
R. S.
(
2022
).
Building and breaking social media habits
.
Current Opinion in Psychology
,
45
,
101303
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101303

Baym
N. K.
,
Wagman
K. B.
,
Persaud
C. J.
(
2020
).
Mindfully scrolling: Rethinking Facebook after time deactivated
.
Social Media + Society,
6
(
2
),
1
10
. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305120919105

Beattie
A.
,
Cassidy
E.
(
2020
).
Locative disconnection: The use of location-based technologies to make disconnection easier, enforceable and exclusive
.
Convergence,
27
(
2
),
395
413
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354856520956854

Benrud‐Larson
L. M.
,
Sandroni
P.
,
Schrag
A.
,
Low
P. A.
(
2005
).
Depressive symptoms and life satisfaction in patients with multiple system atrophy
.
Movement disorders: Official Journal of the Movement Disorder Society
,
20
(
8
),
951
957
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20450

Berger
S.
,
Wyss
A. M.
,
Knoch
D.
(
2018
).
Low self-control capacity is associated with immediate responses to smartphone signals
.
Computers in Human Behavior
,
86
,
45
51
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.04.031

Birditt
K. S.
,
Jackey
L. M. H.
,
Antonucci
T. C.
(
2009
).
Longitudinal patterns of negative relationship quality across adulthood
.
The Journals of Gerontology: Series B
,
64B
(
1
),
55
64
. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbn031

Bozan
V.
,
Treré
E.
(
2023
). When digital inequalities meet digital disconnection: Studying the material conditions of disconnection in rural Turkey. Convergence. https://doi.org/10.1177/13548565231174

Brown
L.
,
Kuss
D. J.
(
2020
).
Fear of missing out, mental wellbeing, and social connectedness: A seven-day social media abstinence trial
.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health
,
17
(
12
),
12
. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124566

Büchi
M.
,
Festic
N.
,
Latzer
M.
(
2019
).
Digital overuse and subjective well-being in a digitized society
.
Social Media + Society
,
5
(
4
),
2056305119886031
. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305119886031

Campbell
S. W.
,
Ross
M. Q.
(
2022
).
Re-conceptualizing solitude in the digital era: From “being alone” to “noncommunication
.”
Communication Theory
,
32
(
3
),
387
406
. https://doi.org/10.1093/ct/qtab021

Carstensen
L. L.
(
1992
).
Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory
.
Psychology and Aging
,
7
(
3
),
331
. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.7.3.331

Chotpitayasunondh
V.
,
Douglas
K. M.
(
2016
).
How “phubbing” becomes the norm: The antecedents and consequences of snubbing via smartphone
.
Computers in Human Behavior
,
63
,
9
18
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.018

Christensen
T. H.
(
2009
).
“Connected presence” in distributed family life
.
New Media & Society
,
11
(
3
),
433
451
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444808101620

Comparis
. (
2018
). Ein Viertel der Schweizer schaltet nie ab: Comparis umfrage zum ohne Facebook tag. https://www.comparis.ch/comparis/press/medienmitteilungen/artikel/2018/digital/ohne-facebook-tag/digitale-auszeit

de Boer
A. G.
,
van Lanschot
J. J.
,
Stalmeier
P. F.
,
van Sandick
J. W.
,
Hulscher
J. B.
,
de Haes
J. C.
,
Sprangers
M. A.
(
2004
).
Is a single-item visual analogue scale as valid, reliable and responsive as multi-item scales in measuring quality of life?
Quality of Life Research
,
13
,
311
320
. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:QURE.0000018499.64574.1f

de Vries
L. P.
,
Baselmans
B. M. L.
,
Bartels
M.
(
2021
).
Smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment of well-being: A systematic review and recommendations for future studies
.
Journal of Happiness Studies
,
22
(
5
),
2361
2408
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00324-7

Diener
E.
,
Wirtz
D.
,
Biswas-Diener
R.
,
Tov
W.
,
Kim-Prieto
C.
,
Choi
D. W.
,
Oishi
S.
(
2009
). New measures of well-being. Assessing well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener (pp.
247
266
).

Dienlin
T.
,
Masur
P. K.
,
Trepte
S.
(
2017
).
Reinforcement or displacement? The reciprocity of FTF, IM, and SNS communication and their effects on loneliness and life satisfaction
.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
,
22
(
2
),
71
87
. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12183

DiMaggio
P.
,
Hargittai
E.
,
Celeste
C.
,
Schafer
S.
(
2004
). Digital inequality: From unequal access to differentiated use. In
Neckerman
K.
(Ed.),
Social inequality
(pp.
355
400
).
Russell Sage Foundation
.

Franks
J.
,
Chenhall
R.
,
Keogh
L.
(
2018
).
The Facebook sabbatical as a cycle: Describing the gendered experience of young adults as they navigate disconnection and reconnection
.
Social Media + Society
,
4
(
3
),
2056305118801995
. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305118801995

Giannakos
M. N.
,
Chorianopoulos
K.
,
Giotopoulos
K.
,
Vlamos
P.
(
2013
).
Using Facebook out of habit
.
Behaviour & Information Technology
,
32
(
6
),
594
602
. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2012.659218

Gui
M.
,
Büchi
M.
(
2021
).
From use to overuse: Digital inequality in the age of communication abundance
.
Social Science Computer Review
,
39
(
1
),
3
19
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439319851163

Gui
M.
,
Fasoli
M.
,
Carradore
R.
,
Carradore
R.
(
2017
).
“Digital well-being”. Developing a new theoretical tool for media literacy research
.
Italian Journal of Sociology of Education
,
9
(
1
),
155
173
. https://doi.org/10.14658/pupj-ijse-2017-1-8

Hall
J. A.
,
Davis
D. C.
(
2017
).
Proposing the communicate bond belong theory: Evolutionary intersections with episodic interpersonal communication
.
Communication Theory
,
27
(
1
),
21
47
. https://doi.org/10.1111/comt.12106

Hall
J. A.
,
Xing
C.
,
Ross
E. M.
,
Johnson
R. M.
(
2021
).
Experimentally manipulating social media abstinence: Results of a four-week diary study
.
Media Psychology
,
24
(
2
),
259
275
. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1688171

Hargittai
E.
(
2005
).
Survey measures of web-oriented digital literacy
.
Social Science Computer Review
,
23
(
3
),
371
379
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439305275911

Hargittai
E.
(
2008
). The digital reproduction of inequality. In
Grusky
D.
(Ed.),
Social stratification
(pp.
936
944
).
Westview Press
.

Hargittai
E.
(
2020
).
Potential biases in big data: Omitted voices on social media
.
Social Science Computer Review
,
38
(
1
),
10
24
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439318788322

Hargittai
E.
,
Hsieh
Y. P.
(
2012
).
Succinct survey measures of Web-use skills
.
Social Science Computer Review
,
30
(
1
),
95
107
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439310397146

Hargittai
E.
,
Micheli
M.
(
2019
). Internet skills and why they matter. In
Graham
M.
,
Dutton
W. H.
(Eds.),
Society and the internet. How networks of information and communication are changing our lives
(2nd ed., pp.
109
126
).
Oxford University Press
.

Hargittai
E.
,
Neuman
W. R.
,
Curry
O.
(
2012
).
Taming the information tide: Perceptions of information overload in the American home
.
The Information Society
,
28
(
3
),
161
173
. https://doi.org/10.1080/01972243.2012.669450

Henrich
J.
,
Heine
S. J.
,
Norenzayan
A.
(
2010
).
The weirdest people in the world?
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
,
33
(
2–3
),
61
83
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

Hesselberth
P.
(
2018
).
Discourses on 35 ysconnectivity and the right to disconnect
.
New Media & Society
,
20
(
5
),
1994
2010
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444817711449

Hinsch
C.
,
Sheldon
K. M.
(
2013
).
The impact of frequent social Internet consumption: Increased procrastination and lower life satisfaction
.
Journal of Consumer Behaviour
,
12
(
6
),
496
505
. https://doi.org/10.1002/cb.1453

Hofer
M.
(
2020
). A measurement burst study of media use and well-being among older adults: Logistically challenging at best.
Chapter eleven a measurement burst study of media use and well-being among older adults: Logistically challenging at best
(pp.
225
244
).
Columbia University Press
. https://doi.org/10.7312/harg18876-012

Hofer
M.
,
Hargittai
E.
,
Büchi
M.
,
Seifert
A.
(
2019
).
Older adults’ online information seeking and subjective well-being: The moderating role of internet skills
.
International Journal of Communication
,
13
(
00
),
4426
4443
. https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/10981

Hooper
D.
,
Coughlan
J.
,
Mullen
M.
(
2008
). Evaluating model fit: A synthesis of the structural equation modelling literature. 7th European Conference on Research Methodology for Business and Management Studies (pp.
195–200)
.

Jackson
L. A.
,
Ervin
K. S.
,
Gardner
P. D.
,
Schmitt
N.
(
2001
).
Gender and the Internet: Women communicating and men searching
.
Sex Roles
,
44
(
5
),
363
379
. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010937901821

Jorge
A.
(
2019
).
Social media, interrupted: Users recounting temporary disconnection on instagram
.
Social Media + Society
,
5
(
4
),
2056305119881691
. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305119881691

Kantar
. (
2018
). Kantar China social media impact report 2018. https://cn-en.kantar.com/media/social/2018/kantar-china-social-media-impact-report-2018/

Keessen
L.
(
2023
). Development and Validation of the Need for Solitude Scale.
74th Annual International Communication Association Conference
, Toronto, Canada.

Kraut
R.
,
Lundmark
V.
,
Patterson
M.
,
Kiesler
S.
,
Mukhopadhyay
T.
,
Scherlis
W.
(
1998
).
Internet paradox: A social technology that reduces social involvement and psychological well-being?
American Psychologist
,
53
(
9
),
1017
1031
. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.9.1017

Kuznetsova
A.
,
Brockhoff
P. B.
,
Christensen
R. H. B.
,
Jensen
S. P.
(
2020
). lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models (3.1-3). https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.html

Licoppe
C.
(
2004
).
‘Connected’ presence: The emergence of a new repertoire for managing social relationships in a changing communication technoscape
.
Environment and Planning D: Society and Space
,
22
(
1
),
135
156
. https://doi.org/10.1068/d323t

Marttila
E.
,
Koivula
A.
,
Räsänen
P.
(
2021
).
Does excessive social media use decrease subjective well-being? A longitudinal analysis of the relationship between problematic use, loneliness and life satisfaction
.
Telematics and Informatics
,
59
,
1
11
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2020.101556

Meier
A.
(
2021
).
Studying problems, not problematic usage: Do mobile checking habits increase procrastination and decrease well-being?
Mobile Media & Communication
,
10
(
2
), 272–293. https://doi.org/10.1177/20501579211029326

Meier
A.
,
Reinecke
L.
(
2020
).
Computer-mediated communication, social media, and mental health: A conceptual and empirical meta-review
.
Communication Research
,
48
(
8
),
1182
–1
209
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650220958224

Myin-Germeys
I.
,
Kuppens
P.
(
2022
).
The open handbook of experience sampling methodology: A step-by-step guide to designing, conducting, and analyzing ESM studies
.
Center for Research on Experience Sampling and Ambulatory Methods Leuven
.

Nassen
L. M.
,
Vandebosch
H.
,
Poels
K.
,
Karsay
K.
(
2023
).
Opt-out, abstain, unplug. A systematic review of the voluntary digital disconnection literature
.
Telematics and Informatics
,
81,
101980
.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2023.101980

Nguyen
M. H.
(
2021
).
Managing social media use in an “always-on” society: Exploring digital wellbeing strategies that people use to disconnect
.
Mass Communication and Society
,
24
(
6
),
795
817
. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2021.1979045

Nguyen
M. H.
(
2023
).
“Maybe I should get rid of it for a while…”: Examining motivations and challenges for social media disconnection
.
The Communication Review
,
26
(
2
),
125
150
. https://doi.org/10.1080/10714421.2023.2195795

Nguyen
M. H.
,
Büchi
M.
,
Geber
S.
(
2022
).
Everyday disconnection experiences: Exploring people’s understanding of digital well-being and management of digital media use
.
New Media & Society, online first
, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448221105428

Nguyen
M. H.
,
Hargittai
E.
(
2023
).
Digital inequality in disconnection practices: voluntary nonuse during COVID-19
.
Journal of Communication
,
73
(
5
),
494
510
. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqad021

Nguyen
M. H.
,
Hargittai
E.
,
Fuchs
J.
,
Djukaric
T.
,
Hunsaker
A.
(
2021
).
Trading spaces: How and why older adults disconnect from and switch between digital media
.
The Information Society
,
37
(
5
),
299
311
. https://doi.org/10.1080/01972243.2021.1960659

Nguyen
M. H.
,
Hunsaker
A.
,
Hargittai
E.
(
2020
).
Older adults’ online social engagement and social capital: The moderating role of Internet skills
.
Information, Communication & Society
,
25
(
7
),
942
958
. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2020.1804980

Nuñez
T. R.
,
Radtke
T.
(
2023
).
Is socially disruptive smartphone use detrimental to well-being? A systematic meta-analytic review on being phubbed
.
Behaviour & Information Technology
,
1
29
. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2023.2209213

Orben
A.
(
2020
).
Teenagers, screens and social media: a narrative review of reviews and key studies
.
Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology
,
55
(
4
),
407
414
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-019-01825-4

Radtke
T.
,
Apel
T.
,
Schenkel
K.
,
Keller
J.
,
von Lindern
E.
(
2021
).
Digital detox: An effective solution in the smartphone era? A systematic literature review
.
Mobile Media & Communication
,
10
(
2
), 190–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/20501579211028647

Ross
M. Q.
,
Akgün
E.
,
Campbell
S. W.
(
2023
).
Benefits of solitude for connected individuals in the United States but not China: Situating solitude in communicate bond belong theory
.
Computers in Human Behavior
,
144
,
107731
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2023.107731

Rosseel
Y.
,
Jorgensen
T. D.
,
Rockwood
N.
,
Oberski
D.
,
Byrnes
J.
,
Vanbrabant
L.
,
Savalei
V.
,
Merkle
E.
,
Hallquist
M.
,
Rhemtulla
M.
,
Katsikatsou
M.
,
Barendse
M.
,
Scharf
F.
,
Du
H.
(
2023
). Lavaan: Latent variable analysis (0.6-16). https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lavaan/index.html

Ryan
R. M.
,
Deci
E. L.
(
2001
).
On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being
.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
52
,
141
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141

Schellewald
A.
(
2021
).
On getting carried away by the TikTok algorithm
.
AoIR Selected Papers of Internet Research
,
2021
, pp.
1
4
. https://doi.org/10.5210/spir.v2021i0.12039

Schmuck
D.
(
2020
).
Does digital detox work? Exploring the role of digital detox applications for problematic smartphone use and well-being of young adults using multigroup analysis
.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
,
23
(
8
),
526
532
. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2019.0578

Schnauber-Stockmann
A.
,
Weber
M.
,
Reinecke
L.
,
Schemer
C.
,
Müller
K.
,
Beutel
M.
,
Stark
B.
(
2021
).
Mobile (self-)socialization: The role of media and communication in autonomy and relationship development in adolescence
.
Mass Communication and Society
,
24
(
6
),
867
891
. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2021.1964538

Sheldon
K. M.
,
Abad
N.
,
Hinsch
C.
(
2011
).
A two-process view of Facebook use and relatedness need-satisfaction: Disconnection drives use, and connection rewards it
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
100
(
4
),
766
775
. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022407

Slickdeals
. (
2018
). Iphone users spend $101 every month on tech purchases, nearly double of android users, according to a survey conducted by Slickdeals. https://www.prnewswire.com/news-releases/iphone-users-spend-101-every-month-on-tech-purchases-nearly-double-of-android-users-according-to-a-survey-conducted-by-slickdeals-300739582.html

Taylor
S. H.
,
Bazarova
N. N.
(
2021
).
Always available, always attached: A relational perspective on the effects of mobile phones and social media on subjective well-being
.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
,
26
(
4
),
187
206
. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmab004

Treré
E.
(
2021
).
Intensification, discovery and abandonment: unearthing global ecologies of dis/connection in pandemic times
.
Convergence
,
27
(
6
),
1663
1677
. https://doi.org/10.1177/13548565211036804

Valkenburg
P. M.
,
Peter
J.
(
2007
).
Online communication and adolescent well-being: Testing the stimulation versus the displacement hypothesis
.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
,
12
(
4
),
1169
1182
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00368.x

Valkenburg
P. M.
(
2022
).
Social media use and well-being: What we know and what we need to know.
Current Opinion in Psychology
,
45
,
101294
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.12.006

Valkenburg
P.
,
Beyens
I.
,
Pouwels
J. L.
,
van Driel
I. I.
,
Keijsers
L.
(
2021
).
Social media use and adolescents’ self-esteem: Heading for a person-specific media effects paradigm
.
Journal of Communication
,
71
(
1
),
56
78
. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqaa039

Vanden Abeele
M.
(
2020
). The social consequences of phubbing. In
Ling
R.
,
Fortunati
L.
,
Goggin
G.
,
Lim
S.S.
,
Li
Y.
(Eds.),
The Oxford handbook of mobile communication and society
(pp.
158
174
).
Oxford University Press
.

Vanden Abeele
M. M. P.
,
Halfmann
A.
,
Lee
E. W. J.
(
2022
).
Drug, demon, or donut? Theorizing the relationship between social media use, digital well-being and digital disconnection
.
Current Opinion in Psychology
,
45
,
101295
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.12.007

Vanden Abeele
M. M. P.
,
Hendrickson
A. T.
,
Pollmann
M. M.
,
Ling
R.
(
2019
).
Phubbing behavior in conversations and its relation to perceived conversation intimacy and distraction: An exploratory observation study
.
Computers in Human Behavior
,
100
,
35
47
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.06.004

Vanden Abeele
M. M. P.
,
Nguyen
M. H.
(
2023
).
Digital media as ambiguous goods: Examining the digital well-being experiences and disconnection practices of Belgian adults
.
European Journal of Communication
, Online first,
1
23
. https://doi.org/10.1177/02673231231201487

Vorderer
P.
,
Hefner
D.
,
Reinecke
L.
,
Klimmt
C.
(
2017
).
Permanently online, permanently connected: Living and communicating in a POPC world
.
Routledge
.

Wohn
D. Y.
(
2012
).
The role of habit strength in social network game play
.
Communication Research Reports
,
29
(
1
),
74
79
. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824096.2011.639912

Wolfers
L. N.
,
Utz
S.
(
2022
).
Social media use, stress, and coping
.
Current Opinion in Psychology
,
45
,
101305
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101305

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Associate Editor: Scott Campbell
Scott Campbell
Associate Editor
Search for other works by this author on:

Open Science Framework awards
Open Data Open Data
Digitally shareable data necessary to reproduce the reported results are publicly available for this article.
Preregistered Preregistered
Research design was preregistered.