Abstract

Recent prereading evidence demonstrates that white matter alterations are associated with dyslexia even before the onset of reading instruction. At the same time, remediation of reading difficulties is suggested to be most effective when provided as early as kindergarten, yet evidence is currently lacking on the early neuroanatomical changes associated with such preventive interventions. To address this open question, we investigated white matter changes following early literacy intervention in Dutch-speaking prereaders (aged 5–6 years) with an increased cognitive risk for developing dyslexia. Diffusion-weighted images were acquired before and after a 12-week digital intervention in three groups: (i) at-risk children receiving phonics-based training (n = 31); (ii) at-risk children engaging with active control training (n = 25); and (iii) typically developing children (n = 27) receiving no intervention. Following automated quantification of white matter tracts relevant for reading, we first examined baseline differences between at-risk and typically developing children, revealing bilateral dorsal and ventral differences. Longitudinal analyses showed that white matter properties changed within the course of the training; however, the absence of intervention-specific results suggests that these changes rather reflect developmental effects. This study contributes important first insights on the neurocognitive mechanisms of intervention that precedes formal reading onset.

Introduction

Learning to read is one of the most important educational achievements of early childhood, setting the foundation for long-term learning. Reading development relies on the reorganization of existing brain circuits including dorsal temporoparietal, ventral occipitotemporal, and inferior frontal regions, that facilitate the processing and integration of orthographic, phonological, and semantic information (Pugh et al. 2001; Schlaggar and McCandliss 2007). Although the majority of children learn to read with relative ease during the first school years, around 7% are diagnosed with developmental dyslexia, which means they experience severe and persistent reading difficulties that occur despite adequate opportunity and instruction (Peterson and Pennington 2015). In most educational settings, dyslexia is typically diagnosed after years of demonstrated reading failure, which has a considerable impact on socioemotional well-being (Valas 1999; Grills-Taquechel et al. 2012) and future educational attainment (Dougherty 2003). Importantly, a gap in reading ability between dyslexic and typical readers is shown to be present as early as first grade (Ferrer et al. 2015), and is likely to persist throughout the school years and potentially widen over time if not addressed properly (Stanovich 1986; Ferrer et al. 2015). Research has shown that extensive reading interventions (comprising at least 100 sessions) are more effective if provided during kindergarten and first grade, compared with later grades (Wanzek and Vaughn 2007). Yet, in current practice, struggling readers are likely to receive the necessary support only after years of reading failure and thus after the most sensitive period for intervention has passed. This has been described as the “dyslexia paradox” (Ozernov-Palchik and Gaab 2016). With the aim of reducing or closing the gap, preventive interventions (i.e., interventions that take place before the onset of reading acquisition) offer promising potential to overcome the consequences of this paradox and ensure that reducing or even closing the gap between struggling and typical readers does not become unattainable. To date, however, no studies have investigated the neuroanatomical changes associated with preventive interventions in young children at risk for dyslexia. Hence, the goal of this study is to characterize white matter microstructural changes during such an early intervention.

In the last two decades, an abundance of neuroimaging studies provided evidence of atypical brain structure in dyslexia, and especially in white matter. Long-range white matter pathways carry signals between cortical regions and therefore constitute a key part of the reading circuitry. Diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) studies, which enable the identification and quantification of white matter connections, have identified both dorsal and ventral pathways linked to reading: the arcuate fasciculus (AF), the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), and the inferior frontooccipital fasciculus (IFOF) (Ben-Shachar et al. 2007; Vandermosten, Boets, Wouters, et al. 2012; Wandell and Yeatman 2013; Zhang et al. 2014). The AF dorsally connects inferior frontal to superior temporal and inferior parietal cortex (Catani et al. 2005). The left AF is thought to constitute a key region for sensory-motor mapping of sound to articulation (Saur et al. 2008) as well as grapheme–phoneme mapping (Gullick and Booth 2014). Indeed, several studies support the role of the left AF sustaining phonological aspects of reading (Yeatman et al. 2011; Saygin et al. 2013; Vanderauwera et al. 2015; Vandermosten et al. 2015), while reduced fractional anisotropy (FA) has been reported in both children (Vanderauwera et al. 2017; Van Der Auwera et al. 2021) and adults (Vandermosten, Boets, Poelmans, et al. 2012) with dyslexia. The IFOF and ILF are ventral association tracts connecting the occipital to the inferior frontal and to the anterior temporal cortex, respectively (Saur et al. 2008; Forkel et al. 2014). The left IFOF has been linked to orthographic aspects of reading in both adults and school-aged children (Vandermosten, Boets, Poelmans, et al. 2012; Vanderauwera et al. 2018; Banfi et al. 2019). A different pattern seems to emerge in prereaders, however, with evidence suggesting associations between bilateral IFOF and early phonological ability (Walton et al. 2018) as well as early reading-related cognitive measures (Vanderauwera et al. 2018). Properties of bilateral ILF have been shown to correlate with phonological decoding in school-aged children (Lebel et al. 2013), while in prereading children, left ILF properties were related to emergent literacy skills including both phonological and orthographic aspects (Vanderauwera et al. 2018). In terms of anatomy, both the IFOF and ILF likely contain projections adjacent to the posterior occipitotemporal cortex around the visual word form area (Yeatman et al. 2013), a region specialized in visual letter recognition, and thus important for word recognition and consequently reading (Dehaene and Cohen 2011).

Most strikingly, recent studies in prereaders suggest that these white matter alterations associated with dyslexia are evident even before the onset of reading acquisition. More specifically, properties of the left frontotemporal and frontoparietal AF segments and left IFOF were shown to differ between prereaders with and without a familial risk for dyslexia (Vandermosten et al. 2015; Langer et al. 2017; Wang et al. 2017). In addition to a link with familial risk, FA differences in bilateral AF were already present in prereading children who later developed dyslexia compared with children who later developed typical reading skills (Vanderauwera et al. 2017). Longitudinal evidence of children throughout reading development also suggests that white matter properties of the left AFdirect in kindergarten were predictive of later reading outcomes (Vanderauwera et al. 2017; Borchers et al. 2019; Van Der Auwera et al. 2021), while the rate of white matter development was significantly associated with subsequent reading skill (Yeatman, Dougherty, Ben-Shachar, 2012; Wang et al. 2017). Moreover, recent evidence points out that early deficits in the left AFdirect of dyslexic compared with typical readers remain present throughout primary school (Van Der Auwera et al. 2021). Taken together, these findings provide converging evidence that early differences in white matter structure predate the formal onset of reading, suggesting a central role in determining (a) typical trajectories of reading development.

In light of these findings, the question arises whether preventive intervention can target these early white matter deviances and potentially have an impact on subsequent reading development. Previous research provides convincing evidence that white matter is malleable and subject to learning- and experience-dependent changes in controlled designs. Studies in school-aged children demonstrated that explicit training in high-level cognitive domains such as spelling and math, resulted in behavioral improvements accompanied by training-induced white matter plasticity (Gebauer et al. 2012; Jolles et al. 2016). Such training-induced plasticity was also demonstrated in a younger age group, where Ekerdt et al. (2020) showed white matter changes in typically developing 4-year-old preliterate children following three weeks of oral word learning. More specifically, children in the oral word training group had increased FA in the left dorsal precentral gyrus, when compared to both active and passive control groups.

Only a few studies have directly investigated structural plasticity following targeted reading intervention. Following a reading intervention in school-aged children between 6 and 9 years old, increased cortical thickness was reported in treatment responders, when compared with treatment nonresponders and a waiting control group (Romeo et al. 2018). With respect to white matter studies, Keller and Just (2009) demonstrated for the first time that poor readers (aged 8–12 years) who received intensive reading instruction over the course of 6 months, showed increased FA in the left centrum semiovale in contrast to both poor readers who received no training and good readers. Using a longitudinal design with several imaging assessments, Huber et al. (2018) provide additional evidence for reading intervention-related white matter plasticity in children with reading difficulties. The authors reported widespread changes in diffusion properties together with significant growth in reading skill, after a training period of eight weeks. On the other hand, Partanen et al. (2021) did not find any evidence of intervention-related white matter changes following a three-month intervention in dyslexic children. Overall, these studies provide important insights on how behavioral interventions targeting reading skills can induce measurable changes in white matter properties of school-aged children, although evidence is lacking on the impact of interventions during the sensitive period for learning to read. Moreover, the absence of adequate inclusion of control groups in many training studies makes it difficult to separate maturation from learning or experience effects. As a result, very little is known about the mechanisms of early intervention, particularly when it takes place prior to formal reading instruction.

The present study aimed to address these remaining gaps in knowledge by investigating the impact of a preventive intervention in 5-year-old prereaders with an increased cognitive risk for developing dyslexia. Our objectives were twofold. First, we aimed to examine cognitive risk-related white matter differences prior to intervention by comparing at-risk with typically developing prereaders. Second, by means of a controlled intervention design, we sought to characterize intervention-related white matter changes following 12 weeks of digital at-home training. In order to comprehensively address these goals, we assessed white matter microstructure longitudinally in (i) at-risk children who received phonics-based training adapted for kindergarten, (ii) at-risk children engaging with another immersive game that did not train literacy skills, and (iii) typically developing children receiving no intervention. We hypothesized phonics-based training-related FA changes in white matter connections sustaining early phonological and orthographic processing, therefore focusing on four bilateral pathways: the frontotemporal and frontoparietal segments of the dorsal AF (AFdirect and AFanterior, respectively) and the ventral ILF and IFOF.

Materials and Methods

Study Design

Diffusion-weighted images were collected before and after a 12-week training period to evaluate changes in white matter structure. These data were collected as part of a larger longitudinal intervention project (n = 149) in which additional behavioral and neurophysiological (Vanden Bempt et al. 2021; Van Herck et al. 2021) assessments were carried out. The current study focuses on the direct effects of phonics-based training on white matter properties.

Participants

The participants of this study were 90 monolingual Dutch-speaking children in their third year of kindergarten, which is the last year before starting primary school. Participants had no prior known hearing or neurological problems, no formal diagnosis or risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Smidts and Oosterlaan 2007), and no history of speech and language therapy. All children had received comparable kindergarten education up until the beginning of the study and had not received any formal reading or writing instruction, in accordance with Flemish government guidelines (https://onderwijsdoelen.be/). The data reported in this study were collected between December 2018 and June 2019, thus before the start of the COVID-19 pandemic.

Children with and without an increased cognitive risk for developing dyslexia were selected based on a screening conducted at school at the beginning of their last year of kindergarten. Figure 1 provides a detailed description of participant flow in the study. Over 1900 families responded to a first questionnaire, 1225 of whom met the inclusion criteria described above and were selected to participate in a risk screening. Nonverbal reasoning and subsequent intelligence estimates were calculated using a tablet-based version of the Raven’s Color Progressive Matrices (Raven et al. 1984). Three early literacy predictors were assessed to define risk criteria: letter knowledge, phonological awareness, and rapid automatized naming (Puolakanaho et al. 2007; Caravolas et al. 2012). After excluding incomplete or unreliable scores (n = 21) and children with the lowest 10% of nonverbal intelligence estimates (n = 113), percentile scores for the early literacy measures were calculated in a sample of 1091 Flemish kindergarteners. Children were classified as at-risk if they performed below the 30th percentile on two out of the three measures and below the 40th percentile on letter knowledge. They were classified as typically developing if they performed above the 40th percentile on all measures. For more details on the screening procedure and behavioral assessments, see Verwimp et al. (2020).

Adapted CONSORT flow diagram.
Figure 1

Adapted CONSORT flow diagram.

According to the criteria described above, 60 children with and 30 children without a cognitive risk were selected to take part in the intervention study. Of the 60 at-risk children, 31 received a tablet-based intervention that aimed at training early literacy skills, while the other 29 children engaged with a comparably immersive control intervention. Group assignment for the at-risk children was done using a block randomization procedure (R package randomizr; Coppock 2019), to balance the covariates of sex and birth trimester across groups. The group of typically developing children (n = 30) was matched for sex, age, and nonverbal intelligence and participated in the behavioral and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) assessments but not in any intervention.

For all cognitive and neuroimaging assessments, written informed consent and verbal assent were obtained from all primary caregivers and children, respectively. This study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Leuven University Hospital.

Tablet-Based Intervention

In the present study, early literacy intervention focused on the systematic training of grapheme–phoneme coupling (how letters correspond to sounds), a fundamental aspect of learning to read in alphabetic scripts. Interventions that involve this type of training were shown to be the most beneficial in ameliorating reading deficits in poor readers (Galuschka et al. 2014) and were also suggested to be beneficial for young children at risk of reading delay (Hatcher et al. 2004; Snowling and Hulme 2011, 2012; Lonigan et al. 2013). For the specific purposes of this study, we adapted the GraphoGame interface (Richardson and Lyytinen 2014) to create a new tablet-based version (GraphoGame-Flemish; GG-FL) which is appropriate for Flemish kindergarteners without formal reading instruction. Briefly, the training started by introducing the graphemes, providing exercises of visual and auditory discrimination of graphemes and phonemes, while gradually progressing to exercises of grapheme–phoneme coupling, phoneme blending and counting, and early reading. For a detailed description of the game content and level progression, see Glatz et al. (2022).

Children in the active control (AC) group received a different type of training. For this purpose, six commercially available tablet-based games (LEGO City My City, LEGO DUPLO Town, LEGO DUPLO Connected Train, LEGO Friends: Heartlake Rush, PLAYMOBIL Horse Farm, and PLAYMOBIL Police) were implemented within a single custom-made application. The games were selected because they were age-appropriate, were motivating enough for a training period of 12 weeks, and did not train early literacy skills. These characteristics were confirmed by pilot data of 5-year-old kindergarteners collected before the start of the study.

The intervention was intended for at-home use, whereby all participating families received the necessary equipment (Samsung Galaxy Table E9.6 tablet and Audiotechnica ATH M20X headphones). The games were installed on a child-friendly profile and the app icons were modified into colored stars to conceal the group assignment. They were asked to play their game (GG-FL or AC) for 15 min a day, six days a week for a total duration of 12 weeks. As part of the larger longitudinal project, both intervention groups received an additional story listening game, which they were asked to use for 10 min a day in addition to their normal training schedule. The effects of story listening fall beyond the scope of the current study and will not be discussed further. Note, however, that the two groups (GG-FL and AC) did not differ in the amount of story listening (W = 378.5, P = 0.805) or the total amount of intervention-related tablet exposure (W = 361, P = 0.480). All families received additional support during the intervention period in the form of a detailed instruction manual, intervention calendar to monitor progress, as well as stickers and small rewards for reaching milestones during the training period. Data logs were transmitted daily to a central university server and contact with the parents was maintained throughout the intervention in the case of technical or motivational/compliance issues.

MRI Data Acquisition

Participants were invited for two MRI sessions, one before and one shortly after the intervention phase. All images were acquired on a 3 T Philips Achieva MRI scanner (Philips, Best, The Netherlands) using a 32-channel head coil and identical acquisition parameters. The sequence consisted of seven nondiffusion-weighted volumes, followed by 20, 32, and 60 diffusion-weighted volumes with b-values of 700, 1000, and 2000 s/mm2, respectively. Additional scanning parameters were as follows: 62 transverse slices, slice thickness = 2.2 mm, voxel size 2.2 x 2.3 x 2.2 mm3, TR/TE (ms) = 3593/88 ms, flip angle = 90|${\kern0em }^{\circ }$|⁠, multiband factor MB = 2, EPI factor = 43, and acquisition time = 7:38 min. In the same session, a reversed phase-encoding direction (posterior–anterior) sequence was acquired for subsequent distortion correction during processing. Anatomical 3D T1-weighted images were acquired using a CS-SENSE TFE sequence with following parameters: 240 sagittal slices, isotropic voxel size 0.9 mm3, TR/TE (ms) = 9.1/4.2, and acquisition time = 3:30 min. In line with previous MRI studies of young children (Raschle et al. 2009; Theys et al. 2014; Vanderauwera et al. 2017), an age-appropriate preparation protocol was used to familiarize the children with the scanning procedure and minimize motion artifacts. In order to make the experience more pleasant, the scanner room was decorated in the protocol story theme and the children were given time to acclimatize with the surrounding environment. Communication was maintained during the scan via a built-in microphone system and close monitoring was possible using a camera system. To limit head movement, custom-fit inflatable pads were used for head positioning, as well as positive encouragement during scanning. The children watched a cartoon of their choice during scan acquisition. All participants received a small nonmonetary reward at the end of each visit.

Of the total sample of 90 children reported in the present study, MRI data are not available for six individuals at pretest who either did not wish to participate in MRI assessments (n = 2), or were not able to complete any of the MRI examinations (n = 4). At posttest, MRI data are not available for four additional participants who did not plan a second visit and one dataset that was compromised due to technical issues (see Fig. 1).

MRI Data Preprocessing

Diffusion MRI data were preprocessed using the FMRIB Software Library v6.0 (FSL; Jenkinson et al. 2012). A pair of nondiffusion-weighted images in forward- and reversed-encoding directions were used for estimating the off-resonance field (Andersson et al. 2003) and correcting for susceptibility distortions (Smith et al. 2004). The volumes of all shells of the DWI series were subsequently corrected for motion and eddy current distortions (Andersson and Sotiropoulos 2016) followed by brain mask estimation (Smith 2002). Scalar FA maps were produced using FSL’S DTIFIT, which fits the tensor model on the data. The amount of head motion in each subject was quantified as the mean volume displacement relative to the first volume of the DWI series. This was done by calculating the root mean square of the three translation parameters (x, y, z), similar to Yendiki et al. (2014). Eleven datasets (n = 5 at pretest and n = 6 at post-test) with mean translational movement exceeding 2.2 mm (acquisition voxel size) were excluded from further analyses.

Fiber Tract identification and Quantification

White matter tract identification and quantification was done using the open-source tool automated fiber quantification (AFQ; Yeatman, Dougherty, Myall, et al. 2012) implemented in MATLAB 2016a. First, whole-brain deterministic tractography was performed using a streamlines tracking algorithm with a fourth-order Runge–Kutta path integration method and following parameters: step size = 1, FA threshold = 0.2, minimum length = 50 mm, maximum length = 250 mm, and angle = 30|${\kern0em }^{\circ }$|⁠. This and subsequent steps were performed using all acquired diffusion-weighted volumes from all shells. Next, a waypoint region of interest (ROI)-based tract segmentation was used to define white matter tracts (as described in Wakana et al. 2007), followed by tract refinement and cleaning (removal of fibers deviating more than 4 SD above mean fiber length or more than 5 SD from the fiber core). For a detailed description of the AFQ procedure, see Yeatman, Dougherty, Myall, et al. 2012. The resulting reconstructed tract was then resampled to 100 equidistant nodes spanning the full length of the tract and a weighted FA average was calculated at each node to create a tract profile (ordered anterior to posterior). The weighting procedure was such that fibers deviating from the fiber core count less than those closer to the fiber core. Because the first and the last nodes could not be reliably estimated in all tracts and subjects, they were not included in further analyses. Both the tract profile as well as summarized tract FA measures were analyzed.

The analysis focused on four bilateral pathways of interest defined a priori: the direct frontotemporal and anterior frontoparietal segments of the AF (AFdirect and AFanterior, respectively), the IFOF and the ILF (Vandermosten, Boets, Wouters, et al. 2012; Wandell and Yeatman 2013; Zhang et al. 2014). The investigation of both left- and right-hemispheric pathways is warranted by evidence that (1) reveals bilateral associations between white matter and early language and phonological skills (Vandermosten et al. 2015; Vanderauwera et al. 2018; Walton et al. 2018; Farah et al. 2020), and (2) suggests a potential role of right-hemispheric pathways in promoting better reading outcomes, by means of either compensatory (Hoeft et al. 2011) or protective mechanisms (Wang et al. 2017; Zuk et al. 2021). Here, it is important to clarify that the pathway corresponding to AFanterior in the present study, is referred to as SLF in AFQ, which is consistent with the terminology used in Wakana et al. (2007). Tract identification was performed in 150 datasets from a total sample of 83 subjects. The following tracts could not be identified in certain datasets using the chosen tensor-based tractography and AFQ procedure: left AFdirect (n = 7), right AFdirect (n = 34), left AFanterior (n = 3), right AFanterior (n = 2), left IFOF (n = 2), right IFOF (n = 4), left ILF (n = 0), and right ILF (n = 1). Notably, a higher number is observed for the right AFdirect (frontotemporal segment), which is largely consistent with previous research showing that this pathway may not be detectable in certain individuals, including studies in both children (Eluvathingal et al. 2007; Van Der Auwera et al. 2021) and adults (Catani et al. 2007; Lebel and Beaulieu 2009; Allendorfer et al. 2016). This inability to detect the right AFdirect has been attributed to limitations of deterministic algorithms in regions of crossing fibers (as is the case in the temporoparietal junction), rather than the anatomical absence of the tract from the brain (Yeatman et al. 2011).

Statistical Analyses

All reported analyses were performed in R (version 4.0.0) (R Core Team 2020). For all analyses, the alpha level was set at 0.05. First, we examined white matter risk-related differences prior to intervention. Note that in these pretest FA comparisons, for the purpose of assessing the effect of cognitive risk, the two risk groups were merged (i.e., GG-FL and AC groups, n = 52) and compared with the typically developing group (n = 26). To this end, average tract FA measures (i.e., tract average) and FA values at each node of the tract (i.e., tract profiles consisting of 98 nodes) at pretest were compared between the at-risk children and typically developing children using a linear model, while controlling for in-scanner subject motion.

Given the inherently correlated node structure within each tract profile, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparison corrections can be considered overly conservative and potentially lead to type-II errors. Therefore, a nonparametric permutation-based multiple testing correction was applied for along-tract analyses, as described in Nichols and Holmes (2002) and implemented in several tractometry papers (Yeatman, Dougherty, Myall, et al. 2012; Travis et al. 2017; Farah et al. 2020; Jossinger et al. 2021; Wasserthal et al. 2021). This approach provides the family-wise error (FWE) corrected alpha value for pointwise tests at a given tract, as well as a FWE-corrected cluster size threshold at the user-defined alpha of 0.05. Additional details on the implementation of this procedure are provided in the Supplementary Material.

Group differences were considered significant if they occurred at nodes where the P-value was lower than the FWE-corrected alpha, or if they occurred in a sufficient number of adjacent nodes as determined by the FWE-corrected cluster size, without the need for further P-value adjustment. For the baseline risk-related analysis, corrected alpha thresholds for the different tracts ranged from P < 0.001 to P < 0.003, while cluster-based thresholds ranged from 11 to 14 consecutive nodes.

Table 1

Group characteristics

Group
VariablesGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aTypical control (n = 27)aP-valueb
Sex (female)16/1511/1415/120.700
Familial risk (n)5100.035
Nonverbal intelligence102 (78–133)96 (79–126)101 (78–133)0.394
Handednessc0.857
Left/right/ambidextrous2/28/12/21/22/22/3
Age at pretest scan (months)64 (60–71)66 (60–72)66 (62–73)0.075
Age at post-test scan (months)69 (65–76)70 (65–76)70 (65–77)0.734
Socioeconomic statusc0.112
Low/middle/high/unknown7/12/12/07/13/4/13/10/14/0
Group
VariablesGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aTypical control (n = 27)aP-valueb
Sex (female)16/1511/1415/120.700
Familial risk (n)5100.035
Nonverbal intelligence102 (78–133)96 (79–126)101 (78–133)0.394
Handednessc0.857
Left/right/ambidextrous2/28/12/21/22/22/3
Age at pretest scan (months)64 (60–71)66 (60–72)66 (62–73)0.075
Age at post-test scan (months)69 (65–76)70 (65–76)70 (65–77)0.734
Socioeconomic statusc0.112
Low/middle/high/unknown7/12/12/07/13/4/13/10/14/0

aMedian (range) or occurrence (n).

bGroup differences were assessed using a Kruskal–Wallis test for age and nonverbal intelligence, a Pearson’s Chi-squared test for sex, handedness and maternal education, and a Fisher’s exact test for familial risk.

cAssessment based on maternal educational level (low = no extra degree after secondary school; middle = professional bachelor/academic bachelor; high = Master or PhD).

dBased on a subset of questions adapted from the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971).

Table 1

Group characteristics

Group
VariablesGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aTypical control (n = 27)aP-valueb
Sex (female)16/1511/1415/120.700
Familial risk (n)5100.035
Nonverbal intelligence102 (78–133)96 (79–126)101 (78–133)0.394
Handednessc0.857
Left/right/ambidextrous2/28/12/21/22/22/3
Age at pretest scan (months)64 (60–71)66 (60–72)66 (62–73)0.075
Age at post-test scan (months)69 (65–76)70 (65–76)70 (65–77)0.734
Socioeconomic statusc0.112
Low/middle/high/unknown7/12/12/07/13/4/13/10/14/0
Group
VariablesGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aTypical control (n = 27)aP-valueb
Sex (female)16/1511/1415/120.700
Familial risk (n)5100.035
Nonverbal intelligence102 (78–133)96 (79–126)101 (78–133)0.394
Handednessc0.857
Left/right/ambidextrous2/28/12/21/22/22/3
Age at pretest scan (months)64 (60–71)66 (60–72)66 (62–73)0.075
Age at post-test scan (months)69 (65–76)70 (65–76)70 (65–77)0.734
Socioeconomic statusc0.112
Low/middle/high/unknown7/12/12/07/13/4/13/10/14/0

aMedian (range) or occurrence (n).

bGroup differences were assessed using a Kruskal–Wallis test for age and nonverbal intelligence, a Pearson’s Chi-squared test for sex, handedness and maternal education, and a Fisher’s exact test for familial risk.

cAssessment based on maternal educational level (low = no extra degree after secondary school; middle = professional bachelor/academic bachelor; high = Master or PhD).

dBased on a subset of questions adapted from the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971).

Table 2

Training characteristics

Group
VariableGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aP-valueb
Training exposure (hours)16 (3–25)18 (6–28)0.315
Training period (weeks)12 (6–19)13 (10–20)0.483
Proportion of completed interventionc (%)91 (15–137)101 (32–158)0.315
Group
VariableGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aP-valueb
Training exposure (hours)16 (3–25)18 (6–28)0.315
Training period (weeks)12 (6–19)13 (10–20)0.483
Proportion of completed interventionc (%)91 (15–137)101 (32–158)0.315

aMedian (range).

bGroup differences were assessed using a Wilcoxon rank sum test.

cThe proportion of completed intervention is calculated as the number of hours spent on-task divided by the total amount of hours they were instructed to play during 12 weeks (estimated at 18 h).

Table 2

Training characteristics

Group
VariableGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aP-valueb
Training exposure (hours)16 (3–25)18 (6–28)0.315
Training period (weeks)12 (6–19)13 (10–20)0.483
Proportion of completed interventionc (%)91 (15–137)101 (32–158)0.315
Group
VariableGraphoGame-Flemish (n = 31)aActive control (n = 25)aP-valueb
Training exposure (hours)16 (3–25)18 (6–28)0.315
Training period (weeks)12 (6–19)13 (10–20)0.483
Proportion of completed interventionc (%)91 (15–137)101 (32–158)0.315

aMedian (range).

bGroup differences were assessed using a Wilcoxon rank sum test.

cThe proportion of completed intervention is calculated as the number of hours spent on-task divided by the total amount of hours they were instructed to play during 12 weeks (estimated at 18 h).

To assess changes in white matter over time and intervention effects, average tract FA values (i.e., tract average) were entered in linear mixed-effect models (LMMs) with fixed-effects of group (GG-FL, AC, typical control), session (pretest, post-test), and their interaction, as well as by-subject random intercepts. The same model specification was then used to assess these factors at each node (i.e., tract profiles) to identify clusters of nodes with similar effects. In all reported LMMs, in-scanner subject motion was included as a nuisance predictor. All LMMs were fitted using lmer via the R package lme4 (Bates et al. 2015), and F-statistics and P-values were obtained by calculating type-III analysis of variance tables using Satterthwaite’s degrees-of-freedom approximation (R package lmerTest, Kuznetsova et al. 2017). In order to determine the appropriate P-value or cluster size for the longitudinal analysis, the same permutation-based approach as the one described above was applied. For the intervention analysis, corrected alpha thresholds for the different tracts ranged from P < 0.001 to < 0.002, while cluster-based thresholds ranged from 8 to 11 consecutive nodes.

Effect sizes for all pretest comparisons are reported as Cohen’s d values, while effect sizes for LMM main effects and interactions are reported as Cohen’s |$f$| values (Cohen 1988).

Data Availability

Anonymized data and code to reproduce the figures and findings of this study are publicly available at https://github.com/meconomou/Economou2021_wmintervention.

Results

Participant and group characteristics

Descriptive group statistics are summarized in Table 1. The three groups did not differ significantly with respect to nonverbal intelligence, sex, handedness, socioeconomic status, or age at either MRI visit. More children in the GG-FL group had a familial risk for dyslexia; however, this was not controlled for further given the very low number of occurrences in our sample. A Kruskal–Wallis test revealed no group differences in head motion at either timepoint (pretest: H(2) = 0.29, P = 0.865; post-test: H(2) = 0.96, P = 0.618). There was, however, a main effect of session (F(1,74) = 11.12, P = 0.001), such that less head movement was observed at posttest compared to pretest. As summarized in Table 2, both intervention groups were overall comparable in terms of training duration and total game exposure. Most children played at least 90% of the intended amount (estimated at 18 h) and successful completion of the intervention took slightly longer than the intended 12-week period.

Tract FA profiles before the intervention. Each plot shows the mean tract FA profile ±1 standard error of the mean for the two groups (at-risk and typical control). Bold black panel borders indicate the tracts where global tract FA differences were found. Localized risk-related differences are indicated by gray-shaded regions on the tract profile. The red arrows indicate the position of the corresponding anatomical locations, which are visualized in black on three-dimensional tract renderings next to each plot.
Figure 2

Tract FA profiles before the intervention. Each plot shows the mean tract FA profile ±1 standard error of the mean for the two groups (at-risk and typical control). Bold black panel borders indicate the tracts where global tract FA differences were found. Localized risk-related differences are indicated by gray-shaded regions on the tract profile. The red arrows indicate the position of the corresponding anatomical locations, which are visualized in black on three-dimensional tract renderings next to each plot.

Risk-Related Differences in White Matter Properties Before the Intervention

Tract Average

Here we examined the differences in FA between at-risk and typically developing children (Fig. 2). Overall, the at-risk group had significantly lower FA in the left AFdirect (β = −0.01, t = −2.02, P = 0.047, d = 0.53), left IFOF (β = −0.01, t = −2.15, P = 0.035, d = 0.62), and right IFOF (β = −0.01, t = −2.23, P = 0.029, d = 0.59), but this difference did not reach significance in the right AFdirect (β = −0.02, t = −1.97, P = 0.054, d = 0.55). We did not find any evidence for risk-related differences in global tract FA in bilateral AFanterior (left: β = −0.01, t = −1.52, P = 0.132, d = 0.38; right: β = −0.01, t = −1.13, P = 0.262, d = 0.32) or bilateral ILF (left: β = −0.01, t = −1.56, P = 0.123, d = 0.40; right: β = −0.01, t = −0.97, P = 0.335, d = 0.23). Tracts for which group differences were found in the tract average are depicted with bold black panel borders in Figure 2.

Tract Profiles

The results of the tract profile analysis are shown in Figure 2. Based on the permutation-based multiple testing correction described in the Methods section, significant group differences between at-risk and typically developing children were observed in localized node clusters of both dorsal and ventral pathways. Lower FA for the at-risk group was found in the right AFdirect (nodes 11–13, average d = 0.47), right ILF (nodes 8–9, average d = 0.30), and right IFOF (node 37, d = 0.58). These locations are visualized as red bands on individual tract renderings and as gray-shaded bars on the tract profile plots in Figure 2. No further localized group differences were found in the left AFdirect, left ILF, left IFOF, and bilateral AFanterior. Along-tract effect sizes for these group comparisons are visualized in Supplementary Figure 1. Note that the analyses on the right AFdirect have been conducted with a considerably smaller sample size compared to the other tracts, due to the high number of failed reconstructions. As a result, it is possible that findings concerning the right AFdirect might not be as reliable.

Training Effects on White Matter Properties

Tract Average

A main effect of session (pretest, post-test) was present in several pathways, which was always positive, indicating a systematic increase in FA over time. This effect was significant in the left AFdirect (F(1,63) = 8.09, P = 0.006, |$f$| = 0.36), left AFanterior (F(1,68) = 5.34, P = 0.024, |$f$| = 0.28), left IFOF (F(1,69) = 4.75, P = 0.033, |$f$| = 0.26), and left ILF (F(1,76) = 5.86, P = 0.018, |$f$| = 0.28), but did not reach significance in the right AFdirect (F(1,51) = 3.96, P = 0.052, |$f$| = 0.28), right AFanterior (F(1,70) = 2.16, P = 0.146, |$f$| = 0.18), right IFOF (F(1,70) = 3.86, P = 0.053, |$f$| = 0.23), or right ILF (F(1,72) = 2.04, P = 0.158, |$f$| = 0.17). This analysis revealed no evidence of intervention-related group differences in global tract FA, as indicated by the absence of significant group-by-session interactions in global FA for any of the tracts (right AFanterior: F(2, 67) = 2.65, P = 0.078; all other P > 0.213). The individual tract average FA changes across sessions are shown in Supplementary Figure 2.

Tract Profiles

Longitudinal changes in FA were subsequently examined at each node along the pathways, comparing the three groups. A main effect of session was present in the left AFanterior (nodes 44–57, average |$f$| = 0.10), such that an increase in FA was observed from pretest to posttest. Similar to the global tract average analysis, no significant group-by-session interactions were found. Along-tract effect sizes for the session main effect and the group-by-session interaction effect are visualized in Supplementary Figures 3 and 4, respectively.

Discussion

The present study examined white matter properties in prereading kindergarten children with a cognitive risk for developing dyslexia during an early literacy intervention. The main aim of this study was to investigate the effects of preventive training on white matter development. To this end, at-risk children were assigned to either a digital phonics-based training (GG-FL group) or engaged with other games unrelated to reading (AC group). Before the intervention, we found evidence of global and local risk-related differences in bilateral dorsal and ventral tracts, suggesting that white matter alterations related to dyslexia predate the formal onset of reading. Our longitudinal analysis revealed that while white matter properties changed from pre- to post-test, no evidence was found to support literacy intervention in more detail in the following paragraphs.

Concerning risk-related white matter differences before the intervention, we observed lower FA in bilateral dorsal and ventral pathways in children who performed poorly on reading-related precursors, when compared to typically developing children. More specifically, global average FA differences were found in the left AFdirect, left IFOF, and right IFOF, as well as more localized differences in the right AFdirect, right IFOF, and right ILF. Our findings are consistent with recent evidence reporting prereading differences in children with (a familial risk for) dyslexia (Langer et al. 2017; Vanderauwera et al. 2017; Wang et al. 2017). Together, these observations support the view that early underlying anatomical differences represent a neurobiological predisposition for struggling to read and are not purely driven by reading difficulties.

With respect to location, previous studies in adults and school-aged children have predominantly emphasized the role of left-hemispheric pathways in skilled reading and dyslexia (Yeatman et al. 2011; Vandermosten, Boets, Poelmans, et al. 2012; Zhang et al. 2014). In the present study, our analyses indicate additional risk-related differences in the right homologues, revealing a more bilateral pattern. This is less commonly reported in older children and adults, but is largely in line with prereading evidence supporting more bilateral involvement in younger children (for a review, see Vandermosten et al. 2016). This bilateral involvement might shift toward a left-lateralized pattern within the first few years of formal reading instruction and/or once dyslexia can be diagnosed; however, additional follow-up assessments would be needed to specifically investigate this hemispheric specialization within our sample.

In examining these prereading differences, a tractometry approach was utilized to map FA along the trajectory of white matter tracts (Bells et al. 2011; Yeatman, Dougherty, Myall, et al. 2012; Chamberland et al. 2019; Chandio et al. 2020). Such a fine-grained approach may be more appropriate in investigating regional variations in FA than using global average tract measures, especially given the underlying influence of local anatomical features on FA estimation. Indeed, studies have demonstrated that FA varies significantly along the course of white matter tracts, with these variation patterns remaining relatively consistent across subjects (Yeatman, Dougherty, Myall, et al. 2012; Johnson et al. 2014; Farah et al. 2020). This observation is also confirmed by the present data (see tract profile visualization in Fig. 2).

Notably, both global tract FA differences (in left AFdirect and bilateral IFOF) as well as localized FA differences (in right AFdirect, right ILF and right IFOF) were associated with the presence of a cognitive risk for dyslexia in our study. These localized group differences were identified following a rigorous permutation-based approach to control the FWE rate (Nichols and Holmes 2002) and were observed only in small clusters, ranging from one to three nodes. It is important to clarify here that the number of nodes analyzed, as well as the approach for determining the portion of the tract that will be analyzed, vary considerably among studies. As a result, the size of reported clusters is often not directly comparable. With this in mind, our results are in agreement with previously reported cluster sizes in the literature (see, e.g., Wang et al. 2017; Banfi et al. 2019), although it remains uncertain how anatomically meaningful these small clusters are.

In order to aid interpretation of our findings, effect sizes are provided for the risk-related comparisons as Cohen’s d values (see Supplementary Fig. 1). Based on suggested thresholds (Cohen 1988), a number of along-tract moderate effects are observed in bilateral AFdirect, bilateral ILF, and right IFOF. Despite these moderate effects, the localized group differences in the left AFdirect and left ILF were not identified as significant following correction for multiple comparisons. It is important not to entirely dismiss these results, as they provide relevant insights on the practical implications of our findings, which are independent of significance and sample size (Sullivan and Feinn 2012). Moreover, reporting effect sizes enables comparison of similar effects across studies, which is preferred over solely relying on arbitrary thresholds for interpretation (Lakens 2013).

After 12 weeks of training, an effect of session was observed in several tracts, indicating an increase in FA over the course of the intervention. The absence of any group-by-session interaction, however, suggests that this increase is not uniquely related to the early literacy training and likely reflects developmental effects. This finding is consistent with previous reports of significant FA development in early childhood (Reynolds et al. 2019; Dimond et al. 2020). Although the available studies capture these developmental FA changes in the course of years, our results point out that changes in shorter timescales (i.e., a few months) may also be detectable at this age.

The absence of longitudinal GraphoGame (GG)-specific changes in FA may suggest that the training provided in this study is not associated with measurable changes in white matter. This interpretation, however, warrants caution. Currently available evidence from reading intervention studies is limited and findings are mixed regarding the location and magnitude of training-induced white matter changes. Following intensive reading instruction, Keller and Just (2009) reported a localized FA increase in the left anterior centrum semiovale, whereas Huber et al. (2018) described widespread white matter growth during an intensive reading intervention, including changes in the reading network that we investigated. In contrast to both of these studies and therefore more in line with the findings of the current study, Partanen et al. (2021) reported no measurable effects of a three-month reading intervention on white matter properties of dyslexic readers.

Several factors contribute to the observed variability among studies, including intervention-related aspects or divergence in study design and analysis. For instance, interventions that target specific aspects of reading acquisition may lead to more regional changes, as was the case in the study of Keller and Just (2009) (and might be less straightforward to detect with a region-of-interest analysis). On the other hand, widespread plasticity may be a reflection of the general learning experience resulting from receiving multicomponential training, as was the case in Huber et al. (2018). Similarly, the inclusion of an AC group (such as in our study and that of Keller and Just 2009) or lack thereof (such as in Huber et al. 2018; Partanen et al. 2021) might yield different patterns of intervention effects. A recent meta-analysis has shown that the lack of randomization or comparison with a business-as-usual group instead of an AC group, has an impact on estimated effect sizes in early literacy intervention research (Verhoeven et al. 2020). As such, research design is a very relevant factor to consider when interpreting (null) intervention effects. Intervention characteristics such as intensity and exposure are also likely to play a key role in explaining the findings of the current study. Both Keller and Just (2009) and Huber et al. (2018) report more than a 100 h of intervention received by the participants, which is markedly higher than the total intervention exposure in our study and could explain why we did not find an effect on white matter properties. The intervention reported in the current study led to improvements in letter sound knowledge and word decoding (Vanden Bempt et al. 2021). Yet, given that extensive interventions comprising 100 or more sessions are put forward as the standard for effective remediation (Wanzek and Vaughn 2007), the training provided in our study might have only induced behavioral effects. Lastly, the role of the statistical framework used in the present study needs to be addressed when drawing conclusions about the findings. Other methodologically similar studies often use less conservative approaches to adjust for multiple along-tract comparisons, compared to the approach described in the current study. The methodological choice can have an influence on both the results and the conclusions and therefore should be factored in when interpreting findings within and across studies. For completeness, uncorrected P-values for the analyses are shown in Supplementary Figures 57.

A final consideration when discussing the present findings and the variation among reported intervention results, concerns developmental effects. While some evidence exists to support plasticity in school-aged children following reading interventions, less is known about white matter changes during early (preschool) interventions. In the present study, we specifically investigate the prereading brain and therefore focus on structural changes during the sensitive period of learning to read (Ozernov-Palchik and Gaab 2016), whereas the studies described above examine intervention mechanisms in later stages of reading development. Hence, it might be that the impact of such intervention on white matter is more profound after the onset of primary school compared to the prereading stage. Another possibility is that the timing of the intervention provided in our study might be associated with other structural changes besides white matter. For instance, recent evidence shows that cortical gray matter volume in left temporal and temporoparietal regions linked to reading is still increasing within the first few years of primary school (Phan et al. 2021). This provides an indication that gray matter structure is amenable to (intervention-induced) plasticity during early childhood.

A number of limitations are recognized in the present study. First, while FA has been extensively used in studies of reading- and dyslexia-related differences (Ben-Shachar et al. 2007; Vandermosten, Boets, Wouters, et al. 2012; Vandermosten, Boets, Poelmans, et al. 2012) and has been established as a valuable index of neuroplasticity (Sagi et al. 2012; Zatorre et al. 2012), its biological interpretation remains limited. Due to the demonstrably low specificity of FA for a single neurobiological process, the underlying neural changes that might influence FA change cannot be understood based on this index alone. Access to complementary information about underlying tissue architecture, ideally from multimodal investigations, would be an advantage of future studies and could uncover a different pattern of results. Recent evidence mapping intervention-related white matter changes to more specific neurobiological processes (Huber et al. 2021) further corroborates this view. A second consideration pertains to the cohort sample size, despite best efforts and a large-scale screening in more than 1000 prereaders. This is especially relevant for our analyses in the right AFdirect, where lower FA was observed in at-risk children. Given that the right AFdirect was not successfully reconstructed in a relatively large subset of participants (30%), this finding is potentially less robust and should be interpreted with caution. Owing to similar constraints, exploring individual differences was not possible and we thus report only intervention effects at the group level. The sample size, dropout and data quality are undoubtedly major practical challenges of longitudinal neuroimaging studies in young children up to the age of five, such as the present one (Turesky et al. 2021). They nevertheless advance our understanding of intervention mechanisms during this sensitive period of early reading acquisition.

To summarize, here we report on a longitudinal investigation of white matter properties in 5-year-old kindergarten children following an early literacy training. Our findings extend previous research supporting an early link between structural white matter alterations and a cognitive risk for developing reading difficulties. The results provide evidence of an overall change in white matter properties within the course of the intervention, however no specific training-related effects were observed. The present study is one of the first to investigate the neurocognitive mechanisms of preventive intervention in young pre-reading children. Longitudinal follow-up of the children’s reading skills will help clarify the long-lasting impact of preventive training, which, in turn, has the potential to inform future remediation strategies for at-risk readers at a time when they are most beneficial.

Funding

Research Council of KU Leuven (C14/17/046). J.V. was a postdoctoral fellow of the Research Foundation Flanders (12T4818N).

Notes

We would like to thank Cara Verwimp, Fran Vanfleteren, Klara Schevenels, Lauren Blockmans, Justine Soetaert, Caitlan Nys, Valerie Geerits, and Aymara Taillieu for their assistance with MRI protocol preparation and data collection. We are grateful to Ron Peeters for technical MRI support. We are most thankful to all the participating families and children without whom this research would not be possible. Conflict of Interest: None declared.

References

Allendorfer
 
JB
,
Hernando
 
KA
,
Hossain
 
S
,
Nenert
 
R
,
Holland
 
SK
,
Szaflarski
 
JP
.
2016
.
Arcuate fasciculus asymmetry has a hand in language function but not handedness
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
37
(
9
):
3297
3309
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23241.

Andersson
 
JLR
,
Sotiropoulos
 
SN
.
2016
.
An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging
.
NeuroImage
.
125
:
1063
1078
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019.

Andersson
 
JLR
,
Skare
 
S
,
Ashburner
 
J
.
2003
.
How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging
.
NeuroImage
.
20
(
2
):
870
888
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7.

Banfi
 
C
,
Koschutnig
 
K
,
Moll
 
K
,
Schulte-Körne
 
G
,
Fink
 
A
,
Landerl
 
K
.
2019
.
White matter alterations and tract lateralization in children with dyslexia and isolated spelling deficits
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
40
(
3
):
765
776
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24410.

Bates
 
D
,
Mächler
 
M
,
Bolker
 
B
,
Walker
 
S
.
2015
.
Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4
.
J Stat Softw
.
67
(
1
):
1
48
. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.

Bells
 
S
,
Cercignani
 
M
,
Deoni
 
S
,
Assaf
 
Y
.
2011
.
“Tractometry” – comprehensive multi-modal quantitative assessment of white matter along specific tracts
.
Proc Int Soc Magn Reson Med
.
19
(
2009
):
678
 .

Ben-Shachar
 
M
,
Dougherty
 
RF
,
Wandell
 
BA
.
2007
.
White matter pathways in reading
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
.
17
(
2
):
258
270
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.006.

Borchers
 
LR
,
Bruckert
 
L
,
Dodson
 
CK
,
Travis
 
KE
,
Marchman
 
VA
,
Shachar
 
MB
,
Feldman
 
HM
.
2019
.
Microstructural properties of white matter pathways in relation to subsequent reading abilities in children: a longitudinal analysis
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
224
(
2
):
891
905
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-018-1813-z.

Caravolas
 
M
,
Lervåg
 
A
,
Mousikou
 
P
,
Efrim
 
C
,
Litavský
 
M
,
Onochie-Quintanilla
 
E
,
Salas
 
N
,
Schöffelová
 
M
,
Defior
 
S
,
Mikulajová
 
M
, et al.  
2012
.
Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies
.
Psychol Sci
.
23
(
6
):
678
686
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434536.

Catani
 
M
,
Jones
 
DK
,
Ffytche
 
DH
.
2005
.
Perisylvian language networks of the human brain
.
Ann Neurol
.
57
(
1
):
8
16
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.20319.

Catani
 
M
,
Allin
 
MPG
,
Husain
 
M
,
Pugliese
 
L
,
Mesulam
 
MM
,
Murray
 
RM
,
Jones
 
DK
.
2007
.
Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
104
(
43
):
17163
17168
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0702116104.

Chamberland
 
M
,
Raven
 
EP
,
Genc
 
S
,
Duffy
 
K
,
Descoteaux
 
M
,
Parker
 
GD
,
Tax
 
CMW
,
Jones
 
DK
.
2019
.
Dimensionality reduction of diffusion MRI measures for improved tractometry of the human brain
.
NeuroImage
.
200
(
March
):
89
100
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.020.

Chandio
 
BQ
,
Risacher
 
SL
,
Pestilli
 
F
,
Bullock
 
D
,
Yeh
 
FC
,
Koudoro
 
S
,
Rokem
 
A
,
Harezlak
 
J
,
Garyfallidis
 
E
.
2020
.
Bundle analytics, a computational framework for investigating the shapes and profiles of brain pathways across populations
.
Sci Rep
.
10
(
1
):
1
18
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-74054-4.

Cohen
 
J
.
1988
.
Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
. 2nd ed.
Hillsdale, N.J.L.
:
Erlbaum Associates
.

Coppock
 
A
.
2019
.
Randomizr: easy-to-use tools for common forms of random assignment and sampling
. .

Dehaene
 
S
,
Cohen
 
L
.
2011
.
The unique role of the visual word form area in reading
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
15
(
6
):
254
262
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.&2011.04.003.

Dimond
 
D
,
Rohr
 
CS
,
Smith
 
RE
,
Dhollander
 
T
,
Cho
 
I
,
Lebel
 
C
,
Dewey
 
D
,
Connelly
 
A
,
Bray
 
S
.
2020
.
Early childhood development of white matter fiber density and morphology
.
NeuroImage
.
210
(
October 2019
):
116552
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116552.

Dougherty
 
C
.
2003
.
Numeracy, literacy and earnings: evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth
.
Econ Educ Rev
.
22
(
5
):
511
521
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7757(03)00040-2.

Ekerdt
 
CEM
,
Kühn
 
C
,
Anwander
 
A
,
Brauer
 
J
,
Friederici
 
AD
.
2020
.
Word learning reveals white matter plasticity in preschool children
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
225
(
2
):
607
619
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02024-7.

Eluvathingal
 
TJ
,
Hasan
 
KM
,
Larry
 
K
,
Fletcher
 
JM
,
Ewing-Cobbs
 
L
.
2007
.
Quantitative diffusion tensor tractography of association and projection fibers in normally developing children and adolescents
.
Cereb Cortex
.
17
(
12
):
2760
2768
. .

Farah
 
R
,
Tzafrir
 
H
,
Horowitz-Kraus
 
T
.
2020
.
Association between diffusivity measures and language and cognitive-control abilities from early toddler’s age to childhood
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
225
(
3
):
1103
1122
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02062-1.

Ferrer
 
E
,
Shaywitz
 
BA
,
Holahan
 
JM
,
Marchione
 
KE
,
Michaels
 
R
,
Shaywitz
 
SE
.
2015
.
Achievement gap in reading is present as early as first grade and persists through adolescence
.
J Pediatr
.
167
(
5
):
1121
1125.e2
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.07.045.

Forkel
 
SJ
,
Thiebaut de Schotten
 
M
,
Kawadler
 
JM
,
Dell’Acqua
 
F
,
Danek
 
A
,
Catani
 
M
.
2014
.
The anatomy of fronto-occipital connections from early blunt dissections to contemporary tractography
.
Cortex
.
56
(
iii
):
73
84
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.005.

Galuschka
 
K
,
Ise
 
E
,
Krick
 
K
,
Schulte-Körne
 
G
.
2014
.
Effectiveness of treatment approaches for children and adolescents with reading disabilities: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials
.
PLoS One
.
9
(
2
):e89900. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089900.

Gebauer
 
D
,
Fink
 
A
,
Filippini
 
N
,
Johansen-Berg
 
H
,
Reishofer
 
G
,
Koschutnig
 
K
,
Kargl
 
R
,
Purgstaller
 
C
,
Fazekas
 
F
,
Enzinger
 
C
.
2012
.
Differences in integrity of white matter and changes with training in spelling impaired children: a diffusion tensor imaging study
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
217
(
3
):
747
760
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-011-0371-4.Differences.

Glatz
 
T
,
Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Bempt
 
FV
,
Economou
 
M
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2022
.
Design and structure of GraphoGame-Flemish, an app-based tool to support early reading acquisition
. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/CYKED.

Grills-Taquechel
 
AE
,
Fletcher
 
JM
,
Vaughn
 
SR
,
Stuebing
 
KK
.
2012
.
Anxiety and reading difficulties in early elementary school: evidence for unidirectional- or bi-directional relations?
 
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev
.
43
(
1
):
35
47
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-011-0246-1.

Gullick
 
MM
,
Booth
 
JR
.
2014
.
Individual differences in crossmodal brain activity predict arcuate fasciculus connectivity in developing readers
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
26
(
7
):
1331
1346
. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.

Hatcher
 
PJ
,
Hulme
 
C
,
Snowling
 
MJ
.
2004
.
Explicit phoneme training with phonic reading instructions helps young children at risk
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
43
:
338
358
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00225.x.

Hoeft
 
F
,
McCandliss
 
BD
,
Black
 
JM
,
Gantman
 
A
,
Zakerani
 
N
,
Hulme
 
C
,
Lyytinen
 
H
,
Whitfield-Gabrieli
 
S
,
Glover
 
GH
,
Reiss
 
AL
, et al.  
2011
.
Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci
.
108
(
1
):
361
366
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1008950108.

Huber
 
E
,
Donnelly
 
PM
,
Rokem
 
A
,
Yeatman
 
JD
.
2018
.
Rapid and widespread white matter plasticity during an intensive reading intervention
.
Nat Commun
.
9
(
1
):
1
13
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04627-5.

Huber
 
E
,
Mezer
 
A
,
Yeatman
 
JD
.
2021
.
Neurobiological underpinnings of rapid white matter plasticity during intensive reading instruction
.
NeuroImage
.
243
(
August
):
118453
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118453.

Jenkinson
 
M
,
Beckmann
 
CF
,
Behrens
 
TEJ
,
Woolrich
 
MW
,
Smith
 
SM
.
2012
.
FSL
.
NeuroImage
.
62
:
782
790
. .

Johnson
 
RT
,
Yeatman
 
JD
,
Wandell
 
BA
,
Buonocore
 
MH
,
Amaral
 
DG
,
Nordahl
 
CW
.
2014
.
Diffusion properties of major white matter tracts in young, typically developing children
.
NeuroImage
.
88
:
143
154
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.025.Diffusion.

Jolles
 
D
,
Wassermann
 
D
,
Chokhani
 
R
,
Richardson
 
J
,
Tenison
 
C
,
Bammer
 
R
,
Fuchs
 
L
,
Supekar
 
K
,
Menon
 
V
.
2016
.
Plasticity of left Perisylvian white-matter tracts is associated with individual differences in math learning
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
221
(
3
):
1337
1351
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0975-6.

Jossinger
 
S
,
Kronfeld
 
V
,
Avital
 
D
,
Ofer
 
Z
,
Michal
 
A
,
Shachar
 
B
.
2021
.
Speech rate association with cerebellar white - matter diffusivity in adults with persistent developmental stuttering
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
226
:801–816. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02210-7.

Keller
 
TA
,
Just
 
MA
.
2009
.
Altering cortical connectivity: remediation-induced changes in the white matter of poor readers
.
Neuron
.
64
(
5
):
624
631
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.018.

Kuznetsova
 
A
,
Brockhoff
 
PB
,
Christensen
 
RHB
.
2017
.
lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models
.
J Stat Softw
.
82
(
13
):
1
26
. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13.

Lakens
 
D
.
2013
.
Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs
.
Front Psychol
.
4
(
NOV
):
1
12
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863.

Langer
 
N
,
Peysakhovich
 
B
,
Zuk
 
J
,
Drottar
 
M
,
Sliva
 
DD
,
Smith
 
S
,
Becker
 
BLC
,
Grant
 
PE
,
Gaab
 
N
.
2017
.
White matter alterations in infants at risk for developmental dyslexia
.
Cereb Cortex
.
27
(
2
):
1027
1036
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv281.

Lebel
 
C
,
Beaulieu
 
C
.
2009
.
Lateralization of the arcuate fasciculus from childhood to adulthood and its relation to cognitive abilities in children
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
30
(
11
):
3563
3573
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20779.

Lebel
 
C
,
Shaywitz
 
B
,
Holahan
 
J
,
Shaywitz
 
S
,
Marchione
 
K
,
Beaulieu
 
C
.
2013
.
Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of reading ability in dysfluent and non-impaired readers
.
Brain Lang
.
125
(
2
):
215
222
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.009.

Lonigan
 
CJ
,
Purpura
 
DJ
,
Wilson
 
SB
,
Walker
 
PM
,
Clancy-Menchetti
 
J
.
2013
.
Evaluating the components of an emergent literacy intervention for preschool children at risk for reading difficulties
.
J Exp Child Psychol
.
114
(
1
):
111
130
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.08.010.

Nichols
 
TE
,
Holmes
 
AP
.
2002
.
Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
15
(
1
):
1
25
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.1058.

Oldfield RC.

1971
.
The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory
.
Neuropsychologia
.
9
(
1
):
97
113
.

Ozernov-Palchik
 
O
,
Gaab
 
N
.
2016
.
Tackling the ‘dyslexia paradox’: reading brain and behavior for early markers of developmental dyslexia
.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci
.
7
(
2
):
156
176
. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1383.

Partanen
 
M
,
Kim
 
DHC
,
Rauscher
 
A
,
Siegel
 
LS
,
Giaschi
 
DE
.
2021
.
White matter but not grey matter predicts change in reading skills after intervention
.
Dyslexia
.
27
:
224
244
. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1668.

Peterson
 
RL
,
Pennington
 
BF
.
2015
.
Developmental dyslexia
.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol
.
11
(
1
):
283
307
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842.

Phan
 
TV
,
Sima
 
D
,
Smeets
 
D
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Vandermosten
 
M
.
2021
.
Structural brain dynamics across reading development: a longitudinal MRI study from kindergarten to grade 5
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
42
(
14
):
4497
4509
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25560.

Pugh
 
KR
,
Mencl
 
WE
,
Jenner
 
AR
,
Katz
 
L
,
Frost
 
SJ
,
Lee
 
JR
,
Shaywitz
 
SE
,
Shaywitz
 
BA
.
2001
.
Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability
.
J Commun Disord
.
34
(
6
):
479
492
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(01)00060-0.

Puolakanaho
 
A
,
Ahonen
 
T
,
Aro
 
M
,
Eklund
 
K
,
Leppänen
 
PHT
,
Poikkeus
 
AM
,
Tolvanen
 
A
,
Torppa
 
M
,
Lyytinen
 
H
.
2007
.
Very early phonological and language skills: estimating individual risk of reading disability
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl
.
48
(
9
):
923
931
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01763.x.

R Core Team
.
2020
.
R: a language and environment for statistical computing
.
Vienna, Austria
:
R Foundation for Statistical Computing
, .

Raschle
 
NM
,
Lee
 
M
,
Buechler
 
R
,
Christodoulou
 
JA
,
Chang
 
M
,
Vakil
 
M
,
Stering
 
PL
,
Gaab
 
N
.
2009
.
Making MR imaging child’s play - Pediatric neuroimaging protocol, guidelines and procedure
.
J Vis Exp
.
29
:
1
5
. https://doi.org/10.3791/1309.

Raven
 
JC
,
Court
 
JH
,
Raven
 
J
.
1984
.
Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales
.
London
:
Lewis
.

Reynolds
 
JE
,
Grohs
 
MN
,
Dewey
 
D
,
Lebel
 
C
.
2019
.
Global and regional white matter development in early childhood
.
NeuroImage
.
196
(
March
):
49
58
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.004.

Richardson
 
U
,
Lyytinen
 
H
.
2014
.
The GRAPHOGAME method: the theoretical and methodological background of the technology-enhanced learning environment for learning to read
.
Hum Technol – Interdiscipl J Hum ICT Environ
.
10
(
1
):
39
60
. https://doi.org/10.17011/ht/urn.201405281859.

Romeo
 
RR
,
Christodoulou
 
JA
,
Halverson
 
KK
,
Murtagh
 
J
,
Cyr
 
AB
,
Schimmel
 
C
,
Chang
 
P
,
Hook
 
PE
,
Gabrieli
 
JDE
.
2018
.
Socioeconomic status and reading disability: neuroanatomy and plasticity in response to intervention
.
Cereb Cortex
.
28
(
7
):
2297
2312
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx131.

Sagi
 
Y
,
Tavor
 
I
,
Hofstetter
 
S
,
Tzur-Moryosef
 
S
,
Blumenfeld-Katzir
 
T
,
Assaf
 
Y
.
2012
.
Learning in the fast lane: new insights into neuroplasticity
.
Neuron
.
73
(
6
):
1195
1203
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.025.

Saur
 
D
,
Kreher
 
BW
,
Schnell
 
S
,
Kümmerera
 
D
,
Kellmeyera
 
P
,
Vrya
 
MS
,
Umarova
 
R
,
Musso
 
M
,
Glauche
 
V
,
Abel
 
S
, et al.  
2008
.
Ventral and dorsal pathways for language
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
105
(
46
):
18035
18040
. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0805234105.

Saygin
 
ZM
,
Norton
 
ES
,
Osher
 
DE
,
Beach
 
SD
,
Cyr
 
AB
,
Ozernov-Palchik
 
O
,
Yendiki
 
A
,
Fischl
 
B
,
Gaab
 
N
,
Gabrieli
 
JDE
.
2013
.
Tracking the roots of reading ability: white matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children
.
J Neurosci
.
33
(
33
):
13251
13258
. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013.

Schlaggar
 
BL
,
McCandliss
 
BD
.
2007
.
Development of neural systems for reading
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
.
30
:
475
503
. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135645.

Smidts
 
DP
,
Oosterlaan
 
J
.
2007
.
GvK, Gedragsvragenlijst voor Kleuters: Handleiding
.
Amsterdam
:
Harcourt Test Publishers
.

Smith
 
SM
.
2002
.
Fast robust automated brain extraction
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
17
(
3
):
143
155
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062.

Smith
 
SM
,
Jenkinson
 
M
,
Woolrich
 
MW
,
Beckmann
 
CF
,
Behrens
 
TEJ
,
Johansen-Berg
 
H
,
Bannister
 
PR
,
De Luca
 
M
,
Drobnjak
 
I
,
Flitney
 
DE
, et al.  
2004
.
Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL
.
NeuroImage
.
23
(
Suppl. 1
):
208
219
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051.

Snowling
 
MJ
,
Hulme
 
C
.
2011
.
Evidence-based interventions for reading and language difficulties: creating a virtuous circle
.
Br J Educ Psychol
.
81
(
1
):
1
23
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02014.x.

Snowling
 
MJ
,
Hulme
 
C
.
2012
.
Interventions for children’s language and literacy difficulties
.
Int J Lang Commun Disord
.
47
(
1
):
27
34
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00081.x.

Stanovich
 
KE
.
1986
.
Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy
.
Read Res Q
.
21
(
4
):
360
407
. https://doi.org/10.1598/rrq.21.4.1.

Sullivan
 
GM
,
Feinn
 
R
.
2012
.
Using effect size—or why the P value is not enough
.
J Graduate Med Educ
.
4
(
3
):
279
282
. https://doi.org/10.4300/jgme-d-12-00156.1.

Theys
 
C
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2014
.
Diffusion tensor imaging and resting-state functional MRI-scanning in 5- and 6-year-old children: training protocol and motion assessment
.
PLoS One
.
9
(
4
):
1
7
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094019.

Travis
 
KE
,
Adams
 
JN
,
Kovachy
 
VN
,
Ben-Shachar
 
M
,
Feldman
 
HM
.
2017
.
White matter properties differ in 6-year old readers and pre-readers
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
222
(
4
):
1685
1703
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-016-1302-1.

Turesky
 
TK
,
Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Gaab
 
N
.
2021
.
Imaging the rapidly developing brain: current challenges for MRI studies in the first five years of life
.
Dev Cogn Neurosci.
 
47
:
100893
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100893.

Valas
 
H
.
1999
.
Students with learning disabilities and low-achieving students: peer acceptance, loneliness, self-esteem, and depression
.
Soc Psychol Educ
.
3
(
3
):
173
192
. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009626828789.

Van Der Auwera
 
S
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
,
Vanderauwera
 
J
.
2021
.
A three-time point longitudinal investigation of the arcuate fasciculus throughout reading acquisition in children developing dyslexia
.
NeuroImage
.
237
(
October 2020
):
118087
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118087.

Van Herck
 
S
,
Bempt
 
FV
,
Economou
 
M
,
Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Glatz
 
T
,
Dieudonné
 
B
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Ghesquière
 
P
,
Wouters
 
J
.
2021
.
Ahead of maturation: enhanced speech envelope training boosts rise time discrimination in pre-readers at cognitive risk for dyslexia
.
Dev Sci.
(
April
):
1–12
. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13186.

Vanden Bempt
 
F
,
Economou
 
M
,
Van Herck
 
S
,
Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Glatz
 
T
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2021
.
Digital game-based phonics instruction promotes print knowledge in pre-readers at cognitive risk for dyslexia
.
Front Psychol
.
12
(
September
):
1
18
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.720548.

Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Dell’Acqua
 
F
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2015
.
Disentangling the relation between left temporoparietal white matter and reading: a spherical deconvolution tractography study
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
36
(
8
):
3273
3287
. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22848.

Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2017
.
Early dynamics of white matter deficits in children developing dyslexia
.
Dev Cogn Neurosci
.
27
(
June
):
69
77
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.003.

Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Vos
 
AD
,
Forkel
 
SJ
,
Catani
 
M
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2018
.
Neural organization of ventral white matter tracts parallels the initial steps of reading development: a DTI tractography study
.
Brain Lang
.
183
(
February
):
32
40
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2018.05.007.

Vandermosten
 
M
,
Boets
 
B
,
Poelmans
 
H
,
Sunaert
 
S
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2012
.
A tractography study in dyslexia: neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing
.
Brain
.
135
(
3
):
935
948
. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr363.

Vandermosten
 
M
,
Boets
 
B
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2012
.
A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
36
(
6
):
1532
1552
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.002.

Vandermosten
 
M
,
Vanderauwera
 
J
,
Theys
 
C
,
De Vos
 
A
,
Vanvooren
 
S
,
Sunaert
 
S
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
.
2015
.
A DTI tractography study in pre-readers at risk for dyslexia
.
Dev Cogn Neurosci
.
14
:
8
15
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.05.006.

Vandermosten
 
M
,
Hoeft
 
F
,
Norton
 
ES
.
2016
.
Integrating MRI brain imaging studies of pre-reading children with current theories of developmental dyslexia: a review and quantitative meta-analysis
.
Curr Opin Behav Sci
.
10
:
155
161
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.06.007.

Verhoeven
 
L
,
Voeten
 
M
,
van Setten
 
E
,
Segers
 
E
.
2020
.
Computer-supported early literacy intervention effects in preschool and kindergarten: a meta-analysis
.
Educ Res Rev
 
30
(
January
). doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2020.100325.

Verwimp
 
C
,
Vanden Bempt
 
F
,
Kellens
 
S
,
Economou
 
M
,
Vandermosten
 
M
,
Wouters
 
J
,
Ghesquière
 
P
,
Vanderauwera
 
J
.
2020
.
Pre-literacy heterogeneity in Dutch-speaking kindergartners: latent profile analysis
.
Ann Dyslexia
.
70
(
3
):275–294. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-020-00207-9.

Wakana
 
S
,
Caprihan
 
A
,
Panzenboeck
 
MM
,
Fallon
 
JH
,
Perry
 
M
,
Gollub
 
RL
,
Hua
 
K
,
Zhang
 
J
,
Jiang
 
H
,
Dubey
 
P
, et al.  
2007
.
Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter
.
NeuroImage
.
36
(
3
):
630
644
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.049.

Walton
 
M
,
Dewey
 
D
,
Lebel
 
C
.
2018
.
Brain white matter structure and language ability in preschool-aged children
.
Brain Lang
.
176
(
September 2016
):
19
25
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2017.10.008.

Wandell
 
BA
,
Yeatman
 
JD
.
2013
.
Biological development of reading circuits
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
.
23
(
2
):
261
268
. https://doi.org/10.1038/jid.2014.371.

Wang
 
Y
,
Mauer
 
MV
,
Raney
 
T
,
Peysakhovich
 
B
,
Becker
 
BLC
,
Sliva
 
DD
,
Gaab
 
N
.
2017
.
Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls
.
Cereb Cortex
.
27
(
4
):
2469
2485
. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw095.

Wanzek
 
J
,
Vaughn
 
S
.
2007
.
Research-based implications from extensive early reading interventions
.
Sch Psychol Rev
.
36
(
4
):
541
561
.

Wasserthal J, Maier-Hein KH, Neher PF, Wolf RC, Northoff G, Waddington JL, Kubera KM, Fritze S, Harneit A, Geiger LS, et al.

2021
.
White matter microstructure alterations in cortico-striatal networks are associated with parkinsonism in schizophrenia spectrum disorders
.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
.
50
:
64
74
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.04.007.

Yeatman
 
J
,
Dougherty
 
RF
,
Rykhlevskaia
 
E
,
Sherbondy
 
A
,
Deutsch
 
GK
,
Wandell
 
BA
,
Ben-Shachar
 
M
.
2011
.
Anatomical properties of the arcuate fasciculus predict phonological and reading skills in children
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
23
(
11
):
3304
3317
. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.

Yeatman
 
JD
,
Dougherty
 
RF
,
Ben-Shachar
 
M
,
Wandell
 
BA
.
2012
.
Development of white matter and reading skills
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
109
(
44
):E3045–E3053. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1206792109.

Yeatman
 
JD
,
Dougherty
 
RF
,
Myall
 
NJ
,
Wandell
 
BA
,
Feldman
 
HM
.
2012
.
Tract profiles of white matter properties: automating Fiber-tract quantification
.
PLoS One
.
7
(
11
):e49790. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049790.

Yeatman
 
JD
,
Rauschecker
 
AM
,
Wandell
 
BA
.
2013
.
Anatomy of the visual word form area: adjacent cortical circuits and long-range white matter connections
.
Brain Lang
.
125
(
2
):
146
155
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.010.Anatomy.

Yendiki
 
A
,
Koldewyn
 
K
,
Kakunoori
 
S
,
Kanwisher
 
N
,
Fischl
 
B
.
2014
.
Spurious group differences due to head motion in a diffusion MRI study
.
NeuroImage
.
88
:
79
90
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.027.Spurious.

Zatorre
 
RJ
,
Fields
 
RD
,
Johansen-Berg
 
H
.
2012
.
Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning
.
Nat Neurosci
.
15
(
4
):
528
536
. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3045.

Zhang
 
M
,
Chen
 
C
,
Xue
 
G
,
Lu
 
Z l
,
Mei
 
L
,
Xue
 
H
,
Wei
 
M
,
He
 
Q
,
Li
 
J
,
Dong
 
Q
.
2014
.
Language-general and -specific white matter microstructural bases for reading
.
NeuroImage
.
98
:
435
441
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.080.

Zuk
 
J
,
Dunstan
 
J
,
Norton
 
E
,
Yu
 
X
,
Ozernov-Palchik
 
O
,
Wang
 
Y
,
Hogan
 
TP
,
Gabrieli
 
JDE
,
Gaab
 
N
.
2021
.
Multifactorial pathways facilitate resilience among kindergarteners at risk for dyslexia: a longitudinal behavioral and neuroimaging study
.
Dev Sci
.
24
(
1
):e12983. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12983.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)