Abstract

Background

The physical and cognitive benefits of moderate-vigorous intensity physical activity (MVPA) for adults have been well documented. Recently, there has been increasing interest in the independent health benefits of light-intensity physical activity (LPA). This research has primarily focused on the relationship between LPA and morbidity and mortality risk, with few studies investigating cognitive associations. The purpose of this scoping review was to catalog existing evidence on the association between device-based or technologically measured LPA and cognition among adults, identify trends in the literature, and recommend future areas for research.

Methods

Six electronic databases were searched between January and June 2020. Forty published studies met the inclusion criteria, which included both healthy and clinical young and older adult populations. Among the 40 articles were 14 acute exercise studies, 4 randomized control trials (RCTs), 18 cross-sectional studies, and 4 longitudinal studies.

Results

7/14 (50%) acute, 3/4 (75%) RCT, 10/18 (56%) cross-sectional, and 2/4 (50%) longitudinal studies reported a significant, positive relationship between LPA and one or more cognitive outcomes. These heterogeneous findings can largely be attributed to the diverse study designs and populations, as well as the numerous assessments used to test the cognitive domains.

Conclusion

These collective findings suggest LPA may be a potential lifestyle intervention to improve cognition across adulthood. However, the inconsistent approaches used among these studies suggest a more concerted, unified scientific approach is needed to further understand the LPA-cognition relationship.

Physical activity is consistently associated with enhanced physical and mental health across the life span. Moderate-vigorous intensity physical activity (MVPA) has traditionally been promoted as the most effective intensity level to induce the greatest health benefits, with official recommendations to achieve a minimum of 150 minutes per week of moderate-intensity, or 75 minutes per week of vigorous-intensity physical activity (1). However, 31.1% of adults across the globe do not meet these recommendations (2). Intrapersonal factors (such as lack of self-efficacy, motivation, or enjoyment) and environmental factors (such as decreases in occupational physical activity, increased availability of labor-saving technology and increased reliance on passive commuting) have largely contributed to the declines in MVPA engagement and increased sedentary behavior (2–6), which is defined as any waking behavior in a sitting or reclining position with an energy expenditure ≤ 1.5 METs (7). Additionally, the worldwide number of adults ≥ 60 years of age is expected to comprise 22% of the total population by 2050 (8), which will have broad-reaching implications for global health care systems. While physical activity engagement is widely promoted as a lifestyle behavior for healthy aging, this population often faces age-related functional and physical limitations preventing MVPA participation (9). Although the health benefits of MVPA are well-documented, less is known about the health benefits of light physical activity (LPA) across the life span and specifically among aging populations.

LPA, defined as any activity requiring an energy expenditure of 1.6–2.9 METs (10), has become an emerging area of interest for its independent contributions to physical health (11–13). After sedentary behavior, which makes up approximately 9.3 hours or 60% of waking hours, LPA is the most engaged in behavior by making up 6.5 hours or 35% of waking hours among adults (4). Currently, no specific guidelines exist for LPA engagement, but broad recommendations of “move more, sit less” are promoted by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (14). More recently, researchers have begun studying the positive health contributions of LPA, especially in the context of cardiometabolic risk and mortality, independent of MVPA engagement (15,16). A systematic review of studies utilizing accelerometer data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey observed LPA’s favorable, significant associations with insulin sensitivity, triglyceride and high-density lipoprotein cholesterol levels, adiposity measures, diabetes, and mortality risk (17). However, the authors noted that of the 40 studies included in the review, only 18 adjusted for MVPA. A more recent systematic review (18) built on these previous findings by only including studies which adjusted for MVPA, and largely corroborated the findings of the preceding review (17). Independent of MVPA participation, Amagasa and colleagues (18) found LPA was inversely associated with metabolic syndrome, waist circumference, triglyceride levels, insulin, and mortality. Notably, the research team included cognitive function as one of the reviewed health-related outcomes. Only 2 cross sectional studies met their inclusion criteria and were reviewed, with inconsistent findings, indicating that the cognitive implications of LPA engagement are less clear and are in need of a more thorough investigation.

Therefore, the purpose of this review was to detail the current evidence regarding the association of LPA and cognition among adults in order to understand if any clinically meaningful associations exist. Given the vast number of research designs, populations studied, and cognitive outcomes investigated across the studies, a scoping review was identified as the most appropriate approach to examine the current status of research activity and catalog the existing evidence examining LPA and cognition (19). Scoping reviews are generally used as a first step to “map the literature” of an emerging research field where there is a paucity of synthesized information in order to gauge the necessity of an in-depth systematic review, identify gaps in the literature and provide informed recommendations for further research in the area (20).

Method

This scoping review was conducted following the 5-stage methodological framework for conducting transparent, rigorous scoping reviews suggested by Arksey and O’Malley (20) and updated by Levac and colleagues (19). The 5 stages below follow along with the methodological steps suggested by the aforementioned authors.

Identify the Research Question

The guiding research question for this review was “what is the current evidence regarding the association between LPA and cognition among adults?” The intentional breadth of this question allowed for a broader scope of this field to be surveyed. Furthermore, given LPA is a health behavior which is widely engaged in by populations of all abilities, both clinical (individuals diagnosed with a physical or mental disease) and nonclinical (individuals who do not have any physical or mental clinical diagnoses) adult populations were included in an effort to avoid overlooking relevant studies which may hold clinically meaningful implications. The operational definition of “cognition” was defined as “the set of mental processes that contribute to perception, memory, intellect and action” (21). Therefore, the primary cognitive domains of focus for this scoping review were memory, executive function, attention and processing speed. The additional parameter of “overall cognitive function” was also included to account for studies which assessed global cognitive status (typically in aging studies).

Identify Relevant Studies

A search string was developed by the study author (E.E.), with the assistance of a health sciences librarian, and was based off previously published search strings used in systematic reviews pertaining to cognition (21) and light physical activity (18). In an effort to broadly capture studies examining LPA as a concept, specific physical activity modalities which can performed at light intensities (such as yoga, walking, dance, tai chi, balance, and flexibility training) were not intentionally included in the search string. However, if a study’s primary aim was to study the effects of LPA and utilized one of these modalities in the design, it was eligible for inclusion. The search parameters for cognition included broad terms (ie, cognition, cognitive function, neurocognition, and brain function) as well as specific functions (ie, memory, executive function, attention, and processing speed). The following medical subject headings were specified in the search string: exercise, executive function, cognition, mental processes, memory, attention, and problem solving (22). The list of search terms used can be found in Table 1. Databases searched included CINHAL, PubMed, SPORTDiscus, Scopus, Web of Science, and PsycINFO. All citations were exported to Mendeley Desktop (Version 1.19.4) and Excel, where duplicates were identified and removed.

Table 1.

List of Search Terms Entered Into the 6 Databases

Terms Related to Light Physical ActivityTerms Related to CognitionTerms Related to Population of Interest
Light-intensity physical activity*CognitionAdult*
Low-intensity physical activity*Cognitive function
Light-intensity lifestyleBrain function
Light-intensity exerciseExecutive function
Low-intensity exerciseExecutive control
Light-intensity activity*Memory
Low-intensity activity*Mental processes
Light-intensity physical activity*Reaction time
Low-intensity physical activity*Response latency
Light-intensity lifestyleAccuracy
Light-intensity exerciseAttention
Low-intensity exerciseTask switching
Problem solving
Decision making
Multitasking
Planning
Reasoning
Comprehension
Spatial memory
Episodic memory
Long-term memory
Declarative memory
Intelligence
Neurocognition
Neurocognitive
Neuro-cognition
Neuro-cognitive
Attentiveness
Concentration
Concentrate
Information retrieval
Information processing
Perceptual skills
Terms Related to Light Physical ActivityTerms Related to CognitionTerms Related to Population of Interest
Light-intensity physical activity*CognitionAdult*
Low-intensity physical activity*Cognitive function
Light-intensity lifestyleBrain function
Light-intensity exerciseExecutive function
Low-intensity exerciseExecutive control
Light-intensity activity*Memory
Low-intensity activity*Mental processes
Light-intensity physical activity*Reaction time
Low-intensity physical activity*Response latency
Light-intensity lifestyleAccuracy
Light-intensity exerciseAttention
Low-intensity exerciseTask switching
Problem solving
Decision making
Multitasking
Planning
Reasoning
Comprehension
Spatial memory
Episodic memory
Long-term memory
Declarative memory
Intelligence
Neurocognition
Neurocognitive
Neuro-cognition
Neuro-cognitive
Attentiveness
Concentration
Concentrate
Information retrieval
Information processing
Perceptual skills

Note: “*” used as wildcard character to search for all phrases which included the truncated term.

Table 1.

List of Search Terms Entered Into the 6 Databases

Terms Related to Light Physical ActivityTerms Related to CognitionTerms Related to Population of Interest
Light-intensity physical activity*CognitionAdult*
Low-intensity physical activity*Cognitive function
Light-intensity lifestyleBrain function
Light-intensity exerciseExecutive function
Low-intensity exerciseExecutive control
Light-intensity activity*Memory
Low-intensity activity*Mental processes
Light-intensity physical activity*Reaction time
Low-intensity physical activity*Response latency
Light-intensity lifestyleAccuracy
Light-intensity exerciseAttention
Low-intensity exerciseTask switching
Problem solving
Decision making
Multitasking
Planning
Reasoning
Comprehension
Spatial memory
Episodic memory
Long-term memory
Declarative memory
Intelligence
Neurocognition
Neurocognitive
Neuro-cognition
Neuro-cognitive
Attentiveness
Concentration
Concentrate
Information retrieval
Information processing
Perceptual skills
Terms Related to Light Physical ActivityTerms Related to CognitionTerms Related to Population of Interest
Light-intensity physical activity*CognitionAdult*
Low-intensity physical activity*Cognitive function
Light-intensity lifestyleBrain function
Light-intensity exerciseExecutive function
Low-intensity exerciseExecutive control
Light-intensity activity*Memory
Low-intensity activity*Mental processes
Light-intensity physical activity*Reaction time
Low-intensity physical activity*Response latency
Light-intensity lifestyleAccuracy
Light-intensity exerciseAttention
Low-intensity exerciseTask switching
Problem solving
Decision making
Multitasking
Planning
Reasoning
Comprehension
Spatial memory
Episodic memory
Long-term memory
Declarative memory
Intelligence
Neurocognition
Neurocognitive
Neuro-cognition
Neuro-cognitive
Attentiveness
Concentration
Concentrate
Information retrieval
Information processing
Perceptual skills

Note: “*” used as wildcard character to search for all phrases which included the truncated term.

Study Selection

Original research studies using any research design (ie, intervention or observational) were eligible for inclusion. The inclusion criteria were the following: (i) include device-based or technologically measured LPA categorized by one of the following American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) definitions of “very light” or “light” relative intensity: <37–45% maximal oxygen uptake (VO2max); <30–39% heart rate reserve (HRR) or % VO2 reserve; <57–63% HRR (23) OR accelerometer-classified LPA, (ii) include at least one validated behavioral cognitive performance measure, (iii) directly examine an association between LPA and cognitive outcomes, (iv) use an adult (18+) sample, and (v) an original research study of either acute (defined as a single isolated bout of LPA), randomized control trial (RCT), cross-sectional or longitudinal design.

Exclusion criteria included the following: (i) cognitive function used as a screening measure and not an outcome variable, (ii) structural or functional brain outcomes (measured via MRI, fMRI, ECG, PET scans, etc.) as the only outcomes reported, (iii) use of self-report LPA as the exposure variable, (iv) not a full peer-reviewed article (ie, conference abstract, thesis), (v) clinical psychological conditions (ie, depression, anxiety) as cognitive outcomes, (vi) LPA designated as a control condition, and (vii) use of animal models. Arksey and O’Malley (20) suggested that search criteria in scoping reviews be amendable to allow for additional criteria to be added once greater familiarity with the field was reached.

The first author (E.E.) conducted the searches and, after duplicates were removed, eliminated articles based on irrelevance of titles and abstracts. Full texts were then retrieved and read to determine basis for inclusion. For articles that did not clearly fit within, or outside of, the search criteria, E.E. and N.G. met to discuss their eligibility. If a consensus could not be reached regarding their status for inclusion, E.M. was consulted to resolve the disagreement. After reviewing all the potentially eligible articles, an additional eighth exclusion criterion of “LPA assessed in experimental/extraordinary conditions” was added. This was to ensure that the only articles included in the review reflected LPA which is engaged in during routine, everyday life. The study selection process is detailed in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram found in Figure 1.

PRISMA flow diagram of study selection process. LPA = light physical activity.
Figure 1.

PRISMA flow diagram of study selection process. LPA = light physical activity.

Charting the Data

Data from all identified studies were extracted using Mendeley Desktop (Version 1.19.4) and Excel. The following data were extracted from each article: first author, year, study design, sample characteristics (mean sample age, gender percentage breakdown, and population characteristics), how LPA was monitored during study period, definition of LPA, cognitive domain(s) assessed, cognitive assessments used, and LPA-specific study findings. The following data were also extracted from relevant studies: time point(s) of data collection (intervention, longitudinal); accelerometer data reduction details (cross-sectional, longitudinal), length/frequency of LPA bouts (acute, intervention); population metrics of LPA, MVPA, and sedentary time (cross-sectional, longitudinal). Reported covariates used in analysis were also recorded.

The fifth and final step of the 5-stage methodological framework, “collating, summarizing, and reporting the results” (20) is detailed in the following sections.

Results

Articles Retrieved

All searches were conducted between January and June 2020 by E.E. with assistance from the college librarian. The initial search yielded 3176 hits, with 2874 titles remaining after duplicates were removed. Next, all titles and abstracts were examined for relevance. After removing 2757 irrelevant hits, 117 articles were reviewed in their entirety and screened for inclusion and exclusion criteria. Eighty articles were excluded for reasons listed in the PRISMA flow (Figure 1). The reference lists of the remaining articles were scanned, yielding 3 more articles.

Article Characteristics

Among the reviewed articles, study designs ranged from: n = 14 acute LPA; n = 4 RCTs; n = 18 cross-sectional; and n = 4 longitudinal. Overall, 32.5% (n = 13) studies examined LPA among young adults (age range 19.1–27.8 years), 25% (n = 10) examined middle-aged adults (age range 35.7–64.78 years), and 45% (n = 18) examined older adults (aged 65 years and older). N = 1 study was comprised of both young and older adults. N = 32 (80%) studies utilized nonclinical, healthy populations. All studies examined either memory (episodic, semantic, and working subdomains), attention/processing speed, executive functioning and/or global cognitive status. The cognitive assessments used are listed in Supplementary Table 1.

Acute Studies Examining LPA

A summary of the 14 acute studies can be found in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Table 2). Overall, 7 (50%) of studies reported at least one significant outcome between the LPA condition and a cognitive domain (24–30). Among these 7 studies, 5 reported associations with executive function (24,26–28,30), 1 reported an association with memory (25), and 1 reported an association with attention (29).

Across all 14 acute studies, 4 measured cognitive performance immediately after the LPA bout. Only 2 found significant pre–post improvements in attention and task-switching performance (27,28). Notably, the 2 studies which reported insignificant associations also measured attention and executive functioning performance (24,31). Among the 5 studies which administered cognitive assessments after a delayed period, 4 reported significant associations among the LPA groups with executive function (24,26,30) and episodic memory (25). Among the 6 studies which administered cognitive assessments during the exercise bout, only 1 study reported significant, positive associations with selective and sustained attention (29).

Overall, 11 studies used young adult samples (mean age range 20.01–27.8; 24–28,32–37), 2 used middle-aged adults (mean age range 35.7–40.2; 29,30) and 1 study included a sample of both young and older adults (mean age 21.8 and 65.5 years, respectively; 31). Two studies were randomized trials (24,35), and the rest utilized a repeated measures study design.

Randomized Control Trials

Four RCTs met the inclusion criteria and are summarized in Supplementary Table 3. Overall, 3 studies (75%) observed that the LPA experimental group significantly increased in at least one cognitive outcome score, including increased processing speed (38), short-term memory (39) and improvements among the attention/concentration, short-term memory and higher cognitive functioning subscales on the Strub and Black Mental status test (40).

The mean age of the study samples was between 60 and 64 years. Two studies included healthy populations (40,41) and the other 2 studied stroke survivors (38,39). Three studies were composed of group-based exercise classes (39–41); 1 study did not clearly distinguish the intervention delivery (38). Only 2 studies reported controlling for covariates in analyses (40,41), which included age, sex, marital status, years of education, and depression.

Cross-sectional Studies

Eighteen cross-sectional studies were included in this review and are summarized in Supplementary Table 4. Overall, 10 studies (55.55%) found LPA to be significantly associated with one or more cognitive outcomes. Six studies found LPA to be significantly, positively associated with executive functioning (42–47); 2 studies found significant, positive associations with attention/processing speed (45,48); 1 study found significant, positive associations with working memory (42); and 3 studies found significant, positive associations with global cognitive status (49–51).

Study sample sizes varied from 72 to 7098 participants. Fourteen included healthy adult samples (42–44,46–56) and 5 studied clinical populations including adults diagnosed with peripheral arterial disease (57), adults diagnosed with schizophrenia (48), older adults with cognitive impairments (45,58) and postmenopausal breast cancer survivors (59). One study included both clinical and nonclinical populations (48). Only one study sampled young adults (mean age 19.1 years; 46), with the remaining studies examining middle aged to older adult populations (mean age range 41.1–88.0). All studies reported LPA and MVPA metrics obtained from accelerometers. The average amount of LPA was 264.22 minutes/day (SD = 119.87), and values ranged from 47.5 to 550.6 minutes/day. The average minutes of MVPA per day was 41.98 (SD = 33.75). Only 10 studies reported sedentary time (42,43,49–54,56,59), which averaged 583.27 minutes/day (SD = 100.49). Sixteen studies reported covariates incorporated into analyses (42–46,48–52,54–59) including demographics, health behaviors, health status, and accelerometer wear time. Twelve studies specifically included covariates of physical activity and/or cognitive status in their analyses (42–44,49–52,54–56,59,60).

Longitudinal Studies

Four longitudinal studies, summarized in Supplementary Table 5, were eligible for inclusion and were composed of sample sizes ranging from 15 to 6452 participants. Overall, 2 studies (50%) found significant associations between accelerometer-measured LPA and cognitive outcomes. One study reported that baseline LPA was predictive of reduced rate of cognitive decline 2 years later (61). The second study reported LPA, which was assessed every month for 4 months, to be significantly correlated with general intelligence and attention (62). Additionally, average LPA engaged in over the 4-month observation period was significantly correlated with attention performance scores at the final, fourth month assessment period.

Three studies included healthy, older adult populations (mean age range 66–74.52; 60–63) and one study examined a small sample (n = 15, mean age 78 years) of older adults diagnosed with cerebrovascular disease (62). All studies assessed LPA at baseline, with average LPA engagement at 199.25 minutes/day (SD = 104.16) and average MVPA engagement at 15.75 minutes/day (SD = 18.17). Follow-up time points ranged from 4 months to 5 years; however, only 2 studies administered accelerometers at both baseline and follow-up (60,62). Average LPA measured at final follow-up time points among the 2 studies was 208.07 minutes/day (SD = 44.14) and average MVPA was 14.87 minutes/day (SD = 10.71). Only 2 studies reported daily sedentary time (62,63) with one study (62) including sleep time in this metric. Notably, only one study included MVPA in analyses as a simultaneous predictor of cognitive outcomes (61).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first scoping review to document the existing evidence from studies examining the direct associations between LPA and cognition. Overall, there is no consistent trend among the 40 reviewed studies, with mixed results for both acute and long-term LPA engagement and its effects on cognitive functions.

Acute exercise has been widely recognized for its short-term improvements on cognitive performance (64). Across the reviewed studies the length of LPA bout ranged between 5.5 and 30 minutes. This in addition to the design parameters such as intensity, mode of exercise, type, and timing of cognitive task administration may have influenced the results (65), making it difficult to compare studies and ascertain acute LPA’s influence on cognition. A previous meta-analysis (65) on the cognitive benefits of acute exercise found that when cognition was assessed during exercise, exercise intensity did not have a significant effect on performance. This was echoed in the current review, where 5 out of 6 studies administering assessments during the LPA bout reported no significant associations with cognitive outcomes. However, among studies where cognition was tested after an acute LPA bout the findings were mixed. The aforementioned meta-analysis (65) suggested that lighter intensity exercise may show cognitive associations when assessments are administered immediately after exercise, possibly due to activation of the “appropriate level of physiological mechanism[s]” (65) to facilitate improved cognitive performance. The study authors also reported significant effect sizes for the effects of an acute bout of LPA on cognition during exercise (d = 0.092), immediately after exercise (d = 0.169), and after a delay (d = 0.245; 65). These small effect sizes provide one possibility for the discrepancy among the results of our current studies, suggesting some studies may have been underpowered.

While a majority (3 out of 4) of the reviewed RCTs reported positive, significant associations between the LPA arm and one or more cognitive outcomes, there were several significant shortcomings in the study designs. None of the studies controlled for participants’ fitness levels or physical activity engagement prior to the study, one had an ambiguous classification of the light-intensity exercise arm (41), and all studies were composed of smaller sample sizes (ranging from 35–72 participants). Additionally, only 4 RCTs were eligible for inclusion, as these studies reported the specific intensity set for their LPA-arm. Previous systematic reviews and meta-analyses of RCTs have noted a consistent lack of intensity dosage reporting among exercise RCTs (66,67). Thus, it is possible more RCTs have been conducted utilizing LPA-based intervention arms. However, by failing to accurately document the intensity of exercise, independent effects of LPA and MVPA cannot be distinguished and thereby potential LPA effects may go undetected.

The nature of cross-sectional study designs likely contributed to these inconsistent findings reported among the reviewed studies, due to the inability to infer causality and establish temporality between the outcome and exposure. Such limitations can only be overcome via more rigorous interventions and RCTs. Additionally, numerous inconsistent study designs and measurements were present among the reviewed studies, including health status of participants (4 studies included clinical populations), varied sample sizes, accelerometer data processing approaches, cognitive assessments administered and outcomes reported, covariates included in analyses, and statistical approaches.

While 2 of the 4 included longitudinal studies observed positive, significant associations between LPA engagement (at one or more time points) and cognitive functioning, all studies had notable limitations. These included high attrition rates (60), small sample sizes (62), and potential practice effects on cognitive assessments (62). Additionally, 2 studies (61,63) only collected physical activity at baseline, and these activity levels were regressed onto changes in cognitive scores from baseline to follow-up. Thus, it is uncertain if participants experienced any changes in physical activity over the observation period and how these changes may have impacted their cognitive scores.

Need to Account for Aerobic Fitness and MVPA Engagement

No studies in the present review included participants’ aerobic fitness as a covariate. This is surprising given the strong and consistent association of aerobic fitness with cognitive function (68,69). Controlling for this variable is especially important in acute and cross-sectional studies which conduct between-subject analyses. Including and controlling for aerobic fitness is important to establish with certainty that any significant differences observed in cognitive function are a result of participants’ LPA participation and not due to unaccounted internal factors (ie, aerobic fitness level).

Only 10 observational studies (9 cross-sectional and 1 longitudinal) included MVPA as a simultaneous predictor in their statistical models. While MVPA is not a validated direct proxy for aerobic fitness, this variable provides insight into LPA’s association with cognition after controlling for engagement in higher intensity activities. The fact that only 53% of the included observational studies accounted for MVPA engagement suggests that the current state of the field may not be fully capturing, and controlling for, all the significant physical activity and subsequent aerobic fitness cognitive predictors. This inconsistency may partly explain the heterogeneous findings of LPA’s association with cognition. It is worth noting, however, that no clear trends emerged between studies which did and did not control for MVPA among their results.

LPA Measurement Methodologies and Reporting

Despite the formal classification of LPA as activity with an energy expenditure between 1.6 and 2.9 METs and methods of estimating this intensity recommended by ACSM (23), operationalization of “light-intensity” activity in physical activity research has widely varied (70). As greater interest continues to emerge regarding the overall independent health benefits of LPA, this inconsistent classification may lead to studies being incorrectly excluded or included in future analyses and reviews. Norton and colleagues (70) have emphasized a need for more consistent terminology regarding exercise intensity, and this is evident across studies which have inaccurately used the term “light-intensity” (71,72).

Additionally, among cross-sectional and longitudinal studies which utilized accelerometers, the data collection and processing methods varied greatly. Some of this variation is due to the selection of cut points validated for specific populations, age groups, or device placement; however, all but 2 of studies included in this review used a waist-based wear protocol. Additionally, even among studies which examined similar populations the selected cut points varied. The most frequently used device cut-off to classify LPA across the reviewed observational studies was by Freedson and colleagues (≤1951 counts/min; 73). However, all studies reported using validated cut-offs to classify LPA. Counts-based classification was most widely used (16 studies), followed by device-calculated MET cutoffs (3 studies) and device-based intensity level classification (2 studies). It has been recently emphasized that due to the variety of decision points researchers face when using accelerometers and the frequent reliance on proprietary intensity-calculation algorithms produced by commercially available devices, the research community is limited in the extent to which accelerometer data for LPA can be compared and reproduced between studies (74).

Hypothesized Underlying Mechanisms

The acute and long-term physiological mechanisms underpinning LPA’s influence on cognitive function have not been widely investigated. Soya and colleagues (75) previously found that rodents who ran at a “mild intensity” (defined as sub-lactic threshold) for 30 minutes exhibited greater activation in the CA1 and dentate gyrus regions of the hippocampus and greater levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA across hippocampal sub regions. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor plays a significant role in upregulating synaptic plasticity (76) and neurogenesis (77), especially in the hippocampal region, which can translate to improved cognitive functioning (76). Another study (25) found that healthy young adults who completed a 10-minute bout of light intensity cycling exhibited increased functional connectivity between hippocampal (dentate gyrus/CA3 regions) and cortical (para-hippocampal, angular, and fusiform gyri) regions, and this increase was predictive of increased episodic memory performance.

It is also possible LPA may indirectly affect cognition via upregulating peripheral metabolic pathways. For example, a previous study (78) found that an 8-week low-intensity walking program improved plasma lipid metabolism among sedentary, middle-aged adults. Impaired lipid metabolism has been associated with cognitive decline and increased risk for neurodegenerative diseases (79). LPA has also been suggested to help regulate blood glucose levels (11). Elevated blood glucose levels are linked to risk of Type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease, both of which are significant predictors of impaired cognitive function and risk of neurodegenerative diseases (80,81).

LPA may also affect cognition via displacement of sedentary behavior, which has been previously investigated for its own independent, detrimental cognitive associations (82). High levels of prolonged sedentary behavior may reduce cerebral blood flow (83), disrupt glucose and lipid metabolism (84,85) and increase risk for metabolic syndrome (86)—all factors associated with compromised cognitive functioning. A previous study (83) found that disrupting prolonged (4 hours) sitting with short, frequent light-intensity walking breaks (2-minute breaks every 30 minutes) attenuated decreases in cerebral blood flow observed in the uninterrupted sitting group. Additional intervention studies have demonstrated physiological benefits of breaking up prolonged sitting with light-intensity walking breaks (87–89), albeit most have been conducted on overweight/obese participants.

Lastly, Iso-Markku and colleagues (49) found significant, independent associations between both sedentary behavior and LPA with cognition among a twin cohort. However, these associations were only observed in between-family linear analyses (ie, between different sets of twins), leading the authors to hypothesize that the influence of LPA and sedentary behavior on cognition may be due to “genetic selection and environmental similarity between siblings” (49).

Limitations

Given that this was a scoping review, the literature selection and review process was not subjected to the extensively rigorous methodology that is a typical component of systematic reviews or meta-analyses. However, systematic reviews are typically conducted after such “scoping endeavors” have already mapped out the general trends of the field to date, in order to answer specific questions using studies selected from a very narrow search criteria (20). Thus, the intent of the present work was to capture all LPA-cognition related studies to better understand the current state of the field and which direction(s) may be the most promising to pursue. Thus, not adhering to the “systematic” approach is not particularly a limitation. That being said, because scoping reviews are intended to be broad and all-encompassing, we did not dissect differences of association among populations, age groups or cognitive domains. These specific research questions would be more appropriate for a future systematic review or a meta-analysis.

Conclusion

In order to further our understanding of the potential independent health contributions of LPA, especially in the context of cognitive function, the following considerations are recommended for future research studies:

  1. More rigorous RCTs using LPA as the primary intervention arm. This also includes clearly defined intensity parameters for LPA and fidelity checks during the interventions to ensure participants are adhering to the prescribed intensity.

  2. Incorporate aerobic fitness and MVPA engagement as a covariate in analyses, to understand if individuals of varying aerobic fitness levels and physical activity habits respond differently to LPA.

  3. Design studies with greater power (ie, larger sample sizes) to detect potential small effect sizes.

  4. Determine whether displacing sedentary behavior with LPA results in significant cognitive gains.

Currently, the research supporting the association between LPA and cognition is equivocal and future research in this area will necessitate studies of sufficient methodological rigor to determine the efficacy of this relationship. In this scoping review, we have detailed the existing evidence, identified gaps and issues that must be addressed in order to further our knowledge, and provided suggestions for future research. Given that most individuals can easily engage in LPA, determining its potential as an independent behavior to maintain or improve cognitive functions becomes an important public health goal.

Acknowledgment

The authors would like to thank the UIUC Applied Health Sciences librarian for assistance with search string development.

Funding

None declared.

Author Contributions

E.E.: scoping review conception, search string development, assessment of articles for eligibility, data extraction, and manuscript writing; E.M.: manuscript writing and review; N.G.: scoping review conception, assessment of full-text articles for eligibility, and manuscript writing and review.

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

References

1.

World Health Organization.
Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health.
Accessed
June 3, 2020
. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/44399/9789241599979_eng.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y

2.

Hallal
PC
,
Andersen
LB
,
Bull
FC
, et al.
Global physical activity levels: surveillance progress, pitfalls, and prospects
.
Lancet
.
2012
;
380
(
9838
):
247
257
. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60646-1

3.

Lewis
BA
,
Williams
DM
,
Frayeh
AL
,
Marcus
BH
.
Self-efficacy versus perceived enjoyment as predictors of physical activity behavior
.
Psychol Heal
.
2016
;
31
(
4
):
456
469
. doi:10.1080/08870446.2015.1111372

4.

Dunstan
DW
,
Healy
GN
,
Sugiyama
T
,
Owen
N
.
“Too much sitting” and metabolic risk - has modern technology caught up with us?
US Endocrinol
.
2009
;
5
:
29
33
. doi:10.17925/use.2009.05.1.29

5.

Ryan
RM
,
Frederick
CM
,
Lepes
D
,
Rubio
N
,
Sheldon
KM
.
Intrinsic motivation and exercise adherence
.
Int J Sport Psychol
.
1997
;
28
(
4
):
335
354
.

6.

McAuley
E
,
Blissmer
B
.
Self-efficacy determinants and consequences of physical activity
.
Exerc Sport Sci Rev
.
2000
;
28
:
85
88
.

7.

Tremblay
MS
,
Aubert
S
,
Barnes
JD
, et al.
Sedentary Behavior Research Network (SBRN) - terminology consensus project process and outcome
.
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
.
2017
;
14
:
75
. doi:10.1186/s12966-017-0525-8

8.

World Health Organization.
10 Facts on Ageing and Health.
Accessed
April 20, 2020
. https://www.who.int/features/factfiles/ageing/en/

9.

Schutzer
KA
,
Graves
BS
.
Barriers and motivations to exercise in older adults
.
Prev Med
.
2004
;
39
:
1056
1061
. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.04.003

10.

Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee.
2018 Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee Scientific Report
.
US Department of Health and Human Services
;
2018
.

11.

Healy
GN
,
Dunstan
DW
,
Salmon
J
, et al.
Objectively measured light-intensity physical activity is independently associated with 2-h plasma glucose
.
Diabetes Care
.
2007
;
30
:
1384
1389
. doi:10.2337/dc07-0114

12.

Gando
Y
,
Yamamoto
K
,
Murakami
H
, et al.
Longer time spent in light physical activity is associated with reduced arterial stiffness in older adults
.
Hypertension
.
2010
;
56
:
540
546
. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.110.156331

13.

Buman
MP
,
Hekler
EB
,
Haskell
WL
, et al.
Objective light-intensity physical activity associations with rated health in older adults
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2010
;
172
:
1155
1165
. doi:10.1093/aje/kwq249

14.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
How Much Physical Activity do Adults Need?
Accessed
May 25, 2020
. https://www.cdc.gov/physicalactivity/basics/adults/index.htm

15.

LaMonte
MJ
,
Lewis
CE
,
Buchner
DM
, et al.
Both light intensity and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity measured by accelerometry are favorably associated with cardiometabolic risk factors in older women: the Objective Physical Activity and Cardiovascular Health (OPACH) study
.
J Am Heart Assoc
.
2017
;
6
(
10
):
1
15
. doi:10.1161/JAHA.117.007064

16.

Ku
PW
,
Hamer
M
,
Liao
Y
,
Hsueh
MC
,
Chen
LJ
.
Device-measured light-intensity physical activity and mortality: a meta-analysis
.
Scand J Med Sci Sports
.
2020
;
30
:
13
24
. doi:10.1111/sms.13557

17.

Füzéki
E
,
Engeroff
T
,
Banzer
W
.
Health benefits of light-intensity physical activity: a systematic review of accelerometer data of the national health and nutrition examination survey (NHANES)
.
Sports Med
.
2017
;
47
:
1769
1793
. doi:10.1007/s40279-017-0724-0

18.

Amagasa
S
,
Machida
M
,
Fukushima
N
, et al.
Is objectively measured light-intensity physical activity associated with health outcomes after adjustment for moderate-to-vigorous physical activity in adults? A systematic review
.
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
.
2018
;
15
:
65
. doi:10.1186/s12966-018-0695-z

19.

Levac
D
,
Colquhoun
H
,
O’Brien
KK
.
Scoping studies: advancing the methodology
.
Implement Sci
.
2010
;
5
:
69
. doi:10.1186/1748-5908-5-69

20.

Arksey
H
,
O’Malley
L
.
Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework
.
Int J Soc Res Methodol
.
2005
;
8
(
1
):
19
32
. doi:10.1017/S0922156508005621

21.

Donnelly
JE
,
Hillman
CH
,
Castelli
D
, et al.
Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review
.
Med Sci Sports Exerc
.
2016
;
48
:
1223
1224
. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000000966

22.

Lipscomb
CE
.
Medical subject headings (MeSH)
.
Bull Med Libr Assoc
.
2000
;
88
(
3
):
265
266
. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10928714

23.

American College of Sports Medicine.
Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription
. 10th ed.
Wolters Kluwer
;
2018
.

24.

Brown
DMY
,
Bray
SR
.
Acute effects of continuous and high-intensity interval exercise on executive function
.
J Appl Biobehav Res
.
2018
;
23
(
3
):
e12121
. doi:10.1111/jabr.12121

25.

Suwabe
K
,
Byun
K
,
Hyodo
K
, et al.
Rapid stimulation of human dentate gyrus function with acute mild exercise
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2018
;
115
(
41
):
10487
10492
. doi:10.1073/pnas.1805668115

26.

Byun
K
,
Hyodo
K
,
Suwabe
K
, et al.
Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: an fNIRS study
.
Neuroimage
.
2014
;
98
:
336
345
. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.067

27.

Kamijo
K
,
Nishihira
Y
,
Higashiura
T
,
Kuroiwa
K
.
The interactive effect of exercise intensity and task difficulty on human cognitive processing
.
Int J Psychophysiol
.
2007
;
65
:
114
121
. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.001

28.

Morris
TP
,
Fried
PJ
,
Macone
J
, et al.
Light aerobic exercise modulates executive function and cortical excitability
.
Eur J Neurosci
.
2020
;
51
:
1723
1734
. doi:10.1111/ejn.14593

29.

Torbeyns
T
,
de Geus
B
,
Bailey
S
, et al.
Cycling on a bike desk positively influences cognitive performance
.
PLoS One
.
2016
;
11
:
e0165510
. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0165510

30.

Sandroff
BM
,
Hillman
CH
,
Benedict
RH
,
Motl
RW
.
Acute effects of varying intensities of treadmill walking exercise on inhibitory control in persons with multiple sclerosis: a pilot investigation
.
Physiol Behav
.
2016
;
154
:
20
27
. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.008

31.

Kamijo
K
,
Hayashi
Y
,
Sakai
T
,
Yahiro
T
,
Tanaka
K
,
Nishihira
Y
.
Acute effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in older adults
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
2009
;
64
:
356
363
. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp030

32.

Radel
R
,
Tempest
GD
,
Brisswalter
J
.
The long and winding road: effects of exercise intensity and type upon sustained attention
.
Physiol Behav
.
2018
;
195
:
82
89
. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.07.028

33.

Alderman
BL
,
Olson
RL
,
Mattina
DM
.
Cognitive function during low-intensity walking: a test of the treadmill workstation
.
J Phys Act Health
.
2014
;
11
:
752
758
. doi:10.1123/jpah.2012-0097

34.

Labelle
V
,
Bosquet
L
,
Mekary
S
,
Bherer
L
.
Decline in executive control during acute bouts of exercise as a function of exercise intensity and fitness level
.
Brain Cogn
.
2013
;
81
:
10
17
. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2012.10.001

35.

Loprinzi
PD
,
Kane
CJ
.
Exercise and cognitive function: a randomized controlled trial examining acute exercise and free-living physical activity and sedentary effects
.
Mayo Clin Proc
.
2015
;
90
(
4
):
450
460
. doi:10.1016/j.mayocp.2014.12.023

36.

Loprinzi
PD
,
Day
S
,
Deming
R
.
Acute exercise intensity and memory function: evaluation of the transient hypofrontality hypothesis
.
Med
.
2019
;
55
(
8
):
445
. doi:10.3390/medicina55080445

37.

Mekari
S
,
Fraser
S
,
Bosquet
L
, et al.
The relationship between exercise intensity, cerebral oxygenation and cognitive performance in young adults
.
Eur J Appl Physiol
.
2015
;
115
:
2189
2197
. doi:10.1007/s00421-015-3199-4

38.

Debreceni-Nagy
A
,
Horváth
J
,
Bajuszné Kovács
N
,
Fülöp
P
,
Jenei
Z
.
The effect of low-intensity aerobic training on cognitive functions of severely deconditioned subacute and chronic stroke patients: a randomized, controlled pilot study
.
Int J Rehabil Res
.
2019
;
42
(
3
):
275
279
. doi:10.1097/MRR.0000000000000346

39.

Tang
A
,
Eng
JJ
,
Krassioukov
AV
,
Tsang
TS
,
Liu-Ambrose
T
.
High- and low-intensity exercise do not improve cognitive function after stroke: a randomized controlled trial
.
J Rehabil Med
.
2016
;
48
:
841
846
. doi:10.2340/16501977-2163

40.

Stevenson
JS
,
Topp
R
.
Effects of moderate and low intensity long-term exercise by older adults
.
Res Nurs Health
.
1990
;
13
:
209
218
. doi:10.1002/nur.4770130403

41.

Ruscheweyh
R
,
Willemer
C
,
Krüger
K
, et al.
Physical activity and memory functions: an interventional study
.
Neurobiol Aging
.
2011
;
32
:
1304
1319
. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.08.001

42.

Gothe
NP
.
Examining the effects of light versus moderate to vigorous physical activity on cognitive function in African American adults
.
Aging Ment Health
.
2020
:
1
7
. doi:10.1080/13607863.2020.1768216

43.

Johnson
LG
,
Butson
ML
,
Polman
RC
, et al.
Light physical activity is positively associated with cognitive performance in older community dwelling adults
.
J Sci Med Sport
.
2016
;
19
:
877
882
. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2016.02.002

44.

Kerr
J
,
Marshall
SJ
,
Patterson
RE
, et al.
Objectively measured physical activity is related to cognitive function in older adults
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2013
;
61
:
1927
1931
. doi:10.1111/jgs.12524

45.

Umegaki
H
,
Makino
T
,
Uemura
K
,
Shimada
H
,
Cheng
XW
,
Kuzuya
M
.
Objectively measured physical activity and cognitive function in urban-dwelling older adults
.
Geriatr Gerontol Int
.
2018
;
18
(
6
):
922
928
. doi:10.1111/ggi.13284

46.

Lin
J
,
Wang
K
,
Chen
Z
, et al.
Associations between objectively measured physical activity and executive functioning in young adults
.
Percept Mot Skills
.
2018
;
125
:
278
288
. doi:10.1177/0031512517745438

47.

Wilbur
J
,
Marquez
DX
,
Fogg
L
, et al.
The relationship between physical activity and cognition in older Latinos
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
2012
;
67
:
525
534
. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr137

48.

Chen
LJ
,
Steptoe
A
,
Chung
MS
,
Ku
PW
.
Association between actigraphy-derived physical activity and cognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia
.
Psychol Med
.
2016
;
46
:
2375
2384
. doi:10.1017/S0033291716000921

49.

Iso-Markku
P
,
Waller
K
,
Vuoksimaa
E
, et al.
Objectively measured physical activity profile and cognition in Finnish elderly twins
.
Alzheimers Dement (N Y)
.
2018
;
4
:
263
271
. doi:10.1016/j.trci.2018.06.007

50.

Suzuki
K
,
Niimura
H
,
Kida
H
, et al.
Increasing light physical activity helps to maintain cognitive function among the community-dwelling oldest old population: a cross-sectional study using actigraph from the Arakawa 85+ study
.
Geriatr Gerontol Int
.
2020
:
1
6
. doi:10.1111/ggi.13967

51.

Wu
ZJ
,
Wang
ZY
,
Hu
BQ
, et al.
Relationships of accelerometer-based measured objective physical activity and sedentary behaviour with cognitive function: a comparative cross-sectional study of China’s elderly population
.
BMC Geriatr
.
2020
;
20
:
149
. doi:10.1186/s12877-020-01521-y

52.

Zhu
W
,
Howard
VJ
,
Wadley
VG
, et al.
Association between objectively measured physical activity and cognitive function in older adults-the reasons for geographic and racial differences in stroke study
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2015
;
63
:
2447
2454
. doi:10.1111/jgs.13829

53.

Zlatar
ZZ
,
Hays
CC
,
Mestre
Z
, et al.
Dose-dependent association of accelerometer-measured physical activity and sedentary time with brain perfusion in aging
.
Exp Gerontol
.
2019
;
125
:
110679
. doi:10.1016/j.exger.2019.110679

54.

Fanning
J
,
Porter
G
,
Awick
EA
, et al.
Replacing sedentary time with sleep, light, or moderate-to- vigorous physical activity: effects on self-regulation and executive
.
J Behav Med
.
2017
;
40
(
2
):
332
342
. doi:10.1007/s10865-016-9788-9.

55.

Kimura
K
,
Yasunaga
A
,
Wang
LQ
.
Correlation between moderate daily physical activity and neurocognitive variability in healthy elderly people
.
Arch Gerontol Geriatr
.
2013
;
56
:
109
117
. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2012.10.004

56.

Amagasa
S
,
Inoue
S
,
Murayama
H
, et al.
Associations of sedentary and physically-active behaviors with cognitive-function decline in community-dwelling older adults: compositional data analysis from the NEIGE study
.
J Epidemiol
.
2020
;
30
(
11
):
503
508
. doi:10.2188/jea.je20190141

57.

Cavalcante
BR
,
Germano-Soares
AH
,
Gerage
AM
, et al.
Association between physical activity and walking capacity with cognitive function in peripheral artery disease patients
.
Eur J Vasc Endovasc Surg
.
2018
;
55
:
672
678
. doi:10.1016/j.ejvs.2018.02.010

58.

Makizako
H
,
Liu-Ambrose
T
,
Shimada
H
, et al.
Moderate-intensity physical activity, hippocampal volume, and memory in older adults with mild cognitive impairment
.
Journals Gerontol - Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci
.
2015
;
70
(
4
):
480
486
. doi:10.1093/gerona/glu136

59.

Marinac
CR
,
Godbole
S
,
Kerr
J
,
Natarajan
L
,
Patterson
RE
,
Hartman
SJ
.
Objectively measured physical activity and cognitive functioning in breast cancer survivors
.
J Cancer Surviv
.
2015
;
9
:
230
238
. doi:10.1007/s11764-014-0404-0

60.

Halloway
S
,
Wilbur
J
,
Schoeny
ME
,
Barnes
LL
.
The relation between physical activity and cognitive change in older latinos
.
Biol Res Nurs
.
2017
;
19
:
538
548
. doi:10.1177/1099800417715115

61.

Stubbs
B
,
Chen
LJ
,
Chang
CY
,
Sun
WJ
,
Ku
PW
.
Accelerometer-assessed light physical activity is protective of future cognitive ability: a longitudinal study among community dwelling older adults
.
Exp Gerontol
.
2017
;
91
:
104
109
. doi:10.1016/j.exger.2017.03.003

62.

Kojima
M
,
Nagano
A
.
Assessment of physical activity and cognitive function and their potential correlation in convalescent patients of cerebrovascular disease
.
Sci Rep
.
2019
;
9
:
3782
. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-40460-6

63.

Zhu
W
,
Wadley
VG
,
Howard
VJ
,
Hutto
B
,
Blair
SN
,
Hooker
SP
.
Objectively measured physical activity and cognitive function in older adults
.
Med Sci Sports Exerc
.
2017
;
49
:
47
53
. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000001079

64.

Audiffren
M
.
Acute exercise and psychological functions: a cognitive energetic approach
. In: McMorris T, Tomporowski PD, Audiffren M, eds.
Exercise and Cognitive Fucntion
.
John Wiley and Sons Ltd
;
2009
:
3
39
.

65.

Chang
YK
,
Labban
JD
,
Gapin
JI
,
Etnier
JL
.
The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis
.
Brain Res
.
2012
;
1453
:
87
101
. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068

66.

Kelly
ME
,
Loughrey
D
,
Lawlor
BA
,
Robertson
IH
,
Walsh
C
,
Brennan
S
.
The impact of exercise on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Ageing Res Rev
.
2014
;
16
:
12
31
. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2014.05.002

67.

Falck
RS
,
Davis
JC
,
Best
JR
,
Crockett
RA
,
Liu-Ambrose
T
.
Impact of exercise training on physical and cognitive function among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Neurobiol Aging
.
2019
;
79
:
119
130
. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.007

68.

Barnes
DE
,
Yaffe
K
,
Satariano
WA
,
Tager
IB
.
A longitudinal study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy older adults
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2003
;
51
:
459
465
. doi:10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51153.x

69.

Kramer
AF
,
Colcombe
S
.
Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study
.
Perspect Psychol Sci
.
2003
;
13
(
2
):
213
217
. doi:10.1177/1745691617707316

70.

Norton
K
,
Norton
L
,
Sadgrove
D
.
Position statement on physical activity and exercise intensity terminology
.
J Sci Med Sport
.
2010
;
13
:
496
502
. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2009.09.008

71.

Wu
C
,
Yi
Q
,
Zheng
X
, et al.
Effects of mind-body exercises on cognitive function in older adults: a meta-analysis
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2019
;
67
(
4
):
749
758
. doi:10.1111/jgs.15714

72.

Voss
MW
,
Weng
TB
,
Narayana-Kumanan
K
, et al.
Acute exercise effects predict training change in cognition and connectivity
.
Med Sci Sports Exerc
.
2020
;
52
:
131
140
. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000002115

73.

Freedson
PS
,
Melanson
E
,
Sirard
J
.
Calibration of the computer science and applications, inc. accelerometer
.
Med Sci Sports Exerc
.
1998
;
30
:
777
781
. doi:10.1097/00005768-199805000-00021

74.

Burchartz
A
,
Anedda
B
,
Auerswald
T
, et al.
Assessing physical behavior through accelerometry – state of the science, best practices and future directions
.
Psychol Sport Exerc
.
2020
;
49
. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101703

75.

Soya
H
,
Nakamura
T
,
Deocaris
CC
, et al.
BDNF induction with mild exercise in the rat hippocampus
.
Biochem Biophys Res Commun
.
2007
;
358
:
961
967
. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2007.04.173

76.

Sendtner
M
.
Neurotrophic factors
. In: Beal MF, Lang AE, Ludolph A, eds.
Neurodegenerative Diseases: Neurobiology, Pathogenesis and Therapeutics
.
Cambridge University Press
;
2005
:
94
107
. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511544873.009

77.

Scharfman
H
,
Goodman
J
,
Macleod
A
,
Phani
S
,
Antonelli
C
,
Croll
S
.
Increased neurogenesis and the ectopic granule cells after intrahippocampal BDNF infusion in adult rats
.
Exp Neurol
.
2005
;
192
:
348
356
. doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2004.11.016

78.

Butcher
LR
,
Thomas
A
,
Backx
K
,
Roberts
A
,
Webb
R
,
Morris
K
.
Low-intensity exercise exerts beneficial effects on plasma lipids via PPARgamma
.
Med Sci Sports Exerc
.
2008
;
40
:
1263
1270
. doi:10.1249/MSS.0b013e31816c091d

79.

Panza
F
,
D’Introno
A
,
Colacicco
AM
, et al.
Lipid metabolism in cognitive decline and dementia
.
Brain Res Rev
.
2006
;
51
:
275
292
. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.11.007

80.

Breteler
MM
,
Claus
JJ
,
Grobbee
DE
,
Hofman
A
.
Cardiovascular disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly people: the Rotterdam Study
.
BMJ
.
1994
;
308
:
1604
1608
. doi:10.1136/bmj.308.6944.1604

81.

Stewart
R
,
Liolitsa
D
.
Type 2 diabetes mellitus, cognitive impairment and dementia
.
Diabet Med
.
1999
;
16
:
93
112
. doi:10.1046/j.1464-5491.1999.00027.x

82.

Falck
RS
,
Davis
JC
,
Liu-Ambrose
T
.
What is the association between sedentary behaviour and cognitive function? A systematic review
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2017
;
51
:
800
811
. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2015-095551

83.

Carter
SE
,
Draijer
R
,
Holder
SM
,
Brown
L
,
Thijssen
DHJ
,
Hopkins
ND
.
Regular walking breaks prevent the decline in cerebral blood flow associated with prolonged sitting
.
J Appl Physiol (1985)
.
2018
;
125
:
790
798
. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00310.2018

84.

Zderic
TW
,
Hamilton
MT
.
Physical inactivity amplifies the sensitivity of skeletal muscle to the lipid-induced downregulation of lipoprotein lipase activity
.
J Appl Physiol (1985)
.
2006
;
100
:
249
257
. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00925.2005

85.

Wheeler
MJ
,
Dempsey
PC
,
Grace
MS
, et al.
Sedentary behavior as a risk factor for cognitive decline? A focus on the influence of glycemic control in brain health
.
Alzheimers Dement (N Y)
.
2017
;
3
:
291
300
. doi:10.1016/j.trci.2017.04.001

86.

Edwardson
CL
,
Gorely
T
,
Davies
MJ
, et al.
Association of sedentary behaviour with metabolic syndrome: a meta-analysis
.
PLoS One
.
2012
;
7
:
e34916
. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034916

87.

Grace
MS
,
Formosa
MF
,
Bozaoglu
K
, et al.
Acute effects of active breaks during prolonged sitting on subcutaneous adipose tissue gene expression: an ancillary analysis of a randomised controlled trial
.
Sci Rep
.
2019
;
9
:
3847
. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-40490-0

88.

Dunstan
DW
,
Kingwell
BA
,
Larsen
R
, et al.
Breaking up prolonged sitting reduces postprandial glucose and insulin responses
.
Diabetes Care
.
2012
;
35
:
976
983
. doi:10.2337/dc11-1931

89.

Bailey
DP
,
Locke
CD
.
Breaking up prolonged sitting with light-intensity walking improves postprandial glycemia, but breaking up sitting with standing does not
.
J Sci Med Sport
.
2015
;
18
:
294
298
. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2014.03.008

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)
Decision Editor: Anne B Newman, MD, MPH, FGSA
Anne B Newman, MD, MPH, FGSA
Decision Editor
Search for other works by this author on: