Figure 1
Overview of analytical steps. (A) We began with previously identified sensory-biased regions in caudolateral frontal cortex, along the PCS and IFS (Michalka et al. 2015; Noyce et al. 2017). Within each individual subject, that subject’s auditory- and visual-biased regions were used as seeds to compute seed-to-whole-hemisphere resting-state functional connectivity. We thresholded (see Methods) and z-transformed the resulting connectivity maps before taking the difference between auditory-biased and visual-biased connectivity. The resulting differential connectivity maps were used to identify candidate sensory-biased regions (Fig. 3A). We assessed each candidate region using split-half reliability of task activation (Fig. 3B) and hand-scoring the region’s appearance in individual subjects (Supplementary Table S1). (B) Regions that exhibited consistent sensory-biased WM recruitment both within and across subjects were denoted as reliable regions; we drew subject-specific labels for each region in each subject for analysis (Fig. 4). We reported the mean location and size (Fig. 4 and Table 1), the degree of WM-specific recruitment (Fig. 5), and used hierarchical clustering of seed-to-seed functional connectivity to investigate the structure of sensory-biased networks (Fig. 6). (C) Regions that did not exhibit consistent sensory-biased recruitment were investigated using the candidate search space labels; we again reported the degree of WM-specific recruitment in each (Fig. 7).

Overview of analytical steps. (A) We began with previously identified sensory-biased regions in caudolateral frontal cortex, along the PCS and IFS (Michalka et al. 2015; Noyce et al. 2017). Within each individual subject, that subject’s auditory- and visual-biased regions were used as seeds to compute seed-to-whole-hemisphere resting-state functional connectivity. We thresholded (see Methods) and z-transformed the resulting connectivity maps before taking the difference between auditory-biased and visual-biased connectivity. The resulting differential connectivity maps were used to identify candidate sensory-biased regions (Fig. 3A). We assessed each candidate region using split-half reliability of task activation (Fig. 3B) and hand-scoring the region’s appearance in individual subjects (Supplementary Table S1). (B) Regions that exhibited consistent sensory-biased WM recruitment both within and across subjects were denoted as reliable regions; we drew subject-specific labels for each region in each subject for analysis (Fig. 4). We reported the mean location and size (Fig. 4 and Table 1), the degree of WM-specific recruitment (Fig. 5), and used hierarchical clustering of seed-to-seed functional connectivity to investigate the structure of sensory-biased networks (Fig. 6). (C) Regions that did not exhibit consistent sensory-biased recruitment were investigated using the candidate search space labels; we again reported the degree of WM-specific recruitment in each (Fig. 7).

Close
This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

Close

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

View Article Abstract & Purchase Options

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Close