Table 2.

Core Propositions of the Lifespan Developmental Perspective (Baltes, 1987)

Proposition From Baltes (1987) Corollary Arguments and Evidence
Proposition #1Development is a lifelong processFrom conception to death, development is characterized by continuous (i.e., cumulative) and discontinuous (i.e., emergent or innovative) processes; all age periods, including infancy, childhood, adolescence, adulthood, and old age, are of equal importance from the lifespan perspective (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #2Development is multidimensional and multidirectionalConsidering cognitive abilities, some indicators (e.g., working memory), on average, tend to decline with age, some remain relatively stable across the lifespan, and others (e.g., experience-based knowledge and judgment) typically improve with age (Baltes et al., 1999; Salthouse, 2012)
Proposition #3Development involves the dynamic and joint occurrence of growth (or gains) and decline (or losses) in different domains of functioningBoth learning (which leads to personal growth) and physical decline are possible in different phases of the lifespan. However, losses increasingly outweigh gains in functioning at higher ages (i.e., changing loss–gain ratio; Baltes, 1987).
Proposition #4Development is partially context-dependentDevelopment is embedded in, coincides with, influences, and is influenced by, historical time and events, sociocultural conditions, and other contextual factors or levels of organization (Baltes, 1987; Lerner, 1996).
Proposition #5Development results from the interaction of three systems of influence which define the paradigm of contextualism and the “tri-factor model” of developmental contextualismBaltes (1987) and Lerner (1996) describe normative age-graded influences (i.e., age-related person and contextual determinants that most people encounter as they age), normative history-graded influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that most people living during a certain historical period are experiencing), and non-normative influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that are rather idiosyncratic and uncommon).
Proposition #6There is potential for plasticity, or within-person modifiability, in developmentPerson and contextual factors can positively or negatively impact an individual’s experiences and behavior at any age, and the lifespan perspective aims to better understand the range, potentials, and limits of plasticity in development (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #7Individual development needs to be studied at multiple levels of analysisDevelopment must be understood across different levels, ranging from the biological level to the psychological and social-relational levels to the sociocultural and macro-institutional levels (Lerner, 1996). Thus, development needs to be investigated by scholars working in multiple scientific disciplines, including anthropology, biology, sociology, medical sciences, and psychology. A mono-disciplinary view is not sufficient to fully understand the nature of development (Baltes, 1987).
Proposition From Baltes (1987) Corollary Arguments and Evidence
Proposition #1Development is a lifelong processFrom conception to death, development is characterized by continuous (i.e., cumulative) and discontinuous (i.e., emergent or innovative) processes; all age periods, including infancy, childhood, adolescence, adulthood, and old age, are of equal importance from the lifespan perspective (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #2Development is multidimensional and multidirectionalConsidering cognitive abilities, some indicators (e.g., working memory), on average, tend to decline with age, some remain relatively stable across the lifespan, and others (e.g., experience-based knowledge and judgment) typically improve with age (Baltes et al., 1999; Salthouse, 2012)
Proposition #3Development involves the dynamic and joint occurrence of growth (or gains) and decline (or losses) in different domains of functioningBoth learning (which leads to personal growth) and physical decline are possible in different phases of the lifespan. However, losses increasingly outweigh gains in functioning at higher ages (i.e., changing loss–gain ratio; Baltes, 1987).
Proposition #4Development is partially context-dependentDevelopment is embedded in, coincides with, influences, and is influenced by, historical time and events, sociocultural conditions, and other contextual factors or levels of organization (Baltes, 1987; Lerner, 1996).
Proposition #5Development results from the interaction of three systems of influence which define the paradigm of contextualism and the “tri-factor model” of developmental contextualismBaltes (1987) and Lerner (1996) describe normative age-graded influences (i.e., age-related person and contextual determinants that most people encounter as they age), normative history-graded influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that most people living during a certain historical period are experiencing), and non-normative influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that are rather idiosyncratic and uncommon).
Proposition #6There is potential for plasticity, or within-person modifiability, in developmentPerson and contextual factors can positively or negatively impact an individual’s experiences and behavior at any age, and the lifespan perspective aims to better understand the range, potentials, and limits of plasticity in development (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #7Individual development needs to be studied at multiple levels of analysisDevelopment must be understood across different levels, ranging from the biological level to the psychological and social-relational levels to the sociocultural and macro-institutional levels (Lerner, 1996). Thus, development needs to be investigated by scholars working in multiple scientific disciplines, including anthropology, biology, sociology, medical sciences, and psychology. A mono-disciplinary view is not sufficient to fully understand the nature of development (Baltes, 1987).
Table 2.

Core Propositions of the Lifespan Developmental Perspective (Baltes, 1987)

Proposition From Baltes (1987) Corollary Arguments and Evidence
Proposition #1Development is a lifelong processFrom conception to death, development is characterized by continuous (i.e., cumulative) and discontinuous (i.e., emergent or innovative) processes; all age periods, including infancy, childhood, adolescence, adulthood, and old age, are of equal importance from the lifespan perspective (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #2Development is multidimensional and multidirectionalConsidering cognitive abilities, some indicators (e.g., working memory), on average, tend to decline with age, some remain relatively stable across the lifespan, and others (e.g., experience-based knowledge and judgment) typically improve with age (Baltes et al., 1999; Salthouse, 2012)
Proposition #3Development involves the dynamic and joint occurrence of growth (or gains) and decline (or losses) in different domains of functioningBoth learning (which leads to personal growth) and physical decline are possible in different phases of the lifespan. However, losses increasingly outweigh gains in functioning at higher ages (i.e., changing loss–gain ratio; Baltes, 1987).
Proposition #4Development is partially context-dependentDevelopment is embedded in, coincides with, influences, and is influenced by, historical time and events, sociocultural conditions, and other contextual factors or levels of organization (Baltes, 1987; Lerner, 1996).
Proposition #5Development results from the interaction of three systems of influence which define the paradigm of contextualism and the “tri-factor model” of developmental contextualismBaltes (1987) and Lerner (1996) describe normative age-graded influences (i.e., age-related person and contextual determinants that most people encounter as they age), normative history-graded influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that most people living during a certain historical period are experiencing), and non-normative influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that are rather idiosyncratic and uncommon).
Proposition #6There is potential for plasticity, or within-person modifiability, in developmentPerson and contextual factors can positively or negatively impact an individual’s experiences and behavior at any age, and the lifespan perspective aims to better understand the range, potentials, and limits of plasticity in development (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #7Individual development needs to be studied at multiple levels of analysisDevelopment must be understood across different levels, ranging from the biological level to the psychological and social-relational levels to the sociocultural and macro-institutional levels (Lerner, 1996). Thus, development needs to be investigated by scholars working in multiple scientific disciplines, including anthropology, biology, sociology, medical sciences, and psychology. A mono-disciplinary view is not sufficient to fully understand the nature of development (Baltes, 1987).
Proposition From Baltes (1987) Corollary Arguments and Evidence
Proposition #1Development is a lifelong processFrom conception to death, development is characterized by continuous (i.e., cumulative) and discontinuous (i.e., emergent or innovative) processes; all age periods, including infancy, childhood, adolescence, adulthood, and old age, are of equal importance from the lifespan perspective (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #2Development is multidimensional and multidirectionalConsidering cognitive abilities, some indicators (e.g., working memory), on average, tend to decline with age, some remain relatively stable across the lifespan, and others (e.g., experience-based knowledge and judgment) typically improve with age (Baltes et al., 1999; Salthouse, 2012)
Proposition #3Development involves the dynamic and joint occurrence of growth (or gains) and decline (or losses) in different domains of functioningBoth learning (which leads to personal growth) and physical decline are possible in different phases of the lifespan. However, losses increasingly outweigh gains in functioning at higher ages (i.e., changing loss–gain ratio; Baltes, 1987).
Proposition #4Development is partially context-dependentDevelopment is embedded in, coincides with, influences, and is influenced by, historical time and events, sociocultural conditions, and other contextual factors or levels of organization (Baltes, 1987; Lerner, 1996).
Proposition #5Development results from the interaction of three systems of influence which define the paradigm of contextualism and the “tri-factor model” of developmental contextualismBaltes (1987) and Lerner (1996) describe normative age-graded influences (i.e., age-related person and contextual determinants that most people encounter as they age), normative history-graded influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that most people living during a certain historical period are experiencing), and non-normative influences (i.e., person and contextual determinants that are rather idiosyncratic and uncommon).
Proposition #6There is potential for plasticity, or within-person modifiability, in developmentPerson and contextual factors can positively or negatively impact an individual’s experiences and behavior at any age, and the lifespan perspective aims to better understand the range, potentials, and limits of plasticity in development (Baltes, 1987)
Proposition #7Individual development needs to be studied at multiple levels of analysisDevelopment must be understood across different levels, ranging from the biological level to the psychological and social-relational levels to the sociocultural and macro-institutional levels (Lerner, 1996). Thus, development needs to be investigated by scholars working in multiple scientific disciplines, including anthropology, biology, sociology, medical sciences, and psychology. A mono-disciplinary view is not sufficient to fully understand the nature of development (Baltes, 1987).
Close
This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

Close

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

View Article Abstract & Purchase Options

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Close