Abstract

Objectives

Poor sleep is associated with multiple age-related neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric conditions. The hippocampus plays a special role in sleep and sleep-dependent cognition, and accelerated hippocampal atrophy is typically seen with higher age. Hence, it is critical to establish how the relationship between sleep and hippocampal volume loss unfolds across the adult lifespan.

Methods

Self-reported sleep measures and MRI-derived hippocampal volumes were obtained from 3105 cognitively normal participants (18–90 years) from major European brain studies in the Lifebrain consortium. Hippocampal volume change was estimated from 5116 MRIs from 1299 participants for whom longitudinal MRIs were available, followed up to 11 years with a mean interval of 3.3 years. Cross-sectional analyses were repeated in a sample of 21,390 participants from the UK Biobank.

Results

No cross-sectional sleep—hippocampal volume relationships were found. However, worse sleep quality, efficiency, problems, and daytime tiredness were related to greater hippocampal volume loss over time, with high scorers showing 0.22% greater annual loss than low scorers. The relationship between sleep and hippocampal atrophy did not vary across age. Simulations showed that the observed longitudinal effects were too small to be detected as age-interactions in the cross-sectional analyses.

Conclusions

Worse self-reported sleep is associated with higher rates of hippocampal volume decline across the adult lifespan. This suggests that sleep is relevant to understand individual differences in hippocampal atrophy, but limited effect sizes call for cautious interpretation.

Statement of Significance

Hippocampal atrophy is commonly seen in aging and age-related brain disorders, and hippocampus is important in sleep and sleep-dependent cognition. Hippocampal volume was not related to self-reported sleep. However, participants reporting worse sleep quality, efficiency, problems, and daytime tiredness showed greater hippocampal volume loss over time. On average 0.22% greater annual loss of hippocampal volume was seen in high versus than low scorers. This suggests that sleep is relevant to understand individual differences in hippocampal atrophy, but limited effect sizes call for cautious interpretation.

Introduction

Disturbed sleep is associated with normal aging [1–3] and several age-related neurological and psychiatric conditions, including dementia [4–9]. The hippocampus plays a special role in sleep and sleep-dependent cognition [10, 11], and hippocampal atrophy increases in normal aging [12] and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [13, 14]. Rodent research has shown that sleep deprivation can reduce spine density and attenuate synaptic efficacy in the hippocampus [15], possibly through changes in neural plasticity, reduction of hippocampal cell proliferation and neurogenesis [16]. Thus, it has been suggested that the hippocampus may be especially sensitive to sleep deprivation [16] over extended time periods [17].

Inspired by the mechanistic relationship between different sleep measures and hippocampal morphology established in rodents, studies have tested the association between sleep and hippocampal volume in humans (see Table 1). Most have compared patients with different sleep-related conditions and normal controls. Many report smaller hippocampi in patients [18–22], or an inverse relationship between hippocampal volume and poor sleep [23, 24]. However, other studies found no relationship [25–27] or even larger volumes in patients [28]. Of three studies testing the relationship between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume in healthy older adults, two reported that worse sleep or fatigue was associated with lower hippocampal volume [29, 30] whereas one found no significant relationship [31]. Two studies tested the association between self-reported sleep and longitudinal changes in hippocampal volume, and neither found significant effects [32, 33]. Given the sparsity of longitudinal studies, it is important to assess the relationship between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume changes with high statistical power [34], which was the main purpose of the present study. We took advantage of the large-scale, European multi-site longitudinal Lifebrain consortium (http://www.lifebrain.uio.no/), to address the following main question: Is self-reported sleep related to hippocampal volume and volume change? We investigated this using an adult lifespan approach, testing if and how sleep, hippocampal volume, and their relationship change with age.

Table 1.

Representative studies on self-reported sleep, sleep disturbances, and hippocampal volume

ReferenceNPopulationResultComment
Patients with sleep disorders (patients/ controls)
 Dusak et al. [21]22/20Obstructive sleep apnea+Smaller hippocampus in sleepy patients (n = 12), negative correlation with excessive daytime sleepiness.
 Joo et al. [22]36/36Narcoleptics with cataplexy+Smaller hippocampus in patients, volume correlated with mean sleep and REM sleep latencies.
 Joo et al. [19]27/30Primary insomnia+Smaller volume in patients, CA1 volume correlated with PSQI and higher arousal index (polysomnography) in the patients.
 Morrell et al. [20]7/7Obstructive sleep apnea+Lower GM concentration in left hippocampus in patients.
 Morell et al. [27]60/60Obstructive sleep apneaNo differences in the hippocampus, smallerGM volume in right middle temporal gyrus in the patients.
 Neylan et al. [23]17/10PTSD+Insomnia Sleep Index and PSQI associated with smaller volumes of CA3/dendate subfields in the combined sample. No significance for other subfields. In total 5 subfields were tested.
 Noh et al. [24]20/20Primary insomnia−/+No difference in hippocampus volume. In patients, hippocampus correlated negatively with duration of insomnia and the arousal index.
 Riemann et al. [18]8/8Primary insomnia+Smaller hippocampal volume in patients.
 Rosenzweig et al. [28]32/32Obstructive sleep apneaLarger volume in patients
 Sforza et al. [74]232Older, sleep-disordered breathing+Negative correlation between worse sleep and hippocampal volume
 Spiegelhalder et al. [25]28/38Primary insomniaNo significant results
 Winkelman et al. [26]20/15Primary insomniaNo main effect on hippocampal volume. Actigraph measures of poor sleep maintenance were associated with smaller volume in patients.
Normal controls
 Alperin et al. [29]69Older+Smaller hippocampi in poor sleepers, shorter sleep duration correlated with lower volume in the full sample.
 Carvalho et al. [30]1,374Middle-aged/older+Fatigue related to smaller hippocampal volume
 Lo et al. [33]119Middle-aged/older, longitudinalNo with hippocampus change
 Sexton et al. [32]147Adult lifespan, longitudinalNo relationship between PSQI and hippocampal volume change
 Sabeti et al. [31]7990+ yearsNo relationships.
 Taki et al. [75]290Children/adolescents+Hippocampal body correlated with sleep duration during weekdays.
ReferenceNPopulationResultComment
Patients with sleep disorders (patients/ controls)
 Dusak et al. [21]22/20Obstructive sleep apnea+Smaller hippocampus in sleepy patients (n = 12), negative correlation with excessive daytime sleepiness.
 Joo et al. [22]36/36Narcoleptics with cataplexy+Smaller hippocampus in patients, volume correlated with mean sleep and REM sleep latencies.
 Joo et al. [19]27/30Primary insomnia+Smaller volume in patients, CA1 volume correlated with PSQI and higher arousal index (polysomnography) in the patients.
 Morrell et al. [20]7/7Obstructive sleep apnea+Lower GM concentration in left hippocampus in patients.
 Morell et al. [27]60/60Obstructive sleep apneaNo differences in the hippocampus, smallerGM volume in right middle temporal gyrus in the patients.
 Neylan et al. [23]17/10PTSD+Insomnia Sleep Index and PSQI associated with smaller volumes of CA3/dendate subfields in the combined sample. No significance for other subfields. In total 5 subfields were tested.
 Noh et al. [24]20/20Primary insomnia−/+No difference in hippocampus volume. In patients, hippocampus correlated negatively with duration of insomnia and the arousal index.
 Riemann et al. [18]8/8Primary insomnia+Smaller hippocampal volume in patients.
 Rosenzweig et al. [28]32/32Obstructive sleep apneaLarger volume in patients
 Sforza et al. [74]232Older, sleep-disordered breathing+Negative correlation between worse sleep and hippocampal volume
 Spiegelhalder et al. [25]28/38Primary insomniaNo significant results
 Winkelman et al. [26]20/15Primary insomniaNo main effect on hippocampal volume. Actigraph measures of poor sleep maintenance were associated with smaller volume in patients.
Normal controls
 Alperin et al. [29]69Older+Smaller hippocampi in poor sleepers, shorter sleep duration correlated with lower volume in the full sample.
 Carvalho et al. [30]1,374Middle-aged/older+Fatigue related to smaller hippocampal volume
 Lo et al. [33]119Middle-aged/older, longitudinalNo with hippocampus change
 Sexton et al. [32]147Adult lifespan, longitudinalNo relationship between PSQI and hippocampal volume change
 Sabeti et al. [31]7990+ yearsNo relationships.
 Taki et al. [75]290Children/adolescents+Hippocampal body correlated with sleep duration during weekdays.

Result: “−” indicates no relationship between hippocampus volume and sleep or an inverse relationship (e.g. higher volume in patients). “+” indicates the expected relationship between hippocampus volume and sleep, for example, smaller volume in patients or a negative correlation between sleep problems and volume. Results for other brain structures than the hippocampus are not reported.

Table 1.

Representative studies on self-reported sleep, sleep disturbances, and hippocampal volume

ReferenceNPopulationResultComment
Patients with sleep disorders (patients/ controls)
 Dusak et al. [21]22/20Obstructive sleep apnea+Smaller hippocampus in sleepy patients (n = 12), negative correlation with excessive daytime sleepiness.
 Joo et al. [22]36/36Narcoleptics with cataplexy+Smaller hippocampus in patients, volume correlated with mean sleep and REM sleep latencies.
 Joo et al. [19]27/30Primary insomnia+Smaller volume in patients, CA1 volume correlated with PSQI and higher arousal index (polysomnography) in the patients.
 Morrell et al. [20]7/7Obstructive sleep apnea+Lower GM concentration in left hippocampus in patients.
 Morell et al. [27]60/60Obstructive sleep apneaNo differences in the hippocampus, smallerGM volume in right middle temporal gyrus in the patients.
 Neylan et al. [23]17/10PTSD+Insomnia Sleep Index and PSQI associated with smaller volumes of CA3/dendate subfields in the combined sample. No significance for other subfields. In total 5 subfields were tested.
 Noh et al. [24]20/20Primary insomnia−/+No difference in hippocampus volume. In patients, hippocampus correlated negatively with duration of insomnia and the arousal index.
 Riemann et al. [18]8/8Primary insomnia+Smaller hippocampal volume in patients.
 Rosenzweig et al. [28]32/32Obstructive sleep apneaLarger volume in patients
 Sforza et al. [74]232Older, sleep-disordered breathing+Negative correlation between worse sleep and hippocampal volume
 Spiegelhalder et al. [25]28/38Primary insomniaNo significant results
 Winkelman et al. [26]20/15Primary insomniaNo main effect on hippocampal volume. Actigraph measures of poor sleep maintenance were associated with smaller volume in patients.
Normal controls
 Alperin et al. [29]69Older+Smaller hippocampi in poor sleepers, shorter sleep duration correlated with lower volume in the full sample.
 Carvalho et al. [30]1,374Middle-aged/older+Fatigue related to smaller hippocampal volume
 Lo et al. [33]119Middle-aged/older, longitudinalNo with hippocampus change
 Sexton et al. [32]147Adult lifespan, longitudinalNo relationship between PSQI and hippocampal volume change
 Sabeti et al. [31]7990+ yearsNo relationships.
 Taki et al. [75]290Children/adolescents+Hippocampal body correlated with sleep duration during weekdays.
ReferenceNPopulationResultComment
Patients with sleep disorders (patients/ controls)
 Dusak et al. [21]22/20Obstructive sleep apnea+Smaller hippocampus in sleepy patients (n = 12), negative correlation with excessive daytime sleepiness.
 Joo et al. [22]36/36Narcoleptics with cataplexy+Smaller hippocampus in patients, volume correlated with mean sleep and REM sleep latencies.
 Joo et al. [19]27/30Primary insomnia+Smaller volume in patients, CA1 volume correlated with PSQI and higher arousal index (polysomnography) in the patients.
 Morrell et al. [20]7/7Obstructive sleep apnea+Lower GM concentration in left hippocampus in patients.
 Morell et al. [27]60/60Obstructive sleep apneaNo differences in the hippocampus, smallerGM volume in right middle temporal gyrus in the patients.
 Neylan et al. [23]17/10PTSD+Insomnia Sleep Index and PSQI associated with smaller volumes of CA3/dendate subfields in the combined sample. No significance for other subfields. In total 5 subfields were tested.
 Noh et al. [24]20/20Primary insomnia−/+No difference in hippocampus volume. In patients, hippocampus correlated negatively with duration of insomnia and the arousal index.
 Riemann et al. [18]8/8Primary insomnia+Smaller hippocampal volume in patients.
 Rosenzweig et al. [28]32/32Obstructive sleep apneaLarger volume in patients
 Sforza et al. [74]232Older, sleep-disordered breathing+Negative correlation between worse sleep and hippocampal volume
 Spiegelhalder et al. [25]28/38Primary insomniaNo significant results
 Winkelman et al. [26]20/15Primary insomniaNo main effect on hippocampal volume. Actigraph measures of poor sleep maintenance were associated with smaller volume in patients.
Normal controls
 Alperin et al. [29]69Older+Smaller hippocampi in poor sleepers, shorter sleep duration correlated with lower volume in the full sample.
 Carvalho et al. [30]1,374Middle-aged/older+Fatigue related to smaller hippocampal volume
 Lo et al. [33]119Middle-aged/older, longitudinalNo with hippocampus change
 Sexton et al. [32]147Adult lifespan, longitudinalNo relationship between PSQI and hippocampal volume change
 Sabeti et al. [31]7990+ yearsNo relationships.
 Taki et al. [75]290Children/adolescents+Hippocampal body correlated with sleep duration during weekdays.

Result: “−” indicates no relationship between hippocampus volume and sleep or an inverse relationship (e.g. higher volume in patients). “+” indicates the expected relationship between hippocampus volume and sleep, for example, smaller volume in patients or a negative correlation between sleep problems and volume. Results for other brain structures than the hippocampus are not reported.

To ensure that the results were not specific to the self-report instrument (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory—PSQI) [35] and hippocampus segmentation (FreeSurfer [36]) used in Lifebrain, cross-sectional replication analyses were performed using UK Biobank (UKB) data with different self-report measures of sleep and a different hippocampal segmentation approach.

Methods

Lifebrain sample

The sample was derived from the European Lifebrain project (http://www.lifebrain.uio.no/) [37], including participants from major European brain studies: Berlin Study of Aging-II (BASE-II) [38, 39], the BETULA project [40], the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience study (Cam-CAN) [41], Center for Lifebrain Changes in Brain and Cognition longitudinal studies (LCBC) [42, 43], Whitehall-II (WH-II) [44], and University of Barcelona brain studies [45–47]. In total, self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume data from 3,105 participants (18–90 years) were included. Longitudinal information on hippocampal volume was available for 1,298 participants, yielding a total of 5,116 observations. Mean interval from first to last examination was 3.5 years (range 0.2–11.0 years). Participants were screened to be cognitively healthy and in general not suffer from conditions known to affect brain function, such as dementia, major stroke, multiple sclerosis, etc. Exact screening criteria were not identical across subsamples. Sample characteristics are presented in Table 2, and detailed information about each subsample is presented in SI.

Table 2.

Sample characteristics

StudyUnique participantsObservationsMean follow-up interval (sd)Max follow-up interval (range)Age (range)Sex (female/male)
Barcelona1452223.1 (1.2)4.3 (3.7–4.9)69 (48–90)149/73
BASE-II3156281.9 (0.7)1.9 (0.6–3.1)62 (24–81)223/405
Betula3115004.0 (0.3)4.0 (3–5)61 (25–81)251/249
Cam-CAN6479101.4 (0.7)1.4 (0.2–3.5)55 (18–89)464/446
LCBC9142,0832.9 (2.7)4.5 (0.2–11)52 (19–89)1,308/775
Whitehall-II imaging773773NANA70 (60–85)150/623
Total Lifebrain3,1055,1162.6 (2.3)3.5 (0.2–11)58 (18–90)2,545/2,571
Replication (UKB)21,39021,390NANA63 (45–81)11,237/10,153
StudyUnique participantsObservationsMean follow-up interval (sd)Max follow-up interval (range)Age (range)Sex (female/male)
Barcelona1452223.1 (1.2)4.3 (3.7–4.9)69 (48–90)149/73
BASE-II3156281.9 (0.7)1.9 (0.6–3.1)62 (24–81)223/405
Betula3115004.0 (0.3)4.0 (3–5)61 (25–81)251/249
Cam-CAN6479101.4 (0.7)1.4 (0.2–3.5)55 (18–89)464/446
LCBC9142,0832.9 (2.7)4.5 (0.2–11)52 (19–89)1,308/775
Whitehall-II imaging773773NANA70 (60–85)150/623
Total Lifebrain3,1055,1162.6 (2.3)3.5 (0.2–11)58 (18–90)2,545/2,571
Replication (UKB)21,39021,390NANA63 (45–81)11,237/10,153

Follow-up interval refers to time between MRI examinations.

Table 2.

Sample characteristics

StudyUnique participantsObservationsMean follow-up interval (sd)Max follow-up interval (range)Age (range)Sex (female/male)
Barcelona1452223.1 (1.2)4.3 (3.7–4.9)69 (48–90)149/73
BASE-II3156281.9 (0.7)1.9 (0.6–3.1)62 (24–81)223/405
Betula3115004.0 (0.3)4.0 (3–5)61 (25–81)251/249
Cam-CAN6479101.4 (0.7)1.4 (0.2–3.5)55 (18–89)464/446
LCBC9142,0832.9 (2.7)4.5 (0.2–11)52 (19–89)1,308/775
Whitehall-II imaging773773NANA70 (60–85)150/623
Total Lifebrain3,1055,1162.6 (2.3)3.5 (0.2–11)58 (18–90)2,545/2,571
Replication (UKB)21,39021,390NANA63 (45–81)11,237/10,153
StudyUnique participantsObservationsMean follow-up interval (sd)Max follow-up interval (range)Age (range)Sex (female/male)
Barcelona1452223.1 (1.2)4.3 (3.7–4.9)69 (48–90)149/73
BASE-II3156281.9 (0.7)1.9 (0.6–3.1)62 (24–81)223/405
Betula3115004.0 (0.3)4.0 (3–5)61 (25–81)251/249
Cam-CAN6479101.4 (0.7)1.4 (0.2–3.5)55 (18–89)464/446
LCBC9142,0832.9 (2.7)4.5 (0.2–11)52 (19–89)1,308/775
Whitehall-II imaging773773NANA70 (60–85)150/623
Total Lifebrain3,1055,1162.6 (2.3)3.5 (0.2–11)58 (18–90)2,545/2,571
Replication (UKB)21,39021,390NANA63 (45–81)11,237/10,153

Follow-up interval refers to time between MRI examinations.

Self-reported sleep assessment

Sleep was assessed using the PSQI [35], yielding seven domains (sleep quality, latency, duration, efficiency, problems, medication, and daytime tiredness) and a global score, over a 1-month time interval. Each domain is scored 0–3 and the global 0–21. High scores indicate worse sleep, for example, high score on the sleep duration scale means shorter sleep time. The results for the sleep medication question must be treated with caution, since most samples were screened for use of medications possibly affecting CNS function. The Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire (KSQ) [48, 49] was used for Betula. The items in KSQ cover almost perfectly the items in PSQI, and the KSQ was therefore transformed to PSQI scales (see Supplemental Information for details). Since longitudinal information on sleep was lacking for most of the sample, and sleep quality tends to be stable across intervals up to several years [50], sleep was treated as a trait variable. If multiple observations of sleep were available, these were averaged, and the mean value used in the analyses. We have previously found high stability of PSQI score across 3 years (r = 0.81 between examinations 3 years apart, see https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/335612v1).

Magnetic resonance imaging acquisition and analysis

Lifebrain MRI data originated from ten different scanners (Table 3), mainly processed with FreeSurfer 6.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) [36, 51–53] (FreeSurfer 5.3 was used for Whitehall-II), generating hippocampal and intracranial volume (ICV) estimates. Because FreeSurfer is almost fully automated, to avoid introducing possible site-specific biases, gross quality control measures were imposed and no manual editing was done. To assess the influence of scanner on hippocampal volume, seven participants were scanned on seven of the scanners (see Supplemental Information for details). There was a significant main effect of scanner on hippocampal volume (F = 4.13, p = .046) in the Travelling Brains sample. However, the between-participant rank order was almost perfectly retained between scanners, yielding a mean between-scanner Pearson correlation for bilateral hippocampal volume of r = 0.98 (range 0.94–1.00). Thus, including site as a random effect covariate in the analyses of hippocampal volume is likely sufficient to remove the influence of scanner differences. Detailed results are found in Supplementary material.

Table 3.

MR acquisition parameters

SampleScannerTeslaSequence parameters
BarcelonaTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98, TI: 900 ms, slice thickness 1 mm, flip angle: 9°, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 240 slices
BASE-IITim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,500 ms, TE: 4.77 ms, TI: 1,100 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
BetulaDiscovery GE3.0TR: 8.19 ms, TE: 3.2 ms, TI: 450 ms, flip angle: 12°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FOV 250 × 250 mm, 180 slices
Cam-CANTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,250 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 900 ms, flip angle: 9°, slice thickness 1 mm, FOV 256 × 240 mm, 192 slices
LCBCAvanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 3.61 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 m, 160 slices, iPat = 2
Avanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE = 3.79 ms, TI = 1,000 ms, flip angle = 8, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 mm, 160 slices
Skyra Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 850 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 2.22 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 0.8 mm, FoV: 240 × 256 mm, 208 slices, iPat = 2
WH-IIVerio Siemens3.0TR: 2,530 ms, TE: 1.79/3.65/5.51/7.37 ms, TI: 1,380 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 256 × 256 mm
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 1,900 ms, TE: 3.97 ms, TI: 904 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 192 × 192 mm
UKBSkyra 3T Siemens3.0TR: 2,000 ms, TI: 880 ms, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 208 × 256 mm, 256 slices, iPAT = 2
SampleScannerTeslaSequence parameters
BarcelonaTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98, TI: 900 ms, slice thickness 1 mm, flip angle: 9°, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 240 slices
BASE-IITim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,500 ms, TE: 4.77 ms, TI: 1,100 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
BetulaDiscovery GE3.0TR: 8.19 ms, TE: 3.2 ms, TI: 450 ms, flip angle: 12°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FOV 250 × 250 mm, 180 slices
Cam-CANTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,250 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 900 ms, flip angle: 9°, slice thickness 1 mm, FOV 256 × 240 mm, 192 slices
LCBCAvanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 3.61 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 m, 160 slices, iPat = 2
Avanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE = 3.79 ms, TI = 1,000 ms, flip angle = 8, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 mm, 160 slices
Skyra Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 850 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 2.22 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 0.8 mm, FoV: 240 × 256 mm, 208 slices, iPat = 2
WH-IIVerio Siemens3.0TR: 2,530 ms, TE: 1.79/3.65/5.51/7.37 ms, TI: 1,380 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 256 × 256 mm
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 1,900 ms, TE: 3.97 ms, TI: 904 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 192 × 192 mm
UKBSkyra 3T Siemens3.0TR: 2,000 ms, TI: 880 ms, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 208 × 256 mm, 256 slices, iPAT = 2

FoV = field of view, iPat = in-plane acceleration, TE = echo time, TI = inversion time, TR = repetition time.

Table 3.

MR acquisition parameters

SampleScannerTeslaSequence parameters
BarcelonaTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98, TI: 900 ms, slice thickness 1 mm, flip angle: 9°, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 240 slices
BASE-IITim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,500 ms, TE: 4.77 ms, TI: 1,100 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
BetulaDiscovery GE3.0TR: 8.19 ms, TE: 3.2 ms, TI: 450 ms, flip angle: 12°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FOV 250 × 250 mm, 180 slices
Cam-CANTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,250 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 900 ms, flip angle: 9°, slice thickness 1 mm, FOV 256 × 240 mm, 192 slices
LCBCAvanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 3.61 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 m, 160 slices, iPat = 2
Avanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE = 3.79 ms, TI = 1,000 ms, flip angle = 8, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 mm, 160 slices
Skyra Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 850 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 2.22 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 0.8 mm, FoV: 240 × 256 mm, 208 slices, iPat = 2
WH-IIVerio Siemens3.0TR: 2,530 ms, TE: 1.79/3.65/5.51/7.37 ms, TI: 1,380 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 256 × 256 mm
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 1,900 ms, TE: 3.97 ms, TI: 904 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 192 × 192 mm
UKBSkyra 3T Siemens3.0TR: 2,000 ms, TI: 880 ms, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 208 × 256 mm, 256 slices, iPAT = 2
SampleScannerTeslaSequence parameters
BarcelonaTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98, TI: 900 ms, slice thickness 1 mm, flip angle: 9°, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 240 slices
BASE-IITim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,500 ms, TE: 4.77 ms, TI: 1,100 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FoV 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
BetulaDiscovery GE3.0TR: 8.19 ms, TE: 3.2 ms, TI: 450 ms, flip angle: 12°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FOV 250 × 250 mm, 180 slices
Cam-CANTim Trio Siemens3.0TR: 2,250 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 900 ms, flip angle: 9°, slice thickness 1 mm, FOV 256 × 240 mm, 192 slices
LCBCAvanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 3.61 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 m, 160 slices, iPat = 2
Avanto Siemens1.5TR: 2,400 ms, TE = 3.79 ms, TI = 1,000 ms, flip angle = 8, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, FoV: 240 × 240 mm, 160 slices
Skyra Siemens3.0TR: 2,300 ms, TE: 2.98 ms, TI: 850 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 2,400 ms, TE: 2.22 ms, TI: 1,000 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 0.8 mm, FoV: 240 × 256 mm, 208 slices, iPat = 2
WH-IIVerio Siemens3.0TR: 2,530 ms, TE: 1.79/3.65/5.51/7.37 ms, TI: 1,380 ms, flip angle: 7°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 256 × 256 mm
Prisma Siemens3.0TR: 1,900 ms, TE: 3.97 ms, TI: 904 ms, flip angle: 8°, slice thickness: 1.0 mm, FOV: 192 × 192 mm
UKBSkyra 3T Siemens3.0TR: 2,000 ms, TI: 880 ms, slice thickness: 1 mm, FoV: 208 × 256 mm, 256 slices, iPAT = 2

FoV = field of view, iPat = in-plane acceleration, TE = echo time, TI = inversion time, TR = repetition time.

Replication sample: UK Biobank

Cross-sectional analyses were repeated using 21,390 participants from UKB (https://imaging.ukbiobank.ac.uk/) [54] (Table 2), with the sample size varying somewhat with number of missing data for each variable of interest (range 19,782–21,390). UKB does not contain PSQI, but includes several questions related to sleep (i.e. sleep duration, sleeplessness, daytime dozing/sleeping, daytime napping, problems getting up in the morning, snoring), allowing us to evaluate whether the Lifebrain results were specific to the PSQI scales. Hippocampal volume [55] and the volumetric scaling from T1 head image to standard space as proxy for ICV were used in the analyses, generated using publicly available tools, primarily based on FSL (FMRIB Software library, https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki). Details of the imaging protocol (http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/refer.cgi?id=2367) and structural image processing are provided on the UKB website (http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/refer.cgi?id=1977) (see Table 3). Sleep and MRI data were retrieved for the same participant examination. For detailed description of how the UKB data were retrieved and analyzed, see Supplementary material.

Statistical analysis

Analyses were run in R version 3.4.4 [56]. Generalized Additive Models (GAM) and Generalized Additive Mixed Models (GAMM) using the package “mgcv” version 1.8–28 [57] were used to derive age-functions for the different sleep variables and hippocampal volume, and to test the relationship between sleep, hippocampal volume, and volume change. We used smooth terms for age and sleep, random effects for subject and study, and sex as covariates. For analyses including hippocampal volume, estimated ICV was an additional covariate. Interactions between age and sex were tested in separate models. Longitudinal models were tested by including time since baseline and sleep × time as predictors. Additional models were run controlling for symptoms of depression and body mass index (BMI). The R-code and full description of the procedures and results are given in Supplementary material.

Results

Relationships between self-reported sleep and age

The association between age and sleep was significant for all PSQI subscales as well as PSQI global (Table 4 and Figure 1). The relationships varied across scales and deviated from linearity. Scores for sleep duration, efficiency, sleep problems and use of medication increased monotonously with higher age, that is, worse sleep with higher age. In contrast, sleep quality improved almost linearly, whereas problems with sleep latency were gradually reduced until about 50 years of age, before a slight increase was seen toward the end of the age range. Problems with daytime tiredness were decreased until about 70 years, before increasing in the last part of the lifespan. The global score was stable until mid-life, followed by a modest increase. These results suggest that the relationship between self-reported sleep and age is not uniform across different aspects of sleep.

Table 4.

Associations between self-reported sleep and age in Lifebrain

Sleep scaleVariablesedf/estimateF/tp
QualityAge2.233.96.008
Sex−0.07−2.65.008
LatencyAge2.874.15.004
Sex−0.26−7.711.8e−14
DurationAge4.159.001.1e−08
Sex−0.06−2.24.025
EfficiencyAge2.6727.67<2e−16
Sex−0.17−4.891.1e−06
ProblemsAge1.017.592.8e−05
Sex−0.04−2.58.01
MedicationAge1.0279.78<2e−16
Sex−0.11−3.85.0001
TirednessAge4.1313.999.4e−14
Sex−0.01−0.37.71
GlobalAge2.597.742.6e−05
Sex−0.76−6.634.0e−11
Sleep scaleVariablesedf/estimateF/tp
QualityAge2.233.96.008
Sex−0.07−2.65.008
LatencyAge2.874.15.004
Sex−0.26−7.711.8e−14
DurationAge4.159.001.1e−08
Sex−0.06−2.24.025
EfficiencyAge2.6727.67<2e−16
Sex−0.17−4.891.1e−06
ProblemsAge1.017.592.8e−05
Sex−0.04−2.58.01
MedicationAge1.0279.78<2e−16
Sex−0.11−3.85.0001
TirednessAge4.1313.999.4e−14
Sex−0.01−0.37.71
GlobalAge2.597.742.6e−05
Sex−0.76−6.634.0e−11

GAMs are presented for each sleep variable, testing a smooth function of age and a linear function of sex. Study was included as a random effect term of no interest. effective degrees of freedom (edf) and F-values are provided for age, whereas the linear estimate and the t-values are provided for sex. Negative estimates/t-values indicate lower scores for men, that is, less sleep problems. Only cross-sectional data were included in these analyses.

Table 4.

Associations between self-reported sleep and age in Lifebrain

Sleep scaleVariablesedf/estimateF/tp
QualityAge2.233.96.008
Sex−0.07−2.65.008
LatencyAge2.874.15.004
Sex−0.26−7.711.8e−14
DurationAge4.159.001.1e−08
Sex−0.06−2.24.025
EfficiencyAge2.6727.67<2e−16
Sex−0.17−4.891.1e−06
ProblemsAge1.017.592.8e−05
Sex−0.04−2.58.01
MedicationAge1.0279.78<2e−16
Sex−0.11−3.85.0001
TirednessAge4.1313.999.4e−14
Sex−0.01−0.37.71
GlobalAge2.597.742.6e−05
Sex−0.76−6.634.0e−11
Sleep scaleVariablesedf/estimateF/tp
QualityAge2.233.96.008
Sex−0.07−2.65.008
LatencyAge2.874.15.004
Sex−0.26−7.711.8e−14
DurationAge4.159.001.1e−08
Sex−0.06−2.24.025
EfficiencyAge2.6727.67<2e−16
Sex−0.17−4.891.1e−06
ProblemsAge1.017.592.8e−05
Sex−0.04−2.58.01
MedicationAge1.0279.78<2e−16
Sex−0.11−3.85.0001
TirednessAge4.1313.999.4e−14
Sex−0.01−0.37.71
GlobalAge2.597.742.6e−05
Sex−0.76−6.634.0e−11

GAMs are presented for each sleep variable, testing a smooth function of age and a linear function of sex. Study was included as a random effect term of no interest. effective degrees of freedom (edf) and F-values are provided for age, whereas the linear estimate and the t-values are provided for sex. Negative estimates/t-values indicate lower scores for men, that is, less sleep problems. Only cross-sectional data were included in these analyses.

Relationships between age and self-reported sleep in Lifebrain. Generalized additive models (GAMs) were used to obtain age-curves for each sleep variable. Higher scores indicate worse sleep. Sex was included as covariate in the analyses. Dotted lines represent 95% CI.
Figure 1.

Relationships between age and self-reported sleep in Lifebrain. Generalized additive models (GAMs) were used to obtain age-curves for each sleep variable. Higher scores indicate worse sleep. Sex was included as covariate in the analyses. Dotted lines represent 95% CI.

For all sleep variables except daytime tiredness, women reported significantly worse sleep than men. The effect sizes were generally small, however, with the effect of being female generally being less than 0.2 PSQI subscale points. The largest main effect of sex was on latency, where women reported 0.26 points more on the 0–3 points PSQI scale. For the global score, women reported 0.76 points more than men, which is equal to 13.7% of the intercept of 5.55. We also tested age × sex interactions. These were not significant for any scale, suggesting that the association between self-reported sleep and age is similar for men and women.

Self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume

Using both the cross-sectional and longitudinal MRI data to map the age-trajectory of hippocampal volume (Figure 2), we observed the expected nonlinear decline which is more pronounced from about 60 years of age (edf = 7.3, F = 374.5, p < 2e−16). This was confirmed by a significant effect of age on longitudinal change over time (edf = 13.0, F = 162.5, p < 2e−16).

Relationships between age and hippocampal volume in Lifebrain. Left panel: GAMM was used to obtain the age-curve for hippocampal volume, using both cross-sectional and longitudinal information, covarying for sex, ICV, and study (random effect). Dotted lines represent 95% CI. Right panel: Spaghetti plot of hippocampal volume and volume change for all participants, color-coded by sample. x-Axis denotes age in years, y-axis hippocampal volume in mm3.
Figure 2.

Relationships between age and hippocampal volume in Lifebrain. Left panel: GAMM was used to obtain the age-curve for hippocampal volume, using both cross-sectional and longitudinal information, covarying for sex, ICV, and study (random effect). Dotted lines represent 95% CI. Right panel: Spaghetti plot of hippocampal volume and volume change for all participants, color-coded by sample. x-Axis denotes age in years, y-axis hippocampal volume in mm3.

We tested for main effects of sleep on cross-sectional hippocampal volume. Study and Subject ID were random effects, age, sex, and ICV were included as covariates of no interest (see Supplemental Information for details). p was not below the Bonferroni-corrected threshold of 0.00625 (0.05/8) for any of the relationships. We also tested for interaction with sex and age. No interactions with sex were found, whereas age and latency showed a very weak but significant interaction (edf = 1.08, F = 8.05, p = .0038), reflecting slightly higher hippocampal offset volumes and slightly greater age effects for those reporting better sleep. In sum, the cross-sectional relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume were very modest or non-existing (Figure 3).

Relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume in Lifebrain. GAMs were used to test the cross-sectional relationship between self-reported sleep (x-axis) and hippocampal volume (y-axis). Sex, ICV, and study were used as covariates. Dotted lines represent 95% CI. x-Axis denotes sleep score, y-axis hippocampal volume in mm3.
Figure 3.

Relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume in Lifebrain. GAMs were used to test the cross-sectional relationship between self-reported sleep (x-axis) and hippocampal volume (y-axis). Sex, ICV, and study were used as covariates. Dotted lines represent 95% CI. x-Axis denotes sleep score, y-axis hippocampal volume in mm3.

Cross-sectional analyses: replication sample from UK Biobank

Relationships between age and each of the six sleep items from UKB, controlling for sex, are illustrated in Figure 4 (see Supplemental Information for details). Similar to Lifebrain, the age-relationships for all the sleep items were highly significant. Daytime dozing, frequency of napping during the day, and sleeplessness were positively related to age, as was ease of getting up in the morning. Sleep duration also showed higher values with age, but with a negative age-relationship reported by participants below 55 years of age. Snoring showed an inverse U-shaped age-relationship, with higher prevalence of snoring during midlife. The sleep items from UKB are not directly comparable to the PSQI items used in Lifebrain. Still, Daytime dozing and Nap during day increased with age, corresponding to the increase in Daytime tiredness in the last part of the age-span in the Lifebrain sample. Furthermore, sleep duration increased with age among the older participants in both UKB and Lifebrain, although the negative relationship with age until the mid-50s in UKB was not seen in Lifebrain. We tested for main effects of sex. Men reported more daytime dozing (estimate = 0.05, t = 6.98, p = 2.91e−12), that it was easier to get up in the morning (estimate = 0.19, t = 19.6, p < 2e−16), more daytime napping (estimate = 0.19, t = 23.4, p < 2e−16), longer sleep duration (estimate = 0.07, t = 4.74, p = 2.21e−6), less sleeplessness (estimate =  0.22, t = 22.5, p < 2e−16) and more snoring (estimate = 0.16, t = 23.59, p < 2e−16). In sum, similar to Lifebrain, sex had a main effect on all self-reported sleep items. However, men scored worse on the UKB items dozing and snoring, which are not identical to any of the PSQI sleep scales used in Lifebrain.

Relationships between age and self-reported sleep in UK Biobank. GAMs were used to obtain age-curves for each sleep variable. Sex was included as covariate in the analyses. Dotted lines represent 95% CI. For all items except “Sleep duration,” high scores mean poor sleep.
Figure 4.

Relationships between age and self-reported sleep in UK Biobank. GAMs were used to obtain age-curves for each sleep variable. Sex was included as covariate in the analyses. Dotted lines represent 95% CI. For all items except “Sleep duration,” high scores mean poor sleep.

The relationship between age and hippocampal volume in UKB was highly significant (edf = 7.26, F = 374.5, p < 2e−16), see Figure 5. Relationships between the UKB sleep items and hippocampal volume, controlling for age, sex, and ICV, are illustrated in Figure 6. Only sleep duration (p = 7.31e−6) was related to hippocampal volume. As values in the extreme ends of reported sleep seemed to be responsible for the relationship, we restricted the data to include sleep duration between 5 and 9 h only and reran the analysis. Still including 20,755 observations, the p-value increased to .086. No significant age-interactions were found for any of the sleep items. Thus, the UKB results were in agreement with the lack of meaningful cross-sectional sleep-hippocampal volume relationships in Lifebrain, and aligns with previous findings within a Lifebrain cohort (Cam-CAN) which found no cross-sectional associations between PSQI subcomponents and white matter microstructure (indexed by Fractional anisotropy) across 10 tracts [58].

Relationships between age and hippocampal volume in UK Biobank. GAM was used to obtain the age-curve for hippocampal volume, covarying for sex, and ICV.
Figure 5.

Relationships between age and hippocampal volume in UK Biobank. GAM was used to obtain the age-curve for hippocampal volume, covarying for sex, and ICV.

Relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume in UK Biobank. GAMs were used to test the cross-sectional relationship between self-reported sleep (x-axis) and hippocampal volume (y-axis). Sex and ICV were used as covariates. Dotted lines represent 95% CI.
Figure 6.

Relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume in UK Biobank. GAMs were used to test the cross-sectional relationship between self-reported sleep (x-axis) and hippocampal volume (y-axis). Sex and ICV were used as covariates. Dotted lines represent 95% CI.

Longitudinal analyses: self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume change

We tested if sleep was related to change in hippocampal volume over time, restricting the analyses to participants with at least two MRI examinations. Sleep quality (edf = 4.80, F = 3.69, p = .0019), efficiency (edf = 6.90, F = 6.93, p = 2.36e−08), problems (edf = 2.22, F = 5.95, p = .0017) and daytime tiredness (edf = 4.17, F = 8.99, p = 4.13e−07) showed significant associations with hippocampal volume change at the α-threshold corrected for eight tests (Figure 7). These relationships were also confirmed by comparing Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) for models with and without the PSQI × time interaction term included (see Table 5). In general, the relationships reflected participants with worse sleep showing more hippocampal volume loss over time. The exception was daytime tiredness, where worse scores were nonlinearly associated with volume loss after 3 years, exceeding those reporting no tiredness in the beginning of the interval before showing less loss in the last part. For sleep quality, only 21 participants reported the highest score. To make sure these did not unduly influence the results we repeated the analysis without these participants. Sleep quality was still significantly related to volume change at an α-threshold of 0.05, but not at the corrected threshold (edf = 4.03, F = 3.40, p = .008). All analyses were also run testing for interactions with age or sex, with no significant results.

Table 5.

Tests of sleep × time interactions in prediction of hippocampal volume change AIC: Negative values indicate better model fit for the models including the PSQI × time interaction term

PSQI × time (p)ΔAIC
Sleep variable
 Quality0.0019−5.6
 Latency0.986.0
 Duration0.0391.72
 Efficiency0.000000024−26.9
 Problems0.0017−6.1
 Medication0.133.20
 Tired0.00000041−22.6
 Global0.07026.3
PSQI × time (p)ΔAIC
Sleep variable
 Quality0.0019−5.6
 Latency0.986.0
 Duration0.0391.72
 Efficiency0.000000024−26.9
 Problems0.0017−6.1
 Medication0.133.20
 Tired0.00000041−22.6
 Global0.07026.3
Table 5.

Tests of sleep × time interactions in prediction of hippocampal volume change AIC: Negative values indicate better model fit for the models including the PSQI × time interaction term

PSQI × time (p)ΔAIC
Sleep variable
 Quality0.0019−5.6
 Latency0.986.0
 Duration0.0391.72
 Efficiency0.000000024−26.9
 Problems0.0017−6.1
 Medication0.133.20
 Tired0.00000041−22.6
 Global0.07026.3
PSQI × time (p)ΔAIC
Sleep variable
 Quality0.0019−5.6
 Latency0.986.0
 Duration0.0391.72
 Efficiency0.000000024−26.9
 Problems0.0017−6.1
 Medication0.133.20
 Tired0.00000041−22.6
 Global0.07026.3
Relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal change in Lifebrain. The plots illustrate the relationship between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume change over time. Only the four significant relationships are shown. The lines depict the hippocampal change trajectories over 5 years for those with PSQI score = 0 (no problems) or score = 2 (problems). Note that this selection was not used for the statistical analyses and is included to show the nature of the interaction with time only.
Figure 7.

Relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal change in Lifebrain. The plots illustrate the relationship between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume change over time. Only the four significant relationships are shown. The lines depict the hippocampal change trajectories over 5 years for those with PSQI score = 0 (no problems) or score = 2 (problems). Note that this selection was not used for the statistical analyses and is included to show the nature of the interaction with time only.

The interactions were further explored to assess effects sizes, as illustrated in Figure 8 (see Supplemental Information for details). We computed the expected annual change in hippocampal volume from 20, 40, 60, and 80 years, depending on whether the score on each of the four PSQI variables showing significant interactions with time was 0 or 2. Across these items, participants scoring 0 had a mean annualized reduction of hippocampal volume of −0.41% compared to −0.63% for those scoring 2. As there were no age-interactions, this difference was stable across the four tested ages, that is, −0.16 vs −0.36% at age 20 years, −0.09 vs −0.30% at age 40 years, −0.24 vs −0.47% at age 60 years, and −1.14 vs −1.39% at age 80 years for those scoring 0 vs 2, respectively. These analyses also confirmed than 1-year atrophy was higher for participants with high score in daytime tiredness, even though Figure 7 indicates that there is a shift to less atrophy after 3 years. The latter pattern is difficult to explain, and could reflect noise in the data.

Annual percent change in volume as a function of sleep. Tested at four different ages, annual reduction in hippocampal volume was on average 0.22% greater in participants scoring two compared to zero on the PSQI items quality, efficiency, problems, and daytime tiredness. Error bars denote 95% CI.
Figure 8.

Annual percent change in volume as a function of sleep. Tested at four different ages, annual reduction in hippocampal volume was on average 0.22% greater in participants scoring two compared to zero on the PSQI items quality, efficiency, problems, and daytime tiredness. Error bars denote 95% CI.

We re-analyzed the significant time-interactions using BMI and depression scores as covariates. BMI and depression did not contribute significantly and did not affect the sleep–time interactions. The most substantial effect of the additional covariates was that the p-value of sleep problems × time on hippocampal volume increased from 0.002 to 0.017, which probably was a result of lower power due to BMI and depression scores not being available for the full sample (see Supplemental Information for full results).

Finally, we ran a GAMM with all sleep variables showing significant interactions with time included simultaneously as independent variables. Efficiency (edf = 7.9, F = 3.81, p = .00025), problems (edf = 5.0, F = 3.34, p = .004) and tiredness (edf = 4.6, F = 6.68, p = 1.99e−5) were still significantly related to hippocampal volume over time, while sleep quality was not (edf = 1.0, F = 0.15, p = .70).

Comparison of longitudinal and cross-sectional results

We would expect the relationship between sleep and hippocampal volume change over time to be detected as an interaction between age and sleep on hippocampal volume, that is, the relationship between sleep and hippocampal volume would be stronger in the older part of the sample. However, the lack of cross-sectional relationships could be caused by individual differences in hippocampal volume being too large compared to the limited longitudinal effects of sleep. We used the effect size from the interaction between sleep efficiency and time to simulate whether we had power to detect this as a cross-sectional age-interaction. The simulations clearly demonstrated that whereas we had excellent power (~100%) to detect the longitudinal association in our data, the power to detect an age-interaction using the cross-sectional data was only about 10% (Figure 9). This demonstrates that the magnitude of the relationship between sleep and hippocampal change was too small to be detected in our cross-sectional dataset of more than 3,000 participants (see Supplemental Information for details).

Statistical power. The figure illustrates the superior power of the longitudinal design. The x-axis represents the size of PSQI × time (longitudinal) or PSQI × age interactions (cross-sectional). The y-axis represents statistical power. The dotted vertical line represents the observed effect size of the sleep efficiency × time interaction. As shown, the power to detect this is close to 1 (100%) with the longitudinal design, and very poor with the cross-sectional design.
Figure 9.

Statistical power. The figure illustrates the superior power of the longitudinal design. The x-axis represents the size of PSQI × time (longitudinal) or PSQI × age interactions (cross-sectional). The y-axis represents statistical power. The dotted vertical line represents the observed effect size of the sleep efficiency × time interaction. As shown, the power to detect this is close to 1 (100%) with the longitudinal design, and very poor with the cross-sectional design.

Discussion

Worse self-reported sleep was related to greater hippocampal volume reduction over time. This was seen for specific aspects of sleep, that is, quality, efficiency, problems, and daytime tiredness, where a PSQI score of 2 was associated with on average 0.22% more volume loss annually than a score of 0. Two caveats need to be considered. First, it is not clear how molecular mechanisms identified in the rodent experiments can be applied to the present findings, as variations in self-reported sleep are very different from experimentally induced sleep deprivation and the effects of specific molecular mechanisms on volume change are speculative. Second, the longitudinal effect sizes were small, and the relationships between sleep and volume were too weak to be identified in the cross-sectional data—both in Lifebrain and UKB. Implications of the findings are discussed below.

Self-reported sleep and relationship to age

All self-reported sleep measures, in Lifebrain and UKB, were significantly related to age. Whereas sleep duration was shorter, efficiency less and problems increased with higher age in Lifebrain, self-reported sleep quality was better with age, and sleep latency and daytime tiredness showed improvement until middle age or longer. The global PSQI score showed little change before 50 years of age, after which worse scores were seen for the rest of the age-span, suggesting PSQI sum score may not be the best measure to use in lifespan cohorts [58]. These patterns fit well with the results from a meta-analysis of polysomnography data [2]. Despite these findings, self-reported sleep quality was higher in older adults, in line with previous research [59].

Although the cross-sectional age-relationships were highly significant, the effects were relatively modest. For instance, sleep duration scores increased from about 0.3 at 20 years to 0.9 at 85. These numbers are based on the PSQI scoring system where 0 represents 7 h of sleep or more and 1 represents 6–7 h. Fewer hours of sleep in combination with less daytime tiredness and less sleep efficiency could suggest that older adults sleep less because their sleep needs are less. In support of this view are findings that older adults tend to sleep less despite opportunities to sleep more, they show a smaller rebound in slow wave sleep after sleep deprivation, and experience less daytime sleepiness after slow wave sleep deprivation [3]. However, arguments against this interpretation are that shorter sleep in older age may be due to desensitization to a homeostatic sleep drive, that less subjective sleepiness could be due to re-normalization of the subjective feeling of tiredness over time, and that older adults also perform worse on at least some cognitive tasks after sleep deprivation [3]. Thus, the debate on whether shorter sleep duration in aging is a result of less sleep needs or lower sleeping abilities is not settled [60].

Although the UKB data in general supported the finding of worse self-reported sleep with age, sleep duration seemed to increase through most of this older age-range, that is, from 55 years. This is in conflict with the observation in Lifebrain. The reasons for this discrepancy are not clear, but it may be noted that the increase in sleep duration over 30 years in UKB is only about a quarter of an hour. Since data are lacking for young participants in UKB, we do not know whether this represents an old-age or a life-long pattern.

We also tested for main effects of sex, and interactions between sex and age. It has been suggested that some sleep mechanisms are differentially affected by age in men and women whereas others may remain equivalent [3, 61]. For instance, sex-specific changes in the circadian alerting signal have been proposed to account for greater daytime nap propensity in older men [3]. We found that although women in general reported worse sleep than men did, in line with previous studies [62], there were no sex-specific age-effects. The above mentioned meta-analysis concluded that the associations between sleep variables and aging were similar across sexes, but that larger effects of age were observed in women for total sleep time and sleep efficiency [2]. In Lifebrain, the lack of sex × age-interactions suggests that self-reported sleep show similar age-trajectories for men and women.

Self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume change

Low sleep quality and efficiency, more sleep problems, and daytime tiredness were related to more hippocampal volume loss. Two previous longitudinal studies with reasonably large samples of 119 [33] and 147 [32] participants reported no significant effects of self-reported sleep on hippocampal change. The statistical power in the current study allowed detection of such effects with high confidence, although the effects sizes were modest. On average, participants scoring 2 on these four PSQI subscales showed 0.22% more annual volume loss than those scoring 0. This does not mean that the observed relationships between self-reported sleep and hippocampal atrophy are not important. Both sleep [1] and hippocampal volume [63] are substantially affected by chronological age, and both are sensitive to age-related degenerative conditions, such as AD [13, 14, 64]. Cognitively healthy older adults with greater initial levels of sleep fragmentation show more rapid rate of cognitive decline and higher risk of developing AD [65]. Still, multiple brain conditions or diseases also affect sleep [3], and it is not possible from the present observational study to infer the direction of causality. There could be a causal relationship where poor sleep contributes to increased hippocampal atrophy. However, hippocampal atrophy could also contribute to worse self-reported sleep. Hippocampal atrophy tends to increase from about 60 years of age, and in line with this, sleep efficiency and daytime tiredness also deteriorated after middle-age. Still, age was accounted for in the sleep–atrophy relationships, and no significant age-interactions were found. Thus, there are no results from the present study to suggest that increased age-related hippocampal atrophy is caused by—or causes—worsening of self-reported sleep in higher age. Rather, the relationship between worse self-reported sleep and hippocampal change seems to be stable across adult life, even in age-ranges with smaller hippocampal volume loss at group level. Future investigations with repeated measures of both brain structure and sleep quality may be able to examine whether neural changes are precursors to worsening sleep, or whether (negative) changes in sleep quality in old age are associated with accelerated gray matter aging.

Previous studies using physiological measures of sleep have found that hippocampal volume may be implicated in some but not all age-related differences in sleep architecture. In particular, it is speculated that impairments in the functional expression of sleep spindles [66] may be caused by age-related atrophy of cell bodies in hippocampus [3], whereas age differences in slow wave activity seem to be independent of hippocampal structure and rather connected to hippocampal function [11, 67]. How these features translate into aspects of self-reported sleep is unclear, and more importantly, none of these studies measured actual volumetric changes from repeated scanning.

Previous cross-sectional studies of patients with different sleep-related conditions [18–27] and older samples without specific sleep problems [29–31] yielded mixed results regarding the relationship between self-reported sleep and hippocampal volume. We found no cross-sectional relationships in Lifebrain or UKB. As shown in the simulation experiment, the longitudinal relationships were too weak to be detected as age-interactions in the cross-sectional analyses given the large inter-individual variation in hippocampal volume. If there was a strong longitudinal relationship between sleep and hippocampal volume change, this could have been detectable as an age-interaction also in our very large cross-sectional samples. This highlights the strength of the longitudinal research design when investigating brain structural changes.

Limitations

The study has several limitations. First, we used a self-report measure for sleep. The advantage is that sleep is measured in the participants’ natural environment and sleep is measured over a longer period, increasing ecological validity. The disadvantage is that the results reflect self-reported aspects of macro-level sleep architecture, not physiological sleep, and age-related changes in these can be mechanistically distinct from micro-level changes in physiological sleep oscillations [3]. Studies directly testing the relationships between self-reported sleep and objectively measured sleep parameters report correlations of moderate sizes, typically around 0.40 [68–73]. Such results demonstrate some validity of the self-report measures, while also suggesting that it is difficult to make claims about the underlying sleep physiology based on such measures. Second, although the vast majority of the participants were screened for cognitive problems, we did not screen the UKB participants, and conservative screening was not performed for all subsamples in the Lifebrain cohort. Still, the results appear robust and not driven by outliers, so we do not believe sample heterogeneity has affected the outcome. Third, since we lacked adequate longitudinal observations of the sleep variables for a substantial part of the sample, sleep was studied as a trait. This prevented us from addressing dynamic changes in sleep within individuals, which would have been very interesting to relate to hippocampal volume change. Finally, we only tested the relationship with hippocampal atrophy. There are reasons to expect relationships between sleep and other brain regions [3, 32], or white matter structure, which will be topic of later studies.

Conclusion

The present study showed that specific aspects of self-reported sleep—quality, efficiency, problems, and daytime tiredness—are related to increased hippocampal volume loss over time. In the largest study to date combining data from multiple cohorts, we observe modest longitudinal effects and negligible to absent cross-sectional effects. Together these findings contribute to our understanding of hippocampal volume loss across the adult lifespan.

Funding

The Lifebrain project is funded by the EU Horizon 2020 Grant: ‘Healthy minds 0–100 years: Optimising the use of European brain imaging cohorts (‘Lifebrain’).” Grant agreement number: 732592. Call: Societal challenges: Health, demographic change and well-being. In addition, the different sub-studies are supported by different sources: LCBC: The European Research Council’s Starting/Consolidator Grant schemes under grant agreements 283634, 725025 (to A.M.F.) and 313440 (to K.B.W.), as well as the Norwegian Research Council (to A.M.F., K.B.W.), The National Association for Public Health’s dementia research program, Norway (to A.M.F) and the Medical Student Research Program at the University of Oslo. Betula: a scholar grant from the Knut and Alice Wallenberg (KAW) foundation to L.N. Barcelona: Partially supported by a Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO) grant to D-BF [grant number PSI2015-64227-R (AEI/FEDER, UE)]; by the Walnuts and Healthy Aging study (http://www.clinicaltrials.gov; Grant NCT01634841) funded by the California Walnut Commission, Sacramento, California. BASE-II has been supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research under grant numbers 16SV5537/16SV5837/16SV5538/16SV5536K/01UW0808/01UW0706/01GL1716A/01GL1716B, and S.K. has received support from the European Research Council under grant agreement 677804. Work on the Whitehall II Imaging Substudy was mainly funded by Lifelong Health and Well-being Programme Grant G1001354 from the UK Medical Research Council (“Predicting MRI Abnormalities with Longitudinal Data of the Whitehall II Substudy”) to Dr Ebmeier. The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from award 203139/Z/16/Z from the Wellcome Trust. Drs Suri and Zsoldos were funded by award 1117747 from the HDH Wills 1965 Charitable Trust). Dr Suri is now funded by the UK Alzheimer’s Society; CES is supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. Part of the research was conducted using the UK Biobank resource under application number 32048.

Conflict of interest statement. C.E.S. reports consulting fees from Jazz Pharmaceuticals. C.A.D. is a cofounder, stock-owner, board member and consultant in the contract laboratory Vitas AS, performing personalized analyses of blood biomarkers.

References

1.

Scullin
MK
, et al.
Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: integrating a half century of multidisciplinary research
.
Perspect Psychol Sci.
2015
;
10
(
1
):
97
137
.

2.

Ohayon
MM
, et al.
Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan
.
Sleep.
2004
;
27
(
7
):
1255
1273
.

3.

Mander
BA
, et al.
Sleep and human aging
.
Neuron.
2017
;
94
(
1
):
19
36
.

4.

Shi
L
, et al.
Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Sleep Med Rev.
2018
;
40
:
4
16
.

5.

Hatfield
CF
, et al.
Disrupted daily activity/rest cycles in relation to daily cortisol rhythms of home-dwelling patients with early Alzheimer’s dementia
.
Brain.
2004
;
127
(
Pt 5
):
1061
1074
.

6.

Videnovic
A
, et al.
‘The clocks that time us’–circadian rhythms in neurodegenerative disorders
.
Nat Rev Neurol.
2014
;
10
(
12
):
683
693
.

7.

Prinz
PN
, et al.
Sleep, EEG and mental function changes in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type
.
Neurobiol Aging.
1982
;
3
(
4
):
361
370
.

8.

Irwin
MR
, et al.
Vitiello, Implications of sleep disturbance and inflammation for Alzheimer’s disease dementia
.
Lancet Neurol.
2019
.

9.

Mander
BA
, et al.
Sleep: a novel mechanistic pathway, biomarker, and treatment target in the pathology of Alzheimer’s Disease?
Trends Neurosci.
2016
;
39
(
8
):
552
566
.

10.

Krause
AJ
, et al.
The sleep-deprived human brain
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2017
;
18
(
7
):
404
418
.

11.

Mander
BA
, et al.
Impaired prefrontal sleep spindle regulation of hippocampal-dependent learning in older adults
.
Cereb Cortex.
2014
;
24
(
12
):
3301
3309
.

12.

Fjell
AM
, et al.
Critical ages in the life course of the adult brain: nonlinear subcortical aging
.
Neurobiol Aging.
2013
;
34
(
10
):
2239
2247
.

13.

Holland
D
, et al.
Subregional neuroanatomical change as a biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
2009
;
106
(
49
):
20954
20959
.

14.

Braskie
MN
, et al.
A focus on structural brain imaging in the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2014
;
75
(
7
):
527
533
.

15.

Raven
F
, et al.
The role of sleep in regulating structural plasticity and synaptic strength: implications for memory and cognitive function
.
Sleep Med Rev.
2018
;
39
:
3
11
.

16.

Kreutzmann
JC
, et al.
Sleep deprivation and hippocampal vulnerability: changes in neuronal plasticity, neurogenesis and cognitive function
.
Neuroscience.
2015
;
309
:
173
190
.

17.

Novati
A
, et al.
Chronic sleep restriction causes a decrease in hippocampal volume in adolescent rats, which is not explained by changes in glucocorticoid levels or neurogenesis
.
Neuroscience.
2011
;
190
:
145
155
.

18.

Riemann
D
, et al.
Chronic insomnia and MRI-measured hippocampal volumes: a pilot study
.
Sleep.
2007
;
30
(
8
):
955
958
.

19.

Joo
EY
, et al.
Brain gray matter deficits in patients with chronic primary insomnia
.
Sleep.
2013
;
36
(
7
):
999
1007
.

20.

Morrell
MJ
, et al.
Changes in brain morphology associated with obstructive sleep apnea
.
Sleep Med.
2003
;
4
(
5
):
451
454
.

21.

Dusak
A
, et al.
Correlation between hippocampal volume and excessive daytime sleepiness in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome
.
Eur Rev Med Pharmacol Sci.
2013
;
17
(
9
):
1198
1204
.

22.

Joo
EY
, et al.
Hippocampal volume and memory in narcoleptics with cataplexy
.
Sleep Med.
2012
;
13
(
4
):
396
401
.

23.

Neylan
TC
, et al.
Insomnia severity is associated with a decreased volume of the CA3/dentate gyrus hippocampal subfield
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2010
;
68
(
5
):
494
496
.

24.

Noh
HJ
, et al.
The relationship between hippocampal volume and cognition in patients with chronic primary insomnia
.
J Clin Neurol.
2012
;
8
(
2
):
130
138
.

25.

Spiegelhalder
K
, et al.
Insomnia does not appear to be associated with substantial structural brain changes
.
Sleep.
2013
;
36
(
5
):
731
737
.

26.

Winkelman
JW
, et al.
Lack of hippocampal volume differences in primary insomnia and good sleeper controls: an MRI volumetric study at 3 Tesla
.
Sleep Med.
2010
;
11
(
6
):
576
582
.

27.

Morrell
MJ
, et al.
Changes in brain morphology in patients with obstructive sleep apnoea
.
Thorax.
2010
;
65
(
10
):
908
914
.

28.

Rosenzweig
I
, et al.
Hippocampal hypertrophy and sleep apnea: a role for the ischemic preconditioning?
PLoS One.
2013
;
8
(
12
):
e83173
.

29.

Alperin
,
N.
, et al.
Effect of sleep quality on aMCI vulnerable brain regions in cognitively normal elderly individuals
.
Sleep.
2018
;
42
(
3
)
. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsy254.

30.

Carvalho
DZ
, et al.
Excessive daytime sleepiness and fatigue may indicate accelerated brain aging in cognitively normal late middle-aged and older adults
.
Sleep Med.
2017
;
32
:
236
243
.

31.

Sabeti
S
, et al.
Sleep, hippocampal volume, and cognition in adults over 90 years old
.
Aging Clin Exp Res.
2018
;
30
(
11
):
1307
1318
.

32.

Sexton
CE
, et al.
Poor sleep quality is associated with increased cortical atrophy in community-dwelling adults
.
Neurology.
2014
;
83
(
11
):
967
973
.

33.

Lo
JC
, et al.
Sleep duration and age-related changes in brain structure and cognitive performance
.
Sleep.
2014
;
37
(
7
):
1171
1178
.

34.

Keage
HAD
.
Sleep and cognitive aging: emerging bedfellows: editorial for Carvalho
et al.
Sleep Med.
2017
;
32
:
244
245
.

35.

Buysse
DJ
, et al.
The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research
.
Psychiatry Res.
1989
;
28
(
2
):
193
213
.

36.

Fischl
B
, et al.
Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain
.
Neuron.
2002
;
33
(
3
):
341
355
.

37.

Walhovd
KB
, et al.
Healthy minds 0-100 years: optimising the use of European brain imaging cohorts (“Lifebrain”)
.
Eur Psychiatry.
2018
;
50
:
47
56
.

38.

Bertram
L
, et al.
Cohort profile: the Berlin Aging Study II (BASE-II)
.
Int J Epidemiol.
2014
;
43
(
3
):
703
712
.

39.

Gerstorf
D
, et al.
Editorial
.
Gerontology.
2016
;
62
(
3
):
311
315
.

40.

Nilsson
LG
, et al. ,
The Betula prospective cohort study: memory, health, and aging
.
Aging Neuropsychol Cogn.
1997
;
4
:
1
32
.

41.

Shafto
MA
, et al.
The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) study protocol: a cross-sectional, lifespan, multidisciplinary examination of healthy cognitive ageing
.
BMC Neurol.
2014
;
14
:
204
.

42.

Walhovd
KB
, et al.
Neurodevelopmental origins of lifespan changes in brain and cognition
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
2016
;
113
(
33
):
9357
9362
.

43.

Fjell
AM
, et al.
Neuroinflammation and Tau Interact with Amyloid in Predicting Sleep Problems in Aging Independently of Atrophy
.
Cereb Cortex.
2018
;
28
(
8
):
2775
2785
.

44.

Filippini
N
, et al.
Study protocol: the Whitehall II imaging sub-study
.
BMC Psychiatry.
2014
;
14
:
159
.

45.

Abellaneda-Pérez
K
, et al.
Age-related differences in default-mode network connectivity in response to intermittent theta-burst stimulation and its relationships with maintained cognition and brain integrity in healthy aging
.
Neuroimage.
2019
;
188
:
794
806
.

46.

Rajaram
S
, et al.
The Walnuts and Healthy Aging Study (WAHA): protocol for a nutritional intervention trial with walnuts on brain aging
.
Front Aging Neurosci.
2016
;
8
:
333
.

47.

Vidal-Piñeiro
D
, et al.
Task-dependent activity and connectivity predict episodic memory network-based responses to brain stimulation in healthy aging
.
Brain Stimul.
2014
;
7
(
2
):
287
296
.

48.

Westerlund
A
, et al.
Using the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire to identify obstructive sleep apnea syndrome in a sleep clinic population
.
Clin Respir J.
2014
;
8
(
4
):
444
454
.

49.

Nordin
M
, et al.
Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire
.
Sleep Biol Rhythms
.
2013
;
11
:
216
226
.

50.

Pillai
V
, et al.
The nature of stable insomnia phenotypes
.
Sleep.
2015
;
38
(
1
):
127
138
.

51.

Dale
AM
, et al.
Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction
.
Neuroimage.
1999
;
9
(
2
):
179
194
.

52.

Reuter
M
, et al.
Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis
.
Neuroimage.
2012
;
61
(
4
):
1402
1418
.

53.

Jovicich
J
, et al.
Brain morphometry reproducibility in multi-center 3T MRI studies: a comparison of cross-sectional and longitudinal segmentations
.
Neuroimage.
2013
;
83
:
472
484
.

54.

Miller
KL
, et al.
Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study
.
Nat Neurosci.
2016
;
19
(
11
):
1523
1536
.

55.

Alfaro-Almagro
F
, et al.
Image processing and Quality Control for the first 10,000 brain imaging datasets from UK Biobank
.
Neuroimage.
2018
;
166
:
400
424
.

56.

Team, R.C.
,
R: A language and environment for statistical computing
.
Vienna, Austria
:
R Foundation for Statistical Computing
;
2018
. URL https://www.R-project.org/

57.

Wood
SN.
Generalized Additive Models: An Introduction with R
.
Boca Raton, FL
:
Chapman and Hall/CRC;
2006
.

58.

Gadie
A
, et al.
How are age-related differences in sleep quality associated with health outcomes? An epidemiological investigation in a UK cohort of 2406 adults
.
BMJ Open.
2017
;
7
(
7
):
e014920
.

59.

Luca
G
, et al.
Age and gender variations of sleep in subjects without sleep disorders
.
Ann Med.
2015
;
47
(
6
):
482
491
.

60.

Scullin
MK
.
Do older adults need sleep? A review of neuroimaging, sleep, and aging studies
.
Curr Sleep Med Rep.
2017
;
3
(
3
):
204
214
.

61.

Redline
S
, et al.
The effects of age, sex, ethnicity, and sleep-disordered breathing on sleep architecture
.
Arch Intern Med.
2004
;
164
(
4
):
406
418
.

62.

Smagula
SF
, et al.
Risk factors for sleep disturbances in older adults: evidence from prospective studies
.
Sleep Med Rev.
2016
;
25
:
21
30
.

63.

Walhovd
KB
, et al.
Consistent neuroanatomical age-related volume differences across multiple samples
.
Neurobiol Aging.
2011
;
32
(
5
):
916
932
.

64.

Peter-Derex
L
, et al.
Sleep and Alzheimer’s disease
.
Sleep Med Rev.
2015
;
19
:
29
38
.

65.

Lim
AS
, et al.
Sleep fragmentation and the risk of incident Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline in older persons
.
Sleep.
2013
;
36
(
7
):
1027
1032
.

66.

Fogel
S
, et al.
Sleep spindles: a physiological marker of age-related changes in gray matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation
.
Neurobiol Aging.
2017
;
49
:
154
164
.

67.

Mander
BA
, et al.
Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging
.
Nat Neurosci.
2013
;
16
(
3
):
357
364
.

68.

Combs
D
, et al.
Mother knows best? comparing child report and parent report of sleep parameters with polysomnography
.
J Clin Sleep Med.
2019
;
15
(
1
):
111
117
.

69.

Lauderdale
DS
, et al.
Self-reported and measured sleep duration: how similar are they?
Epidemiology.
2008
;
19
(
6
):
838
845
.

70.

O’Brien
E
, et al.
Discrepancies between self-reported usual sleep duration and objective measures of total sleep time in treatment-seeking overweight and obese individuals
.
Behav Sleep Med.
2016
;
14
(
5
):
539
549
.

71.

Biddle
DJ
, et al.
Accuracy of self-reported sleep parameters compared with actigraphy in young people with mental ill-health
.
Sleep Health.
2015
;
1
(
3
):
214
220
.

72.

Cespedes
EM
, et al.
Comparison of self-reported sleep duration with actigraphy: results from the hispanic community health study/study of latinos sueño ancillary study
.
Am J Epidemiol.
2016
;
183
(
6
):
561
573
.

73.

Jackson
CL
, et al.
Agreement between self-reported and objectively measured sleep duration among white, black, Hispanic, and Chinese adults in the United States: multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis
.
Sleep.
2018
;
41
(
6
). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsy057

74.

Sforza
E
, et al.
Hippocampus volume and subjective sleepiness in older people with sleep-disordered breathing: a preliminary report
.
Journal of sleep research.
2016
;
25
:190–193.
6

75.

Taki
Y
, et al.
Sleep duration during weekdays affects hippocampal gray matter volume in healthy children
.
NeuroImage.
2012
;
60
:471–475.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.