Abstract

Background and Hypothesis

Many patients with psychiatric disorders show increased temporal discounting (TD), ie, they discount future rewards more steeply than healthy controls. However, findings for schizophrenia and schizotypy, a personality constellation considered to be on the schizophrenia spectcrum, are less clear. Moreover, the role of future time representation in TD in the schizophrenia spectrum has not been examined. We hypothesized positive associations between schizotypal personality traits and TD and reduced TD when the timepoint of future rewards is represented in dates rather than delay units (the date/delay effect). Further, we explored associations between schizotypy and the magnitude of the date/delay effect.

Study Design

We conducted a large-scale, general-population online study (N = 1000) assessing TD with the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ) and schizotypal traits with the Short Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (sO-LIFE). Time representation in the MCQ (dates vs delays) was manipulated within subject.

Study Results

Associations between TD and sO-LIFE subscales were not significant after Bonferroni correction (all r ≤ .06). The date/delay effect was successfully replicated (P < .001, gav = 0.22). Interestingly, higher values in the sO-LIFE Unusual Experiences subscale predicted the magnitude of the date/delay effect when controlling for influences of other sO-LIFE subscales, age, education, and drug use.

Conclusions

TD was not associated with schizotypy, but individuals with higher levels of positive schizotypy were more sensitive to manipulations of the representation of future timepoints. Future studies should focus on these processes as potential mechanisms in the development and treatment of cognitive-perceptual deficits in the schizophrenia spectrum.

Introduction

Schizotypy refers to a personality constellation resembling schizophrenia symptoms on a subclinical level and reflecting vulnerability for schizophrenia.1 Schizotypy comprises positive, negative, and disorganized dimensions,2 although some conceptualizations include impulsive nonconformity as a fourth dimension.3 The fully dimensional approach, conceptualizing schizotypy as personality dimensions within the general population,4 has gained support by studies revealing a continuum between schizotypal traits and psychotic disorders like schizophrenia.5–10 Therefore, studies examining schizotypy in the general population do not only allow conclusions to be drawn about this multifaceted construct itself but also about the etiology and mechanisms underlying schizophrenia5,9,11 whilst excluding confounds such as medication.9 For instance, higher levels of schizotypy are, like schizophrenia, associated with deficits in cognitive functions such as attention, working memory, and language,7,11,12 albeit with less pronounced effect sizes.9 Parallels between schizotypy and schizophrenia can also be found in maladaptive behaviors such as increased nicotine13–16 and cannabis consumption,14,17,18 or decreased physical activity,19,20 pointing to more impulsive decision making and deficits in pursuing long-term goals in individuals on the schizophrenia spectrum.

Impulsive decision making can be assessed with intertemporal choice tasks, in which participants choose between smaller rewards available sooner and larger rewards available later. This phenomenon of subjectively devaluing future rewards is known as delay discounting or temporal discounting (TD).21 Steeper TD is related to individual differences in impulsivity22–24 and to a broad range of mental disorders,25 including schizophrenia.26–33 Steeper TD has also been found in high schizotypy.34–36 However, for both schizophrenia and schizotypy, findings are not entirely consistent.31,36–40 Given that only few studies have examined associations between schizotypy and TD, with most applying an extreme group approach,34,35 further research is warranted to explore the entire continuum.

Beyond establishing the association between schizophrenia spectrum phenotypes and TD, its potential origins remain unclear. Hypotheses33,34 include roles of cognitive deficits, genetics, smoking (given that smoking is both highly prevalent in schizophrenia13,15 and related to TD41) and disease symptoms and medication. Importantly, studies in schizophrenia patients revealed that cognitive deficits at least partly play a role,26–30,32,33 as patients represent future rewards differently than controls. In intertemporal choice tasks, divergent representations of future rewards may have three causes: aberrant valuation of rewards,42–44 aberrant valuation of (future) time,44–47 and aberrant cognitive mechanisms or strategies in the integration of reward and time information.48,49

All of these can potentially be observed in schizophrenia. First, schizophrenia patients show deviations in reward processing and valuation.50–52 Particularly, they show reduced motivational drive and anticipatory pleasure than healthy controls51,53–55 and use less—or biased—information on reward, probability, and effort to make decisions.31,55–58 As a result, they are less sensitive to changes in these environmental variables, ultimately leading to potentially maladaptive and impulsive behaviors.57 Second, schizophrenia patients show altered time processing and perception,59–61 resulting in decreased precision (ie, higher variability) and, in some tasks, aberrant accuracy of time estimates. A recent meta-analysis60 showed that schizophrenia patients perceive durations, depending on the task, as either longer or shorter than objective time, reflecting altered internal clock mechanisms62 and/or cognitive deficiencies relating to working memory.63 Third, in agreement with working memory deficits in schizophrenia, schizophrenia patients show impairments in integrating affective and cognitive decision features, such as reward and time information of two options.50,64

Importantly, altered time estimation in schizophrenia not only concerns present time60 but also future time,30,65 ie, estimating time periods that individuals have not yet experienced.46 Since the latter is essential in intertemporal choices,46,66,67 it may be that schizophrenia spectrum individuals subjectively overestimate reward delays in intertemporal choice tasks.66,68,69 In combination with above-mentioned differences in reward processing, which, incidentally, also lead to less anticipatory pleasure for more distal rather than proximal rewards,34,70,71 overestimation of delay would ultimately lead to increased impatience and more impulsive behaviors.

The same argumentation may apply to schizotypy, where deviations in reward35,54,72 and time processing73,74 have also been found. However, it should be mentioned that for schizotypy, studies of the complete continuum are scarce and results are less consistent than for schizophrenia, particularly with regards to time perception where findings mostly refer to retrospective time bisection tasks73–75 that generally revealed evidence of a decelerated internal clock.60

Thus, increased TD in the schizophrenia spectrum might be caused by alterations in reward or time processing or their integration. While previous studies have emphasized the role of reward processing,27,34 here we wished to determine whether TD differences are partly due to altered time processing. Given that intertemporal choice tasks involve both reward- and time-related information, measuring the influence of either in isolation is difficult. However, differences in choice due to time perception may be examined using a framing that specifically manipulates sensitivity to time: Usually, time in intertemporal choice tasks is presented in delay units (ie, days) from the present. However, when time is framed in dates, TD is reduced compared to the standard delay condition, an effect known as the date/delay effect.76–79 While the exact cognitive mechanisms underlying the effect are not entirely clear, time sensitivity likely plays a major role. Specifically, time periods represented by dates are perceived as shorter than those represented by time units.80 Thus, this manipulation of time attributes can be drawn upon to offer novel insights into the mechanisms that may underlie altered intertemporal choice in schizotypy.

In this preregistered study, we assessed schizotypal personality traits in a large, general-population sample (N = 1000) to investigate two primary research goals: First, given inconsistent associations between TD and schizotypy,34–36,40 we aimed to examine, for the first time, the association of TD with the four dimensions of schizotypy assessed by the Short Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (sO-LIFE).81 Second, we attempted to replicate the date/delay effect and relate it to schizotypy in order to draw conclusions about the mechanisms of decision making impairments in the schizophrenia spectrum. We hypothesized that (1) TD is positively associated with schizotypy81 in the original delay condition and that (2) the date/delay effect76,77 is replicated, and we planned to investigate whether the magnitude of the date/delay effect is associated with schizotypy.

Methods

Participants

Based on an a priori power analysis with G*Power,82 we aimed for 1000 participants (≥18 years), providing 88.68% power to reveal a correlation of r = 0.1 between schizotypy and TD (two-tailed alpha level of 0.05; see preregistration: https://osf.io/7jxf9). To exclude careless responders, we used 10 items adapted from Jackson’s Infrequency Scale83 (eg, “Have you ever kept a pet monkey for years?“) embedded within the sO-LIFE. All participants who gave at least one “infrequent” answer were excluded before data analysis. Additionally, we followed recommendations84 to exclude participants with low consistency in their response patterns in the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ).85 Specifically, we excluded participants with consistency of <0.80 within any of the different magnitudes (small, medium, large) of either MCQ version (delay/date).

The study was advertised via social media and student mailing lists and data were collected online via Unipark (Tivian XI GmbH, Cologne, Germany). As incentives, participants were given the opportunity to take part in a lottery to win one of five Amazon vouchers (€25 each) and psychology students could additionally receive course credit (which was erroneously not mentioned in our preregistration). Study procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the Department of Psychology of the University of Bonn. In total, 1338 participants completed the study, 338 of whom had to be excluded due to infrequency or consistency criteria, leaving a final sample of N = 1000. All participants provided written consent. Participants (25.6% male, 73.9% female, 0.5% diverse) were on average 29.93 years old (SD = 11.88; range 18–81) and had relatively high educational levels (Supplementary Material A: 9.7% low, 50.5% medium, 38.8% high education; no response: 1.0%).

Measures

Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ)

To assess TD, we used the MCQ85 in a German version. The MCQ comprises 27 decisions between smaller-immediate (SIR) and larger-later rewards (LLR). Items can be divided into three different levels of reward magnitudes, with 9 items at each magnitude level: small (LLR range: €25–€35), medium (LLR range: €50–€60), and large (LLR range: €75–€85). The MCQ has been validated in clinical and nonclinical populations24,85 and shows high test–retest reliability.86 As we wanted to examine the date/delay effect,76,77 two different MCQ versions were used: the time interval until receipt of the LLR was presented either as number of days (eg, "in 117 days") or as dates (eg, "on 30th November 2023"). Participants completed both versions. Choices were hypothetical; however, participants were instructed to answer as if they would actually receive the rewards. The presentation order of items was as in Kirby et al.,85 but the presentation order of the two versions was randomized. Choice consistency and hyperbolic discount rates were estimated based on previous procedures85 and syntax.84 After checking for sufficiently consistent choices, discount rates for each of the 3 magnitude levels in each version were combined into a single index (k) for each version by calculating their geometric mean. Subsequently, discount rates were log-transformed due to skewness (delay: skew = 5.62, SE = 0.08; date: skew = 6.93, SE = 0.08).

Short Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (sO-LIFE)

The sO-LIFE81 comprises 43 items covering four schizotypy dimensions: (1) unusual experiences (UE), reflecting positive schizotypy (eg, odd perceptual experiences, magical thinking); (2) cognitive disorganization (CD), reflecting disorganized schizotypy (eg, difficulties in attention and decision making, social anxiety); (3) introvertive anhedonia (IA), reflecting negative schizotypy (eg, lack of pleasure, avoidance of intimacy); and (iv) impulsive nonconformity (IN), reflecting impulsive and eccentric behaviors (eg, lack of self-control, antisociality). sO-LIFE subscales show high test–retest-reliabilities (0.69–0.87 for a 1-month interval) and acceptable-to-high internal consistencies (0.62–0.80).87 Here, a German version was administered.88 Participants provided dichotomous responses (“no” or “yes”) and a sum score was computed for each subscale.

Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST)

The ASSIST (version 3.1)89,90 is a measure developed by the World Health Organization (WHO) to screen for problematic substance use, resulting in risk scores for a range of psychoactive substances (such as tobacco products, alcohol, cannabis, etc.). For each substance, up to 8 questions are asked, referring to amount, craving and negative consequences of drug use and failed abstinence attempts. Given that our study was conducted online, we used the ASSIST as self-report measure. Alcohol items were omitted since we also used the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT).91 Scoring was done according to the manual,89 with a potential sum score range from 0 to 33 (due to a programming error, question number 3 of the ASSIST was not displayed for any drug, which is why the sum score does not go up to 39 and is not comparable to other studies; given that we only use a small part of the ASSIST as control variable, the missing item is unlikely to influence the results). If a participant indicated that they never had consumed any of the substances (item 1) nor any of them in the last three months (item 2), all subsequent questions were skipped, resulting in a score of 0. In our statistical analysis, we solely used the tobacco data, due to the large number of nonconsumers of the other drugs.

Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)

The AUDIT91 is a measure developed by the WHO to identify problematic alcohol use and to screen for excessive drinking. It contains 10 items on hazardous alcohol use, dependence symptoms, and harmful alcohol use. Questions from the German version92 were answered on 3- or 5-point Likert scales assessing the existence and frequency of harmful alcohol behaviors. Scoring was done according to the manual,91 with a potential sum score range from 0 to 40. Questions 2 to 10 were skipped if a participant indicated they never had drinks containing alcohol (item 1), resulting in a score of 0.

Statistical Analysis

Data analysis was conducted in R (version 4.2.2) using RStudio (version 2022.12.0). All statistical tests were done using a 2-tailed alpha level of .05, unless specified otherwise.

To investigate associations between schizotypy and TD, in a preregistered analysis we calculated Pearson correlations between sum scores of the four subscales and ln(k) in the standard delay version (Bonferroni-corrected alpha level of .05/4 = .0125). Furthermore, following a reviewer comment, we calculated nonpreregistered t-tests to compare schizotypy low- and high-scorers (with grouping based on the lower and upper 10% of the respective subscale score35). In additional nonpreregistered exploratory analyses, we examined Pearson correlations between ln(k) in the date version and subscale scores, and used multiple linear regressions to explore associations while controlling for influences of (1) the other subscales and (2) age, dummy-coded education (low/medium/high), alcohol, and tobacco consumption as well as the other subscales (hierarchical linear regression).

To determine whether the date/delay effect was replicated, we conducted a preregistered paired sample t-test with MCQ version (date vs delay) as independent variable and ln(k) as dependent variable. Hedges gav was computed as measure of effect size.93 For explorative purposes, we conducted a nonpreregistered 2 × 3 (version × magnitude) within-subject ANOVA to analyze whether the date/delay effect interacted with reward magnitude (small, medium, large). Also, we tested the effect of MCQ presentation order (delay first vs date first) on TD and the date/delay effect in a nonpreregistered 2 × 2 (version × order) mixed ANOVA.

We then examined whether the magnitude of the date/delay effect was associated with schizotypy. First, in a preregistered explorative analysis, we considered correlations between ln(k) in the two versions and schizotypy scores and compared them using Williams t-test94 (Bonferroni-corrected alpha level of 0.05/4 = 0.0125). Next, using the previously created groups, we calculated nonpreregistered t-tests comparing schizotypy low- and high-scorers on the date/delay effect. Finally, in a nonpreregistered exploratory analysis, we used multiple linear regressions to examine whether the difference in ln(k) between the two versions (delay minus date) was predicted by any of the four sO-LIFE subscale scores, controlling for influences of age, dummy-coded education, alcohol and tobacco consumption as well as the other subscales (hierarchical linear regression).

Results

Descriptive statistics, Cronbach’s alphas and Pearson correlations are shown in table 1. The association between schizotypy and TD in the delay condition was significant for UE, r(998) = .06, P = .04, but only at the nonadjusted alpha level. When adjusting for multiple comparisons, none of the associations between schizotypal traits and TD were significant, neither in the delay nor in the date condition (all P > .0125). Similarly, comparing TD in schizotypy high- and low-scorers using a nonpreregistered extreme group approach did not reveal any significant differences (Supplementary Material B1–B2). Nonpreregistered multiple linear regressions in which sO-LIFE subscale scores were added as predictors for discount rates as well as hierarchical linear regressions in which age, dummy-coded education, alcohol and tobacco consumption were entered in the first and subscale scores in the second step only yielded significant effects of age, education, and nicotine. Associations between subscale scores and discount rates were nonsignificant (Supplementary Material B3–B6).

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics, Cronbach’s Alphas, and Pearson Correlations

VariableMSDSk(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
(1) ln(k) Delay−5.731.70−0.05(0.93)
(2) ln(k) Date−6.101.650.190.91***(0.93)
(3) Date/Delay Effect0.370.710.810.27***−0.15***(0.25)
(4) Unusual Experiences3.772.570.500.06*0.040.06(0.71)
(5) Cognitive Disorganization4.892.960.220.020.03−0.040.40***(0.77)
(6) Introvertive Anhedonia2.071.801.020.020.04−0.050.09**0.32***(0.56)
(7) Impulsive Nonconformity3.141.940.570.040.06−0.040.40***0.39***0.10**(0.52)
(8) Alcohol (AUDIT)5.144.871.41<0.010.03−0.050.020.11***−0.07*0.25***(0.77)
(9) Tobacco (ASSIST)2.844.861.810.11***0.10**0.040.22***0.16***0.020.30***0.23***(0.58)
VariableMSDSk(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
(1) ln(k) Delay−5.731.70−0.05(0.93)
(2) ln(k) Date−6.101.650.190.91***(0.93)
(3) Date/Delay Effect0.370.710.810.27***−0.15***(0.25)
(4) Unusual Experiences3.772.570.500.06*0.040.06(0.71)
(5) Cognitive Disorganization4.892.960.220.020.03−0.040.40***(0.77)
(6) Introvertive Anhedonia2.071.801.020.020.04−0.050.09**0.32***(0.56)
(7) Impulsive Nonconformity3.141.940.570.040.06−0.040.40***0.39***0.10**(0.52)
(8) Alcohol (AUDIT)5.144.871.41<0.010.03−0.050.020.11***−0.07*0.25***(0.77)
(9) Tobacco (ASSIST)2.844.861.810.11***0.10**0.040.22***0.16***0.020.30***0.23***(0.58)

Note. Values in the correlation diagonal represent Cronbach’s alphas, except for the date/delay effect (computed as ln(k) Delay minus ln(k) Date), for which the reliability was estimated using formula 4 from Trafimow.95 Correlations: df = 998. N = 1000. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Sk = skewness; ln(k) = log-transformed discount rate based on the Monetary Choice Questionnaire.85

*P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001 (unadjusted).

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics, Cronbach’s Alphas, and Pearson Correlations

VariableMSDSk(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
(1) ln(k) Delay−5.731.70−0.05(0.93)
(2) ln(k) Date−6.101.650.190.91***(0.93)
(3) Date/Delay Effect0.370.710.810.27***−0.15***(0.25)
(4) Unusual Experiences3.772.570.500.06*0.040.06(0.71)
(5) Cognitive Disorganization4.892.960.220.020.03−0.040.40***(0.77)
(6) Introvertive Anhedonia2.071.801.020.020.04−0.050.09**0.32***(0.56)
(7) Impulsive Nonconformity3.141.940.570.040.06−0.040.40***0.39***0.10**(0.52)
(8) Alcohol (AUDIT)5.144.871.41<0.010.03−0.050.020.11***−0.07*0.25***(0.77)
(9) Tobacco (ASSIST)2.844.861.810.11***0.10**0.040.22***0.16***0.020.30***0.23***(0.58)
VariableMSDSk(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)
(1) ln(k) Delay−5.731.70−0.05(0.93)
(2) ln(k) Date−6.101.650.190.91***(0.93)
(3) Date/Delay Effect0.370.710.810.27***−0.15***(0.25)
(4) Unusual Experiences3.772.570.500.06*0.040.06(0.71)
(5) Cognitive Disorganization4.892.960.220.020.03−0.040.40***(0.77)
(6) Introvertive Anhedonia2.071.801.020.020.04−0.050.09**0.32***(0.56)
(7) Impulsive Nonconformity3.141.940.570.040.06−0.040.40***0.39***0.10**(0.52)
(8) Alcohol (AUDIT)5.144.871.41<0.010.03−0.050.020.11***−0.07*0.25***(0.77)
(9) Tobacco (ASSIST)2.844.861.810.11***0.10**0.040.22***0.16***0.020.30***0.23***(0.58)

Note. Values in the correlation diagonal represent Cronbach’s alphas, except for the date/delay effect (computed as ln(k) Delay minus ln(k) Date), for which the reliability was estimated using formula 4 from Trafimow.95 Correlations: df = 998. N = 1000. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Sk = skewness; ln(k) = log-transformed discount rate based on the Monetary Choice Questionnaire.85

*P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001 (unadjusted).

Comparisons of discount rates in the two versions showed that participants discounted future rewards significantly more steeply in the delay than in the date condition, t(999) = 16.25, P < .001, gav = 0.22, replicating the date/delay effect (Supplementary Material B7). An exploratory, nonpreregistered within-subject ANOVA examining the interaction between the date/delay effect and the magnitude effect (Supplementary Material B8) revealed main effects of time framing, F(1,999) = 263.89, P < .001, ηp²=0.21, and magnitude, F(1.81,1811.58) = 1030.00, P < .001, ηp²=0.51, but no interaction, F(1.99,1985.02) = 0.77, P = .46, ηp² < 0.01. Furthermore, a nonpreregistered mixed ANOVA examining the effect of MCQ presentation order (Supplementary Material B9) revealed that, while the date/delay effect was still highly significant, F(1,998) = 264.89, P < .001, ηp² = 0.21, TD was generally lower in participants who first completed the date and then the delay version (than vice versa), F(1,998) = 12.91, P < .001, ηp² = 0.01. Additionally, a significant version × order interaction indicated that the date/delay effect was higher in participants who first completed the date version, F(1,998) = 33.61, P < .001, ηp² = 0.03.

Next, we explored whether the date/delay effect was related to schizotypy. Preregistered Williams tests did not yield significant differences between the correlations across the date and delay conditions for any of the subscales (UE: t(997) = 1.70, P = .09; CD: t(997) = −1.24, P = .22; IA: t(997) = −1.60, P = .11; IN: t(997) = −1.51, P = .13). Similarly, in a nonpreregistered extreme group approach comparing the date/delay effect between schizotypy high- and low-scorers, we did not find any significant differences (Supplementary Material B10). However, a nonpreregistered multiple linear regression in which all four subscale scores were used to predict the difference between ln(k) values of the two versions revealed that UE was significantly associated with the magnitude of the date/delay effect while the other subscales were not (Supplementary Material B11). Specifically, higher UE scores were associated with a greater date/delay effect. This effect persisted when controlling for age, education, alcohol, and tobacco consumption in hierarchical linear regression (table 2).

Table 2.

Hierarchical Linear Regression Predicting the Magnitude of the Date/Delay Effect

VariableB95% CI for BSE B
β
R²
ΔR²
LLUL
Step 10.0100.010
 Constant0.270***0.1350.4040.069
 Age0.004*0.0000.0080.002.07
 Education (low)−0.067−0.2340.1000.085−.03
 Education (high)−0.002−0.1030.0990.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.009−0.0180.0010.005−.06
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0010.0180.005.06
Step 20.0190.010*
 Constant0.3230.1450.5010.091
 Age0.004−0.0010.0080.002.06
 Education (low)−0.052−0.2200.1160.086−.02
 Education (high)0.007−0.0950.1090.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.007−0.0170.0030.005-.05
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0020.0180.005.06
 Unusual Experiences0.024*0.0040.0440.010.09
 Cognitive Disorganization−0.007−0.0250.0110.009−.03
 Introvertive Anhedonia−0.019−0.0450.0070.013−.05
 Impulsive Nonconformity−0.021−0.0480.0070.014−.06
VariableB95% CI for BSE B
β
R²
ΔR²
LLUL
Step 10.0100.010
 Constant0.270***0.1350.4040.069
 Age0.004*0.0000.0080.002.07
 Education (low)−0.067−0.2340.1000.085−.03
 Education (high)−0.002−0.1030.0990.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.009−0.0180.0010.005−.06
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0010.0180.005.06
Step 20.0190.010*
 Constant0.3230.1450.5010.091
 Age0.004−0.0010.0080.002.06
 Education (low)−0.052−0.2200.1160.086−.02
 Education (high)0.007−0.0950.1090.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.007−0.0170.0030.005-.05
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0020.0180.005.06
 Unusual Experiences0.024*0.0040.0440.010.09
 Cognitive Disorganization−0.007−0.0250.0110.009−.03
 Introvertive Anhedonia−0.019−0.0450.0070.013−.05
 Impulsive Nonconformity−0.021−0.0480.0070.014−.06

Note. Education was entered as two dummy variables (reference category: medium). CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. N = 990.

*P < .05, ***P < .001

Table 2.

Hierarchical Linear Regression Predicting the Magnitude of the Date/Delay Effect

VariableB95% CI for BSE B
β
R²
ΔR²
LLUL
Step 10.0100.010
 Constant0.270***0.1350.4040.069
 Age0.004*0.0000.0080.002.07
 Education (low)−0.067−0.2340.1000.085−.03
 Education (high)−0.002−0.1030.0990.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.009−0.0180.0010.005−.06
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0010.0180.005.06
Step 20.0190.010*
 Constant0.3230.1450.5010.091
 Age0.004−0.0010.0080.002.06
 Education (low)−0.052−0.2200.1160.086−.02
 Education (high)0.007−0.0950.1090.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.007−0.0170.0030.005-.05
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0020.0180.005.06
 Unusual Experiences0.024*0.0040.0440.010.09
 Cognitive Disorganization−0.007−0.0250.0110.009−.03
 Introvertive Anhedonia−0.019−0.0450.0070.013−.05
 Impulsive Nonconformity−0.021−0.0480.0070.014−.06
VariableB95% CI for BSE B
β
R²
ΔR²
LLUL
Step 10.0100.010
 Constant0.270***0.1350.4040.069
 Age0.004*0.0000.0080.002.07
 Education (low)−0.067−0.2340.1000.085−.03
 Education (high)−0.002−0.1030.0990.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.009−0.0180.0010.005−.06
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0010.0180.005.06
Step 20.0190.010*
 Constant0.3230.1450.5010.091
 Age0.004−0.0010.0080.002.06
 Education (low)−0.052−0.2200.1160.086−.02
 Education (high)0.007−0.0950.1090.052.00
 Alcohol (AUDIT)−0.007−0.0170.0030.005-.05
 Tobacco (ASSIST)0.008−0.0020.0180.005.06
 Unusual Experiences0.024*0.0040.0440.010.09
 Cognitive Disorganization−0.007−0.0250.0110.009−.03
 Introvertive Anhedonia−0.019−0.0450.0070.013−.05
 Impulsive Nonconformity−0.021−0.0480.0070.014−.06

Note. Education was entered as two dummy variables (reference category: medium). CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. N = 990.

*P < .05, ***P < .001

Discussion

The current study is the first to use the sO-LIFE to examine associations between TD and four dimensions of schizotypy. Furthermore, it is the first to replicate the date/delay effect in a large-scale study and to examine its relation to schizotypy. Results are as follows: First, none of the schizotypy traits were significantly related to TD, neither in the delay nor in the date condition. While a significant positive correlation between UE and discount rate occurred in the standard delay version, it did not persist after correcting for multiple comparisons. Second, as expected, the date/delay effect was replicated. The effect was independent of the magnitude effect. Third, when controlling for influences of the other sO-LIFE subscales (and age, education as well as alcohol and tobacco consumption), the UE subscale predicted the magnitude of the date/delay effect. That is, higher values in UE were associated with a higher reduction of TD in the date compared to the delay condition.

The null findings regarding relations between schizotypy and TD are consistent with the only other study examining these relations across the entire schizotypy continuum. Indeed, Weatherly40 found only few subscales of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ)2 to be related to TD in undergraduates, with low effect sizes. Our study extends this finding to a larger sample using the sO-LIFE – a measure that is based on the fully dimensional approach rather than on clinically derived symptom descriptions.1 So far, studies that have found considerable effect sizes of relations between schizotypal traits and TD are usually based on extreme group designs, often with a particular focus on negative schizotypy, be it in studies comparing schizotypy high- and low-scorers34,35 or in those comparing schizophrenia patients and healthy controls.27,29 In our study, however, we did not find TD differences in analyses comparing schizotypy high- and low-scorers. Given the inconsistent findings outlined above, our results could therefore either indicate that deviations in TD are nonexistent (or very low in effect size) across the entire schizophrenia spectrum or that a certain limit of symptomatology has to be exceeded to show significant aberrations, which would better accord with quasi-dimensional than fully dimensional approaches of schizotypy.1 The latter could additionally suggest that deviations in TD in schizophrenia are due to disease processes or medication effects, as previously suggested,33,34 rather than to vulnerability in highly schizotypal individuals. Alternatively (or additionally), combinations of specific trait profiles, for example interactions between higher positive and negative schizotypy, could be necessary to reveal TD differences as both reward and time processing could be altered in such individuals. Given the overall inconsistent findings, however, more work is needed to clarify the magnitude of effect sizes of TD differences across the schizophrenia spectrum.

A more refined approach to this question is to examine the cognitive mechanisms suspected to lead to increased TD and to explore whether these are altered in schizophrenia spectrum individuals. Here, we focused on future time perception by using a date/delay manipulation. Importantly, we replicated the date/delay effect and found it to be robust against the well-known magnitude effect in TD (ie, larger rewards being discounted less steeply than smaller ones96,97) and against order of MCQ presentation (although it was greater when the date version was presented first). The date/delay effect has been found previously,76,77,80,98–101 with a meta-analysis revealing a slightly larger effect size than our study.78 However, this difference could be partly due to our effect size calculation in which we corrected for the correlation of measurements (ie, our uncorrected effect size is dz = 0.51). Importantly, our study was one of the few using a within-subject design99,100 and had by far the largest sample to date.

Importantly, the association of the date/delay effect with schizotypy had not previously been investigated. Here, we observed that higher positive schizotypy (UE) scores were associated with a stronger date/delay effect after controlling for the other subscales. This intriguing finding may indicate that not only reward but also time processing and/or their integration are aberrant in high schizotypy. Interestingly, previous studies have shown that negative symptoms and anhedonia are mainly responsible for deviations in reward valuation50–52 and some studies found high levels in positive symptoms to be associated with aberrant time perception and internal clock mechanisms.61,74 Thus, one may speculate that the distinction between positive and negative symptoms is important for differential influences on time and reward processing, respectively. Furthermore, the finding suggests that simple manipulations such as the date/delay framing could help particularly vulnerable populations to reduce impulsive decision making. This idea is in line with the finding that in schizotypy high-scorers, episodic future thinking (EFT),102 ie, presenting future rewards in combination with positive future (rather than recent) events, helped to reduce TD.103 While that study lacked a control group (thus not revealing whether the manipulation might be particularly effective within schizotypy high-scorers), it is the only other study showing that manipulations of future time perspective can help to reduce TD in a sample with high schizotypy levels. It is of considerable interest to further investigate whether the date/delay and EFT manipulations have particularly beneficial impacts on people high in schizotypal traits and schizophrenia patients, especially considering that schizophrenia patients have been shown to have altered EFT.104 This, of course, would not only provide insights into time processing mechanisms in schizotypy and schizophrenia but also a potential way to reduce impulsive decision making.103

There are some limitations to consider. First, the online setting prevented us from collecting comprehensive clinical data. Thus, diagnostics of mental disorders was not possible, although this may be desirable given the influences of different disorders on TD.25 Similarly, our attempts to control for risky substance consumption must be interpreted with care because they were not validated independently. The possibility that our findings are driven by any one disorder is unlikely, however, given the large sample size. Second, we used a hypothetical intertemporal choice task because previous general population studies showed that they result in similar discount rates as incentive-compatible ones where participants are paid out one or more of their decisions.105,106 However, a study comparing schizophrenia patients and healthy controls only found a difference in TD when using an experiential rather than a hypothetical task, thus raising the question whether the association depends on realization of rewards.31 Whilst other studies, in part, found significant effects with hypothetical tasks,26,27,29,30,32 this is important to consider in future studies. Third, our analysis revealing the association between positive schizotypy and the date/delay effect was largely explorative, included a difference score with potentially limited reliability95 (however, see Thomas and Zumbo107), and yielded only a small effect size, thus warranting replication. Fourth, while we accounted for educational status in our analyses, future studies might also consider assessing further control variables, such as working-memory capacity, IQ or socio-economic status as these are known to be associated with TD.22 Fifth, a more representative sample regarding distributions of education and gender would be desirable, especially since the latter is associated with differences in O-LIFE subscale scores.3

Overall, our large-scale study revealed that schizotypal personality traits are not (or only very weakly) associated with TD. However, positive schizotypy was associated with the magnitude of the date/delay effect, suggesting deviations in future time perception in people with high levels of positive schizotypy and potential reductions of those through the simple date manipulation. Future studies are needed to determine whether this effect is robust in nonclinical and clinical samples to draw conclusions on cognitive mechanisms of altered decision making in schizophrenia.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material is available at https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/schizophreniabulletin/. Study materials, data and analysis scripts as well as supplementary material can be accessed at https://osf.io/xdn7c/.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Eliana Faiola, Jonas Donie, Liza Mekschrat and Tsvetelina Tsvetkova for their invaluable help in study preparation and data collection.

Funding

We gratefully acknowledge intramural funding of this work from the University of Bonn. C Pantelis was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) L3 Investigator Grant (1196508).

References

1.

Grant
 
P
,
Green
 
MJ
,
Mason
 
OJ.
 
Models of schizotypy: the importance of conceptual clarity
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2018
;
44
(
suppl_2
):
S556
S563
. doi: https://doi.org/.

2.

Raine
 
A.
 
The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
1991
;
17
(
4
):
555
564
. doi: https://doi.org/.

3.

Mason
 
O
,
Claridge
 
G
,
Jackson
 
M.
 
New scales for the assessment of schizotypy
.
Pers Individ Dif
.
1995
;
18
(
1
):
7
13
. doi: https://doi.org/.

4.

Claridge
 
G.
 
Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and Health
.
Claridge
 
G
, ed.
Oxford, UK
:
Oxford University Press
;
1997
. doi: https://doi.org/.

5.

Barrantes-Vidal
 
N
,
Grant
 
P
,
Kwapil
 
TR.
 
The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2015
;
41
(
suppl_2
):
S408
S416
. doi: https://doi.org/.

6.

Debbané
 
M
,
Eliez
 
S
,
Badoud
 
D
,
Conus
 
P
,
Flückiger
 
R
,
Schultze-Lutter
 
F.
 
Developing psychosis and its risk states through the lens of schizotypy
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2015
;
41
(
suppl_2
):
S396
S407
. doi: https://doi.org/.

7.

Ettinger
 
U
,
Meyhöfer
 
I
,
Steffens
 
M
,
Wagner
 
M
,
Koutsouleris
 
N.
 
Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: A review of the overlap with schizophrenia
.
Front Psychiatry.
 
2014
;
5
:
18
. doi: https://doi.org/.

8.

Kwapil
 
TR
,
Gross
 
GM
,
Silvia
 
PJ
,
Barrantes-Vidal
 
N.
 
Prediction of psychopathology and functional impairment by positive and negative schizotypy in the chapmans’ ten-year longitudinal study
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2013
;
122
(
3
):
807
815
. doi: https://doi.org/.

9.

Nelson
 
MT
,
Seal
 
ML
,
Pantelis
 
C
,
Phillips
 
LJ.
 
Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
 
2013
;
37
(
3
):
317
327
. doi: https://doi.org/.

10.

Van Os
 
J
,
Linscott
 
RJ
,
Myin-Germeys
 
I
,
Delespaul
 
P
,
Krabbendam
 
L.
 
A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness–persistence–impairment model of psychotic disorder
.
Psychol Med.
 
2009
;
39
(
2
):
179
195
. doi: https://doi.org/.

11.

Ettinger
 
U
,
Mohr
 
C
,
Gooding
 
DC
, et al.  
Cognition and brain function in schizotypy: a selective review
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2015
;
41
(
suppl_2
):
S417
S426
. doi: https://doi.org/.

12.

Giakoumaki
 
SG.
 
Cognitive and prepulse inhibition deficits in psychometrically high schizotypal subjects in the general population: relevance to schizophrenia research
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc.
 
2012
;
18
(
4
):
643
656
. doi: https://doi.org/.

13.

de Leon
 
J
,
Diaz
 
FJ.
 
A meta-analysis of worldwide studies demonstrates an association between schizophrenia and tobacco smoking behaviors
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2005
;
76
(
2–3
):
135
157
. doi: https://doi.org/.

14.

Esterberg
 
ML
,
Goulding
 
SM
,
McClure-Tone
 
EB
,
Compton
 
MT.
 
Schizotypy and nicotine, alcohol, and cannabis use in a non-psychiatric sample
.
Addict Behav.
 
2009
;
34
(
4
):
374
379
. doi: https://doi.org/.

15.

Lohr
 
JB
,
Flynn
 
K.
 
Smoking and schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res.
 
1992
;
8
(
2
):
93
102
. doi: https://doi.org/.

16.

Stewart
 
DW
,
Cohen
 
AS
,
Copeland
 
AL.
 
Cigarette smoking across the schizotypy spectrum
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2010
;
179
(
1
):
113
115
. doi: https://doi.org/.

17.

Koskinen
 
J
,
Löhönen
 
J
,
Koponen
 
H
,
Isohanni
 
M
,
Miettunen
 
J.
 
Rate of cannabis use disorders in clinical samples of patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2010
;
36
(
6
):
1115
1130
. doi: https://doi.org/.

18.

Skinner
 
R
,
Conlon
 
L
,
Gibbons
 
D
,
McDonald
 
C.
 
Cannabis use and non-clinical dimensions of psychosis in university students presenting to primary care
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand.
 
2011
;
123
(
1
):
21
27
. doi: https://doi.org/.

19.

Firth
 
J
,
Stubbs
 
B
,
Vancampfort
 
D
, et al.  
The validity and value of self-reported physical activity and accelerometry in people with schizophrenia: A population-scale study of the UK biobank
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2018
;
44
(
6
):
1293
1300
. doi: https://doi.org/.

20.

Dinzeo
 
TJ
,
Thayasivam
 
U.
 
Schizotypy, lifestyle behaviors, and health indicators in a young adult sample
.
Behavioral Sciences
.
2021
;
11
(
12
):
179
. doi: https://doi.org/.

21.

Frederick
 
S
,
Loewenstein
 
G
,
O’Donoghue
 
T.
 
Time discounting and time preference: A critical review
.
J Econ Lit.
 
2002
;
40
(
2
):
351
401
. doi: https://doi.org/.

22.

Keidel
 
K
,
Rramani
 
Q
,
Weber
 
B
,
Murawski
 
C
,
Ettinger
 
U.
 
Individual differences in intertemporal choice
.
Front Psychol.
 
2021
;
12
:
643670
. doi: https://doi.org/.

23.

Keidel
 
K
,
Ettinger
 
U
,
Murawski
 
C
,
Polner
 
B.
 
The network structure of impulsive personality and temporal discounting
.
J Res Pers
.
2022
;
96
:
104166
. doi: https://doi.org/.

24.

MacKillop
 
J
,
Weafer
 
J
,
Gray
 
JC
,
Oshri
 
A
,
Palmer
 
A
,
de Wit
 
H.
 
The latent structure of impulsivity: Impulsive choice, impulsive action, and impulsive personality traits
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl).
 
2016
;
233
(
18
):
3361
3370
. doi: https://doi.org/.

25.

Amlung
 
M
,
Marsden
 
E
,
Holshausen
 
K
, et al.  
Delay discounting as a transdiagnostic process in psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis
.
JAMA Psychiatry
.
2019
;
76
(
11
):
1176
1186
. doi: https://doi.org/.

26.

Ahn
 
WY
,
Rass
 
O
,
Fridberg
 
DJ
, et al.  
Temporal discounting of rewards in patients with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2011
;
120
(
4
):
911
921
. doi: https://doi.org/.

27.

Bartolomeo
 
LA
,
Chapman
 
HC
,
Raugh
 
IM
,
Strauss
 
GP.
 
Delay discounting in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis and adults with schizophrenia
.
Psychol Med.
 
2021
;
51
(
11
):
1898
1905
. doi: https://doi.org/.

28.

Brown
 
HE
,
Hart
 
KL
,
Snapper
 
LA
,
Roffman
 
JL
,
Perlis
 
RH.
 
Impairment in delay discounting in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder but not primary mood disorders
.
npj Schizophr.
 
2018
;
4
:
9
. doi: https://doi.org/.

29.

Heerey
 
EA
,
Robinson
 
BM
,
McMahon
 
RP
,
Gold
 
JM.
 
Delay discounting in schizophrenia
.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry
.
2007
;
12
(
3
):
213
221
. doi: https://doi.org/.

30.

Heerey
 
EA
,
Matveeva
 
TM
,
Gold
 
JM.
 
Imagining the future: degraded representations of future rewards and events in schizophrenia
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2011
;
120
(
2
):
483
489
. doi: https://doi.org/.

31.

Horan
 
WP
,
Johnson
 
MW
,
Green
 
MF.
 
Altered experiential, but not hypothetical, delay discounting in schizophrenia
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2017
;
126
(
3
):
301
311
. doi: https://doi.org/.

32.

Weller
 
RE
,
Avsar
 
KB
,
Cox
 
JE
,
Reid
 
MA
,
White
 
DM
,
Lahti
 
AC.
 
Delay discounting and task performance consistency in patients with schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2014
;
215
(
2
):
286
293
. doi: https://doi.org/.

33.

Yu
 
LQ
,
Lee
 
S
,
Katchmar
 
N
,
Satterthwaite
 
TD
,
Kable
 
JW
,
Wolf
 
DH.
 
Steeper discounting of delayed rewards in schizophrenia but not first-degree relatives
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2017
;
252
:
303
309
. doi: https://doi.org/.

34.

Cai X lu
 
XI
,
Weigl
 
M
,
Liu
 
B
,
Cheung
 
EFC
,
Ding
 
J
,
Chan
 
RCK.
 
Delay discounting and affective priming in individuals with negative schizotypy
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2019
;
210
:
180
187
. doi: https://doi.org/.

35.

Li
 
Z
,
Shi
 
YF
,
Parker
 
GJ
, et al.  
Devaluation of rewards for the future is associated with schizotypal personality features
.
Aust Psychol
.
2016
;
51
(
6
):
481
489
. doi: https://doi.org/.

36.

Terenzi
 
D
,
Mainetto
 
E
,
Barbato
 
M
,
Rumiati
 
RI
,
Aiello
 
M.
 
Temporal and effort cost decision-making in healthy individuals with subclinical psychotic symptoms
.
Sci Rep.
 
2019
;
9
:
2151
. doi: https://doi.org/.

37.

MacKillop
 
J
,
Tidey
 
JW.
 
Cigarette demand and delayed reward discounting in nicotine-dependent individuals with schizophrenia and controls: an initial study
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl).
 
2011
;
216
(
1
):
91
99
. doi: https://doi.org/.

38.

Wang
 
L
,
Jin
 
S
,
He
 
K
, et al.  
Increased delayed reward during intertemporal decision-making in schizophrenic patients and their unaffected siblings
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2018
;
262
:
246
253
. doi: https://doi.org/.

39.

Wing
 
VC
,
Moss
 
TG
,
Rabin
 
RA
,
George
 
TP.
 
Effects of cigarette smoking status on delay discounting in schizophrenia and healthy control
.
Addict Behav.
 
2012
;
37
(
1
):
67
72
. doi: https://doi.org/.

40.

Weatherly
 
JN.
 
The association between delay discounting and schizotypal personality characteristics in a nonclinical sample
.
Psychol Rec.
 
2012
;
62
(
3
):
529
540
. doi: https://doi.org/.

41.

Bickel
 
WK
,
Odum
 
AL
,
Madden
 
GJ.
 
Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl).
 
1999
;
146
(
4
):
447
454
. doi: https://doi.org/.

42.

Kable
 
JW
,
Glimcher
 
PW.
 
The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice
.
Nat Neurosci.
 
2007
;
10
(
12
):
1625
1633
. doi: https://doi.org/.

43.

Rodriguez
 
CA
,
Turner
 
BM
,
McClure
 
SM.
 
Intertemporal choice as discounted value accumulation
.
PLoS One.
 
2014
;
9
(
2
):
e90138
. doi: https://doi.org/.

44.

Amasino
 
DR
,
Sullivan
 
NJ
,
Kranton
 
RE
,
Huettel
 
SA.
 
Amount and time exert independent influences on intertemporal choice
.
Nat Hum Behav.
 
2019
;
3
(
4
):
383
392
. doi: https://doi.org/.

45.

Bradford
 
WD
,
Dolan
 
P
,
Galizzi
 
MM.
 
Looking ahead: Subjective time perception and individual discounting
.
J Risk Uncertain
.
2019
;
58
(
1
):
43
69
. doi: https://doi.org/.

46.

Kim
 
BK
,
Zauberman
 
G.
 
Perception of anticipatory time in temporal discounting
.
J Neurosci Psychol Econ
.
2009
;
2
(
2
):
91
101
. doi: https://doi.org/.

47.

Takahashi
 
T
,
Oono
 
H
,
Radford
 
MHB.
 
Psychophysics of time perception and intertemporal choice models
.
Physica A.
 
2008
;
387
(
8–9
):
2066
2074
. doi: https://doi.org/.

48.

Ainslie
 
G.
 
Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control
.
Psychol Bull.
 
1975
;
82
(
4
):
463
496
. doi: https://doi.org/.

49.

Hunt
 
LT
,
Hayden
 
BY.
 
A distributed, hierarchical and recurrent framework for reward-based choice
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
 
2017
;
18
(
3
):
172
182
. doi: https://doi.org/.

50.

Gold
 
JM
,
Waltz
 
JA
,
Prentice
 
KJ
,
Morris
 
SE
,
Heerey
 
EA.
 
Reward processing in schizophrenia: A deficit in the representation of value
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2008
;
34
(
5
):
835
847
. doi: https://doi.org/.

51.

Kring
 
AM
,
Barch
 
DM.
 
The motivation and pleasure dimension of negative symptoms: Neural substrates and behavioral outputs
.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol.
 
2014
;
24
(
5
):
725
736
. doi: https://doi.org/.

52.

Strauss
 
GP
,
Waltz
 
JA
,
Gold
 
JM.
 
A review of reward processing and motivational impairment in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2014
;
40
(
suppl_2
):
S107
S116
. doi: https://doi.org/.

53.

Barch
 
DM
,
Dowd
 
EC.
 
Goal representations and motivational drive in schizophrenia: The role of prefrontal-striatal interactions
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2010
;
36
(
5
):
919
934
. doi: https://doi.org/.

54.

Visser
 
KF
,
Chapman
 
HC
,
Ruiz
 
I
,
Raugh
 
IM
,
Strauss
 
GP.
 
A meta-analysis of self-reported anticipatory and consummatory pleasure in the schizophrenia-spectrum
.
J Psychiatr Res.
 
2020
;
121
:
68
81
. doi: https://doi.org/.

55.

Wang
 
J
,
Huang
 
J
,
Yang
 
X
,
Lui
 
SSY
,
Cheung
 
EFC
,
Chan
 
RCK.
 
Anhedonia in schizophrenia: deficits in both motivation and hedonic capacity
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2015
;
168
(
1–2
):
465
474
. doi: https://doi.org/.

56.

Brown
 
JK
,
Waltz
 
JA
,
Strauss
 
GP
,
McMahon
 
RP
,
Frank
 
MJ
,
Gold
 
JM.
 
Hypothetical decision making in schizophrenia: the role of expected value computation and “irrational” biases
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2013
;
209
(
2
):
142
149
. doi: https://doi.org/.

57.

Martinelli
 
C
,
Rigoli
 
F
,
Dolan
 
RJ
,
Shergill
 
SS.
 
Decreased value-sensitivity in schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2018
;
259
:
295
301
. doi: https://doi.org/.

58.

Treadway
 
MT
,
Peterman
 
JS
,
Zald
 
DH
,
Park
 
S.
 
Impaired effort allocation in patients with schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2015
;
161
(
2–3
):
382
385
. doi: https://doi.org/.

59.

Ciullo
 
V
,
Spalletta
 
G
,
Caltagirone
 
C
,
Jorge
 
RE
,
Piras
 
F.
 
Explicit time deficit in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis indicate it is primary and not domain specific
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2016
;
42
(
2
):
505
518
. doi: https://doi.org/.

60.

Thoenes
 
S
,
Oberfeld
 
D.
 
Meta-analysis of time perception and temporal processing in schizophrenia: differential effects on precision and accuracy
.
Clin Psychol Rev.
 
2017
;
54
:
44
64
. doi: https://doi.org/.

61.

Ueda
 
N
,
Maruo
 
K
,
Sumiyoshi
 
T.
 
Positive symptoms and time perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis
.
Schizophr Res Cogn
.
2018
;
13
:
3
6
. doi: https://doi.org/.

62.

Treisman
 
M.
 
Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: implications for a model of the “internal clock.”
Psychol Monogr
.
1963
;
77
(
13
):
1
31
. doi: https://doi.org/.

63.

Roy
 
M
,
Grondin
 
S
,
Roy
 
MA.
 
Time perception disorders are related to working memory impairment in schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2012
;
200
(
2–3
):
159
166
. doi: https://doi.org/.

64.

Heerey
 
EA
,
Bell-Warren
 
KR
,
Gold
 
JM.
 
Decision-making impairments in the context of intact reward sensitivity in schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry.
 
2008
;
64
(
1
):
62
69
. doi: https://doi.org/.

65.

Wallace
 
M.
 
Future time perspective in schizophrenia
.
J Abnormal Soc Psychol
.
1956
;
52
(
2
):
240
245
. doi: https://doi.org/.

66.

Zauberman
 
G
,
Kim
 
BK.
 
Another dimension of psychological time: judgments of anticipated future duration
.
Euresis J
.
2014
;
7
:
61
68
. https://euresis.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/06/EJv7id5_Zauberman-KyuKim.pdf .

67.

Zauberman
 
G
,
Kim
 
BK
,
Malkoc
 
SA
,
Bettman
 
JR.
 
Discounting time and time discounting: subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences
.
J Mark Res.
 
2009
;
46
(
4
):
543
556
. doi: https://doi.org/.

68.

Wittmann
 
M
,
Paulus
 
MP.
 
Decision making, impulsivity and time perception
.
Trends Cogn Sci.
 
2008
;
12
(
1
):
7
12
. doi: https://doi.org/.

69.

Sharma
 
SN
,
Khan
 
A.
 
Interval timing predicts impulsivity in intertemporal choice: combined behavioral and drift-diffusion model evidence
.
J Cogn Psychol.
 
2018
;
30
(
8
):
816
831
. doi: https://doi.org/.

70.

Salzinger
 
K
,
Serper
 
M
Schizophrenia: the immediacy mechanism
.
Int J Psychol Psychol Therapy.
 
2004
;
4
(
2
):
397
409
.

71.

Salzinger
 
K
,
Portnoy
 
S
,
Pisoni
 
DB
,
Feldman
 
RS.
 
The immediacy hypothesis and response-produced stimuli in schizophrenic speech
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
1970
;
76
(
2
):
258
264
. doi: https://doi.org/.

72.

Alfimova
 
MV
,
Lezheiko
 
T
,
Plakunova
 
V
,
Golimbet
 
V.
 
Relationships between schizotypal features, trait anticipatory and consummatory pleasure, and naturalistic hedonic states
.
Motiv Emot
.
2021
;
45
(
5
):
649
660
. doi: https://doi.org/.

73.

Lee
 
KH
,
Dixon
 
JK
,
Spence
 
SA
,
Woodruff
 
PWR.
 
Time perception dysfunction in psychometric schizotypy
.
Pers Individ Dif
.
2006
;
40
(
7
):
1363
1373
. doi: https://doi.org/.

74.

Reed
 
P
,
Randell
 
J.
 
Altered time-perception performance in individuals with high schizotypy levels
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2014
;
220
(
1–2
):
211
216
. doi: https://doi.org/.

75.

Osborne
 
KJ
,
Damme
 
KSF
,
Gupta
 
T
,
Dean
 
DJ
,
Bernard
 
JA
,
Mittal
 
VA.
 
Timing dysfunction and cerebellar resting state functional connectivity abnormalities in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis
.
Psychol Med.
 
2021
;
51
(
8
):
1289
1298
. doi: https://doi.org/.

76.

LeBoeuf
 
RA.
 
Discount rates for time versus dates: the sensitivity of discounting to time-interval description
.
J Marketing Res.
 
2006
;
43
(
1
):
59
72
. doi: https://doi.org/.

77.

Read
 
D
,
Frederick
 
S
,
Orsel
 
B
,
Rahman
 
J.
 
Four score and seven years from now: the date/delay effect in temporal discounting
.
Manage Sci.
 
2005
;
51
(
9
):
1326
1335
. doi: https://doi.org/.

78.

Rung
 
JM
,
Madden
 
GJ.
 
Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Exp Psychol Gen.
 
2018
;
147
(
9
):
1349
1381
. doi: https://doi.org/.

79.

Scholten
 
H
,
Scheres
 
A
,
de Water
 
E
,
Graf
 
U
,
Granic
 
I
,
Luijten
 
M.
 
Behavioral trainings and manipulations to reduce delay discounting: a systematic review
.
Psychon Bull Rev.
 
2019
;
26
(
6
):
1803
1849
. doi: https://doi.org/.

80.

Jiang
 
J
,
Dai
 
J.
 
Time and risk perceptions mediate the causal impact of objective delay on delay discounting: an experimental examination of the implicit-risk hypothesis
.
Psychon Bull Rev.
 
2021
;
28
:
1399
1412
. doi: https://doi.org/.

81.

Mason
 
O
,
Linney
 
Y
,
Claridge
 
G.
 
Short scales for measuring schizotypy
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2005
;
78
(
2–3
):
293
296
. doi: https://doi.org/.

82.

Faul
 
F
,
Erdfelder
 
E
,
Buchner
 
A
,
Lang
 
AG.
 
Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses
.
Behav Res Methods.
 
2009
;
41
(
4
):
1149
1160
. doi: https://doi.org/.

83.

Jackson
 
DN.
 
Manual for the Personality Research Form
.
Goshen, NY
:
Research Psychological Press
;
1967
.

84.

Gray
 
JC
,
Amlung
 
MT
,
Palmer
 
AA
,
MacKillop
 
J.
 
Syntax for calculation of discounting indices from the monetary choice questionnaire and probability discounting questionnaire
.
J Exp Anal Behav.
 
2016
;
106
(
2
):
156
163
. doi: https://doi.org/.

85.

Kirby
 
KN
,
Petry
 
NM
,
Bickel
 
WK.
 
Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls
.
J Exp Psychol Gen.
 
1999
;
128
(
1
):
78
87
.

86.

Kirby
 
KN.
 
One-year temporal stability of delay-discount rates
.
Psychon Bull Rev.
 
2009
;
16
(
3
):
457
462
. doi: https://doi.org/.

87.

Mason
 
OJ.
 
The assessment of schizotypy and its clinical relevance
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2015
;
41
(
suppl_2
):
S374
S385
. doi: https://doi.org/.

88.

Grant
 
P
,
Kuepper
 
Y
,
Mueller
 
EA
,
Wielpuetz
 
C
,
Mason
 
O
,
Hennig
 
J.
 
Dopaminergic foundations of schizotypy as measured by the German version of the Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE)—A suitable endophenotype of schizophrenia
.
Front Hum Neurosci.
 
2013
;
7
:
1
. doi: https://doi.org/.

89.

Humeniuk
 
RE
,
Henry-Edwards
 
S
,
Ali
 
R
,
Poznyak
 
V
,
Monteiro
 
M.
 
The Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST): Manual for Use in Primary Care
.
Geneva
:
World Health Organization
;
2010
.

90.

Ali
 
R
,
Awwad
 
E
,
Babor
 
TF
, et al.  
The Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST): development, reliability and feasibility
.
Addiction.
 
2002
;
97
(
9
):
1183
1194
. doi: https://doi.org/.

91.

Babor
 
TF
,
Higgins-Biddle
 
JC
,
Saunders
 
JB
,
Monteiro
 
MG.
 
The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test
.
Geneva
:
World Health Organization
;
2001
. doi: https://doi.org/

92.

Suchtforschungsverbund
 
Baden-Württemberg
,
UKL
 
Freiburg
.
AUDIT-Fragebogen mit Auswertungsschema [AUDIT questionnaire with scoring scheme]
.
Accessed July 25,
 
2023
. https://www.bundesaerztekammer.de/fileadmin/user_upload/_old-files/downloads/AlkAuditAuswertung.pdf

93.

Lakens
 
D.
 
Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs
.
Front Psychol.
 
2013
;
4
:
863
. doi: https://doi.org/.

94.

Williams
 
EJ.
 
The comparison of regression variables
.
J R Stat Soc.: B (Methodol.).
 
1959
;
21
(
2
):
396
399
. doi: https://doi.org/.

95.

Trafimow
 
D.
 
A defense against the alleged unreliability of difference scores
.
Cogent Mathematics
.
2015
;
2
(
1
):
1064626
. doi: https://doi.org/.

96.

Green
 
L
,
Myerson
 
J
,
McFadden
 
E.
 
Rate of temporal discounting decreases with amount of reward
.
Mem Cognit.
 
1997
;
25
(
5
):
715
723
. doi: https://doi.org/.

97.

Thaler
 
R.
 
Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency
.
Econ Lett
.
1981
;
8
(
3
):
201
207
. doi: https://doi.org/.

98.

DeHart
 
WB
,
Odum
 
AL.
 
The effects of the framing of time on delay discounting
.
J Exp Anal Behav.
 
2015
;
103
(
1
):
10
21
. doi: https://doi.org/.

99.

Dshemuchadse
 
M
,
Scherbaum
 
S
,
Goschke
 
T.
 
How decisions emerge: action dynamics in intertemporal decision making
.
J Exp Psychol Gen.
 
2013
;
142
(
1
):
93
100
. doi: https://doi.org/.

100.

Klapproth
 
F.
 
The date-delay framing effect in temporal discounting depends on substance abuse
.
Behav Process.
 
2012
;
90
(
3
):
420
423
. doi: https://doi.org/.

101.

Naudé
 
GP
,
Kaplan
 
BA
,
Reed
 
DD
,
Henley
 
AJ
,
DiGennaro Reed
 
FD.
 
Temporal framing and the hidden-zero effect: rate-dependent outcomes on delay discounting
.
J Exp Anal Behav.
 
2018
;
109
(
3
):
506
519
. doi: https://doi.org/.

102.

Atance
 
CM
,
O’Neill
 
DK.
 
Episodic future thinking
.
Trends Cogn Sci.
 
2001
;
5
(
12
):
533
539
. doi: https://doi.org/.

103.

Jia
 
LX
,
Liu
 
Z
,
Cui
 
JF
, et al.  
Future thinking is related to lower delay discounting than recent thinking, regardless of the magnitude of the reward, in individuals with schizotypy
.
Aust Psychol
.
2020
;
55
(
5
):
572
581
. doi: https://doi.org/.

104.

Hallford
 
DJ
,
Austin
 
DW
,
Takano
 
K
,
Raes
 
F.
 
Psychopathology and episodic future thinking: a systematic review and meta-analysis of specificity and episodic detail
.
Behav Res Ther.
 
2018
;
102
:
42
51
. doi: https://doi.org/.

105.

Johnson
 
MW
,
Bickel
 
WK.
 
Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting
.
J Exp Anal Behav.
 
2002
;
77
(
2
):
129
146
. doi: https://doi.org/.

106.

Matusiewicz
 
AK
,
Carter
 
AE
,
Landes
 
RD
,
Yi
 
R.
 
Statistical equivalence and test–retest reliability of delay and probability discounting using real and hypothetical rewards
.
Behav Process.
 
2013
;
100
:
116
122
. doi: https://doi.org/.

107.

Thomas
 
DR
,
Zumbo
 
BD.
 
Difference scores from the point of view of reliability and repeated-measures ANOVA: in defense of difference scores for data analysis
.
Educ Psychol Meas
.
2012
;
72
(
1
):
37
43
. doi: https://doi.org/.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected] for reprints and translation rights for reprints. All other permissions can be obtained through our RightsLink service via the Permissions link on the article page on our site—for further information please contact [email protected].