Abstract

Background and Hypothesis

Evidence suggests that emotion regulation is related to schizotypal traits and negative affect in adults. Few studies examined the interplay among these constructs in school-aged children. We examined the complex relationship between schizotypal traits, emotion regulation, and negative affect in children aged 9–12 years.

Study Design

One-thousand-and-nineteen children completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire—children (SPQ-C), the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21), and the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescence (ERQ-CA). Using subscales of these measures as nodes, we estimated a partial correlation network. We estimated a Directed Acyclic Graph to explore the putative directional relationship between schizotypal traits, emotion regulation, and negative affect. Node and bridge centrality indices were estimated.

Results

We found positive correlations between schizotypal dimensions and negative affect (depressed mood, anxiety, and stress) in the network. Emotion suppression was positively correlated with interpersonal and disorganized schizotypal dimensions, and negative affect. Emotion reappraisal was positively correlated with the cognitive–perceptual dimension and negatively correlated with interpersonal schizotypal traits, depressed mood, and stress. Stress showed higher strength than all nodes except depressed mood, and stress showed the highest expected influence (EI). The Bayesian network revealed that schizotypal traits appeared to be driven by stress. Network comparisons preliminarily showed higher EI for emotion reappraisal in girls’ than boys’ networks, and significant impacts of age and schizotypy levels on network patterns.

Conclusion

Children with higher levels of schizotypal traits may have more negative affect and suppression. Stress appears to drive schizotypal traits.

Introduction

Schizotypy refers to the latent psychological organization reflecting the liability to schizophrenia,1–3 and encompasses the cognitive–perceptual (positive schizotypy), interpersonal (negative schizotypy), and disorganized (disorganized schizotypy) dimensions. Schizotypal traits are continuous phenotypes, spanning from subclinical to clinical entities of schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD).4 Schizotypal traits can manifest in typical-developing children at the age of 5–6,5–7 and predict future risk for psychosis in adulthood.8 Studying childhood schizotypal traits can advance our understandings as to how SSD emerge.7,9

Negative affect (ie, depressed mood, anxiety, and stress) is prevalent in people with schizotypal traits,10,11 and aggravates the risk for psychosis transition.12–16 At the dimension level, while some findings showed that positive rather than negative schizotypy showed a stronger association with depressed mood and anxiety,17 others showed that disorganized schizotypy instead of positive schizotypy showed a strong association with NA (negative affect).18 Notably, the research found that the correlation between positive schizotypy and NA disappeared after controlling for disorganized schizotypy.19,20 These prior findings urged further research on schizotypy as separate dimensions. The directionality between schizotypal traits and NA remains unclear, as prior research supported both directions.21 SSD individuals might exhibit NA in response to non-stressful situations,21,22 while NA could also worsen psychotic experiences.4,23

Emotion regulation refers to the processes to modify the frequency, intensity, and duration of emotional experiences,24 and has 2 broad types—suppression and reappraisal.25 The former type reduces the expressions of explicit behaviors induced by emotions, while the latter type reinterprets the situations that have elicited the experienced emotions.25 In typical-developing children, frequent emotion suppression is related to higher NA,26 while frequent reappraisal is related to lower NA.27 SSD individuals typically have difficulties in emotion regulation.28,29 In adult populations, the 3 schizotypal dimensions were correlated positively with suppression.30 People prone to develop delusions (ie, positive schizotypy) were impaired in reappraisal rather than suppression.31 However, reappraisal might reduce NA in people with early psychosis.32,33

Schizotypal traits, emotion regulation, and NA are intricately interrelated. It is necessary to take into account emotion regulation, when we clarify the relationship between schizotypal traits and NA. Traditional statistical models view symptoms as manifestations of the latent disease, which bear a priori assumption of causality. Network analysis, however, views mental disorders as complex systems of interacting symptoms, employing a data-driven approach without any a priori hypothesis.34,35 The key advantage of network analysis allows us to examine the independent relationships between 2 variables while controlling for others.36,37 Moreover, network analysis provides centrality indices to illustrate the interconnectedness of variables35,38 and bridge centrality indices to identify variables that connect different theoretical communities.39 While partial correlation networks are “undirected,” Bayesian networks can employ Directed Acyclic Graphs (DAGs) to represent the overall dependence structure, for exploring possible causal directions.40–43 Bayesian networks are useful to study psychosis.40–43 For example, Moffa et al44 employed DAGs and found that sexual abuse and bullying apparently drove affective symptoms via worry, leading to hallucinations. Although not “confirming” causality, DAGs could unveil complex mechanisms of psychotic disorders, and guide further research on causality issues.44,45

This study aimed to unveil the complex interrelationship between schizotypal traits, NA, and emotion regulation in children, at the dimension level. We also tentatively explored the directional dependency between schizotypal traits and NA using Bayesian networks. We preliminarily examined the effects of age, gender, and high/low schizotypal traits using network comparison tests (NCTs). Based on previous research, we hypothesized that NA would correlate positively with the cognitive–perceptual and disorganized dimensions of schizotypal traits, rather than with the interpersonal dimension. Second, schizotypal trait dimensions would correlate negatively with emotion reappraisal, but positively with emotion suppression. Third, NA would correlate negatively with reappraisal, but positively with suppression. Given the previous mixed findings, we did not make any specific hypothesis on network comparisons, nor assume specific directional dependency between variables.

Method

Participants

We recruited 1333 primarily school students in grades 4–6 from 2 schools in Jiangxi and Shandong provinces. Data were collected via a standardized procedure, so that participants completed self-report questionnaires in quiet environments, where trained assistants were ready to address participants’ questions. Data gathered from 314 children were excluded due to missing data (n = 295), encoding errors (n = 16) and aged >13 (n = 2), aged < 9 (n = 1). Our final sample comprised 1019 participants with complete and valid data (boys = 540, girls = 479, Mage = 10.39, SDage = 0.96). Table 1 shows participants’ demographics. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institution of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (protocol number: H20033). Participants’ guardians provided written informed consent.

Table 1.

Descriptive Characteristics of the Entire Sample

Variables and the dimensions (N = 1019)MinimumMaximumMeanSDSkewnessKurtosis
Age (y)91210.390.960.06−0.95
Length of education (y)4650.82−0.01−1.52
SPQ-C total score0226.284.620.51−0.41
SPQ-C cognitive perceptual082.832.080.37−0.73
SPQ-C interpersonal082.392.060.57−0.55
SPQ-C disorgani zed061.061.371.401.38
ERQ-CA total score105028.629.57−0.15−0.53
ERQ-CA reappraisal63018.336.72−0.16−0.89
ERQ-CA suppression42010.284.320.30−0.73
DASS-21 total score06310.9610.951.492.47
DASS-21 depression0212.933.891.903.80
DASS-21 anxiety0214.114.081.301.52
DASS-21 stress0213.914.051.381.97
Variables and the dimensions (N = 1019)MinimumMaximumMeanSDSkewnessKurtosis
Age (y)91210.390.960.06−0.95
Length of education (y)4650.82−0.01−1.52
SPQ-C total score0226.284.620.51−0.41
SPQ-C cognitive perceptual082.832.080.37−0.73
SPQ-C interpersonal082.392.060.57−0.55
SPQ-C disorgani zed061.061.371.401.38
ERQ-CA total score105028.629.57−0.15−0.53
ERQ-CA reappraisal63018.336.72−0.16−0.89
ERQ-CA suppression42010.284.320.30−0.73
DASS-21 total score06310.9610.951.492.47
DASS-21 depression0212.933.891.903.80
DASS-21 anxiety0214.114.081.301.52
DASS-21 stress0213.914.051.381.97

Abbreviations: DASS-21, The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; ERQ-CA, Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescent; SPQ-C, Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire—Children.

Table 1.

Descriptive Characteristics of the Entire Sample

Variables and the dimensions (N = 1019)MinimumMaximumMeanSDSkewnessKurtosis
Age (y)91210.390.960.06−0.95
Length of education (y)4650.82−0.01−1.52
SPQ-C total score0226.284.620.51−0.41
SPQ-C cognitive perceptual082.832.080.37−0.73
SPQ-C interpersonal082.392.060.57−0.55
SPQ-C disorgani zed061.061.371.401.38
ERQ-CA total score105028.629.57−0.15−0.53
ERQ-CA reappraisal63018.336.72−0.16−0.89
ERQ-CA suppression42010.284.320.30−0.73
DASS-21 total score06310.9610.951.492.47
DASS-21 depression0212.933.891.903.80
DASS-21 anxiety0214.114.081.301.52
DASS-21 stress0213.914.051.381.97
Variables and the dimensions (N = 1019)MinimumMaximumMeanSDSkewnessKurtosis
Age (y)91210.390.960.06−0.95
Length of education (y)4650.82−0.01−1.52
SPQ-C total score0226.284.620.51−0.41
SPQ-C cognitive perceptual082.832.080.37−0.73
SPQ-C interpersonal082.392.060.57−0.55
SPQ-C disorgani zed061.061.371.401.38
ERQ-CA total score105028.629.57−0.15−0.53
ERQ-CA reappraisal63018.336.72−0.16−0.89
ERQ-CA suppression42010.284.320.30−0.73
DASS-21 total score06310.9610.951.492.47
DASS-21 depression0212.933.891.903.80
DASS-21 anxiety0214.114.081.301.52
DASS-21 stress0213.914.051.381.97

Abbreviations: DASS-21, The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; ERQ-CA, Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescent; SPQ-C, Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire—Children.

Measurements

Schizotypal Traits

The self-report Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire—children (SPQ-C)46,47 has 22 items and 3 (cognitive–perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized) subscales. Each item is rated on a yes/no choice. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the total score, the cognitive–perceptual, the interpersonal, and the disorganized subscale scores were 0.84, 0.69, 0.71, and 0.67, respectively. The SPQ-C has been validated in the Chinese setting,46,47 applicable to children aged 8–16 years.46,47

Emotion Regulation

We administered the modified version of the self-report Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescence (ERQ-CA),48 which has demonstrated good reliability and validity in Chinese primary school students aged 9–12.49 Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the ERQ-CA suppression and reappraisal were 0.67 and 0.80, respectively, in this study.

Depression, Anxiety, and Stress

The short-form version of the self-report Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21) was used to measure negative affect.50 DASS-21 comprises 3 factors, including anxiety (7 items), depression (7 items), and stress (7 items). Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale. DASS-21 has been validated in Chinese children and adolescents with good reliability and validity.51 The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the total score, and depression, anxiety, and stress subscale scores were 0.92, 0.82, 0.77, and 0.79, respectively, in this study.51

Data Analysis

Descriptive Analysis

Descriptive and comparative analyses were conducted using the SPSS 22 software. Independent sample t-test (2-tailed) compared the gender-stratified groups in age, emotion regulation, schizotypal traits, and negative affect. The threshold for significance was set at P < .05. Effect size was estimated as Cohen’s d. We examined the kurtosis and skewness of all measurements. In this study, we did not remove participants with extreme values of these scales as “outliers,” because they might represent people with high levels of schizotypy who had highly negative affect and highly altered emotion regulation. However, as sensitivity analysis, we repeated the same analysis after the removal of outliers,52 as shown in Supplementary Material 2.

The Network Analysis for the Entire Sample

We constructed network analysis according to Epskamp, Borsboom, and Fried’s guidelines35,53 using the qgraph package in R version 4.2.3 (R script is shown in Supplementary Material 3). First, variables were transformed into z-scores for comparability and standardization. For the entire sample, we used the SPQ-C subscale scores, ERQ-CA subscale scores, and DASS-21 subscale scores as nodes in the network. The edges represented the partial correlations between each pair of nodes, after controlling for all other nodes. Given the variables (nodes) followed a non-normal distribution, Spearman’s partial correlations were used to estimate the network. The Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator combined with Extended Bayesian Information Criteria model selection was applied, to shrink relatively weak correlations to zero, and limit “false-positive” edges. To balance the inclusion of genuine and spurious edges, we set the tuning parameter to its default value of 0.5.35 Edges in blue indicated positive correlations; edges in red indicated negative correlations. Thicker edges indicated stronger correlations between nodes. Node centrality indices (strength, betweenness, and closeness), predictability, and expected influence (EI) for each node were estimated. We further estimated the stability of the centrality indices, and the accuracy of edge weights, using the R bootnet package.

Bridge Centrality Indices

We estimated bridge centrality to identify the bridging nodes that connect different communities in the network, using the networktools package in R.39 Bridge strength, bridge closeness, bridge betweenness, and bridge EI (1-step) were estimated. These indices quantify the importance of a node in linking other communities. For instance, bridge strength refers to the sum of connectivity between a node and all nodes that are not in the same community within the network. To avoid confirmation biases, we employed an 80th percentile cutoff to select bridge nodes.39

Network Comparison

We employed the NCT in R,54 and assessed the impact of gender, age group, and schizotypy level (high/low) on network patterns. For gender differences, we constructed networks for gender-stratified groups, and compared them in terms of network structure, global strength, edge weights, and centrality indices using 2-tailed permutation tests with 10 000 iterations. The invariance of network structure was tested by evaluating the maximum difference between connection strength matrices. The global strength of the networks was tested by the sum of all absolute connection strengths between all node pairs. Given its broad age range, we divided our sample into 2 groups, namely a subgroup aged 9–10 (n = 544) and another subgroup aged 11–12 (n = 475). Using NCT, we preliminarily examined the potential effect of age on the interrelationship between variables of interest. We further categorized our sample into the high (n = 452) and low (n = 483) schizotypal groups, using a median-split of the SPQ-C total score,55 and conducted NCT.

Bayesian Network

We tentatively explored the directions of the relationship between schizotypal traits and negative affect, using the bnlearn package in R for DAG network analysis.40,56,57 This method has already been used to explore causal relationships related to schizophrenia.40–42 In DAGs, the lack of a direct edge indicates that 2 nodes are independent, or conditionally independent after accounting for the other nodes, whereas the presence of a direct edge indicates a dependence between 2 nodes after conditioning on any other nodes.55,58

Following the guidelines,59 we employed the Hill-Climbing (HC) algorithm,59 a score-based greedy search method, to obtain the Bayesian network structure that best fits the data, based on the Bayesian Information Criterion. We applied the HC algorithm to 1000 bootstrap samples for stability, with each DAG inference including 5 random restarts and 10 perturbations per restart to reduce the risk of local maxima60 (for details, see McNally et al60). The edges in the DAG were determined by arc strength, reflecting their frequency in the bootstrap networks.59 We retained edges with a frequency of at least 85% in the final “consensus” network. Before this, all learned DAGs were transformed into Completed Partially Directed Acyclic Graphs.56,61 After determining the edges, we assessed the frequency of each edge direction in the bootstrap networks. If an edge direction was estimated over 50% of the networks, it was included in the final network.59,60

It should be borne in mind that the influence of unmeasured confounders, such as common causes of 2 or more nodes in the DAG, could potentially undermine the DAG results.55,58 However, it is practically impossible for DAG to include all potential variables and confounders,45,62 therefore the DAG results must be interpreted with caution.

Results

Regularized Partial Correlation Network

The regularized partial correlation network is shown in Figure 1A. After controlling for the inter-relationships between nodes, we observed positive partial correlations between the cognitive–perceptual (SPQ-CP), interpersonal (SPQ-I), and disorganized (SPQ-D) dimensions of schizotypal traits, and negative affect (ie, depressed mood, anxiety, and stress) (edge values = 0.02–0.20). The difference test for edge weights (see Supplementary Figure S1.2) showed that the correlation between SPQ-CP and anxiety was significantly greater than that between SPQ-D, SPQ-I, and anxiety.

(A) Regularized Partial Correlation Network for the Whole Sample. Each Node Represents a Variable. Each Edge Represents the Partial Correlation Between Two Nodes Controlled for All Other Nodes. Thicker Lines Represent Stronger Connections. The Value of Each Edge Represents the Strength of the Partial Correlations. Positive Edge Values Indicate Positive Partial Correlations and Negative Edge Values Indicate Negative Partial Correlations. The Pie Chart Around Each Node Represents the Predictability Values, Which Indicate Prediction of a Specific Node by Other Nodes in the Network. (B) The Standardized Centrality Estimates of the Whole-Sample Network. Abbreviations: DASS.A, Anxiety Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.D, Depression Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.S, Stress Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; ERQ.R, Reappraisal Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; ERQ.S, Suppression Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; SPQ.CP, Cognitive Perceptual Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.D, Disorganized Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.I, Interpersonal Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire
Figure 1.

(A) Regularized Partial Correlation Network for the Whole Sample. Each Node Represents a Variable. Each Edge Represents the Partial Correlation Between Two Nodes Controlled for All Other Nodes. Thicker Lines Represent Stronger Connections. The Value of Each Edge Represents the Strength of the Partial Correlations. Positive Edge Values Indicate Positive Partial Correlations and Negative Edge Values Indicate Negative Partial Correlations. The Pie Chart Around Each Node Represents the Predictability Values, Which Indicate Prediction of a Specific Node by Other Nodes in the Network. (B) The Standardized Centrality Estimates of the Whole-Sample Network. Abbreviations: DASS.A, Anxiety Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.D, Depression Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.S, Stress Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; ERQ.R, Reappraisal Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; ERQ.S, Suppression Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; SPQ.CP, Cognitive Perceptual Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.D, Disorganized Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.I, Interpersonal Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire

Moreover, the SPQ-I and SPQ-D nodes were positively correlated with emotion suppression (edge values = 0.11 and 0.03, respectively). The SPQ-CP node was positively correlated with emotion reappraisal (edge value = 0.05), and the SPQ-I node was negatively correlated with emotion reappraisal (edge value = −0.04). Partial positive correlations were found between emotion suppression and nodes of negative affect (ie, depressed mood, anxiety, and stress) (edge values = 0.01–0.13). Partial negative correlations were found between emotion reappraisal, depressed mood, and stress (edge values = −0.12 and −0.02, respectively).

The relationships between schizotypal traits and the negative affect observed remained stable in the sensitivity analysis. Additionally, there were no significant differences in NCTs between the sensitivity analysis network (excluding “outliers”) and the present network (see Supplementary Material 2 for details), indicating the stability of the results.

Network Inference

Centrality indices are shown in Figure 1B. Based on the difference test for node centrality in Supplementary Figure S1.3, stress had significantly higher strength than all other nodes (except depressed mood). Stress had significantly higher EI than other nodes. Depressed mood displayed higher closeness centrality than emotion suppression, emotion reappraisal, and SPQ-D, but was not significantly higher than the remaining nodes. The predictability of nodes ranged from 26% (emotion reappraisal) to 69% (stress), with an average of 50%. Supplementary Table S1.1 details the node centrality indices. Sensitivity analyses showed similar results, indicating the stability of the results.

Bridging Centrality Indices

Bridge centrality indices are displayed in Supplementary Figure S1.4. Using an 80th quantile cutoff, depressed mood and stress had the highest bridge strength, stress, and SPQ-I had the highest bridge EI (1-level), and depressed mood and SPQ-I showed the highest bridge closeness. However, according to the difference test for bridge node centrality (see Supplementary Figure S1.5), most of the differences between nodes failed to reach statistical significance, except that the bridge EI of emotion reappraisal was significantly lower than all other nodes. Sensitivity analyses showed similar results, indicating the stability of the results.

NCT Results Between Boys and Girls

Independent sample t-tests (see Supplementary Table S1.4) showed that girls scored higher than boys in all dimensions of schizotypal traits, emotion suppression and reappraisal, and NA (depressed mood, anxiety, and stress). Supplementary Figure S1.7 shows the regularized partial correlation networks for boys and girls. NCT found the 2 gender-stratified networks had comparable network structure (ie, the maximum difference in edge weights was 0.15, P = .22) and global strength (ie, the strength difference was 0.29, P = .17, global strength = 3.44 for boys, and 3.73 for girls). Regarding the centrality indices, emotion reappraisal demonstrated a larger EI (P < .05) in the girls’ rather than boy’s network. NCT also found comparable edge weights between the 2 gender-stratified networks (all Ps > .05). However, in the sensitivity analysis excluding the potential outliers, the independent sample t-tests showed that the gender differences in the SPQ-CP and SPQ-D dimensions were not significant. Moreover, the gender difference in emotion reappraisal in terms of EI disappeared.

NCT Results Between the Two Age Groups

For age effect, independent sample t-tests (see Supplementary Table S1.5) showed that children aged 11–12 scored higher than children aged 9–10 in SPQ-D and depressed mood. The regularized partial correlation networks for subgroups aged 9–10 and 11–12 are shown in Supplementary Figure S1.8. NCT showed significant differences in global strength (ie, the strength difference was 0.41, P < .05, with a global strength of 3.24 for the subgroup aged 9–10 and 3.65 for the subgroup aged 11–12), and the network structure (ie, the maximum difference in edge weights was 0.21, P < .01). Additionally, NCT found children aged 11–12 having a stronger correlation between SPQ-CP and anxiety, compared with children aged 9–10 (P < .05). After sensitivity analysis, the findings regarding the subgroup difference in anxiety—SPQ-CP (P < .05) correlation, and network structure (P < .01) remained the same.

NCT Results Between the High and Low Schizotypy Subgroups

As shown in Supplementary Table S1.6, the high schizotypy group exhibited higher levels of schizotypal traits, NA, and used both higher emotion suppression and reappraisal than the low schizotypy group. Supplementary Figure S1.9 displays the regularized partial correlation networks for 2 groups. NCT showed significant differences in the network structure (ie, the maximum difference in edge weights was 0.34, P < .001), but not global strength (ie, the strength difference was 0.57, P = .12, with a global strength of 2.70 for the high schizotypy subgroup and 3.28 for the low schizotypy subgroup).

Moreover, the low schizotypy subgroup demonstrated higher strength in SPQ-CP, higher strength in emotion suppression, and higher strength in reappraisal, as well as higher EI of SPQ-CP and higher EI of emotion reappraisal than the high schizotypy subgroup (Ps < .05). NCT also found significant differences in edge weights after FDR (False Discovery Rate) correction, including SPQ-CP—Reappraisal (P < .05), SPQ-CP—Suppression (P < .05), SPQ-D—DASS Depressed mood (P < .05), Reappraisal—Suppression (P < .01), SPQ-CP—SPQ-I (P < .05), and SPQ-I—SPQ-D (P < .05). Sensitivity analyses showed stable results, implicating that the between-subgroup differences in the network structure, EI of emotion reappraisal, and the Reappraisal—Suppression correlation remained significant (Ps < .05).

Network Stability and Accuracy

Figure 2A shows relatively narrow bootstrapped CIs, suggesting that edge weights were reliable.37 The recommended threshold of the correlation stability (CS) coefficients is 0.5.37 The CS coefficients for strength, closeness, and EI were 0.75, 0.67, and 0.75, respectively, suggesting stability. The CS coefficients for bridge strength, bridge closeness, and bridge EI were 0.60, 0.52, and 0.75, respectively, suggesting stability (see Supplementary Figure S1.6). However, the CS coefficients for betweenness and bridge betweenness were both below 0.05, indicating low stability.

(A) Bootstrapped 95% CIs of Edge Weights of the Whole-Sample Network. The Shaded Area Represents the Bootstrapped CI and the Sample Line Represents the Edge Weight. (B) The Stability of Centrality Indicators Computed by Case-Dropping Bootstrap. The Lines Represent the Average Correlation Between the Central Indicators Estimated From the Bootstrapping Subsamples and the Central Indicators Estimated From the Original Sample. The Areas Represent a Range From 2.5th to 97.5th Quantiles. Abbreviations: DASS.A, Anxiety Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.D, Depression Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.S, Stress Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; ERQ.R, Reappraisal Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; ERQ.S, Suppression Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; SPQ.CP, Cognitive Perceptual Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.D, Disorganized Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.I, Interpersonal Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire
Figure 2.

(A) Bootstrapped 95% CIs of Edge Weights of the Whole-Sample Network. The Shaded Area Represents the Bootstrapped CI and the Sample Line Represents the Edge Weight. (B) The Stability of Centrality Indicators Computed by Case-Dropping Bootstrap. The Lines Represent the Average Correlation Between the Central Indicators Estimated From the Bootstrapping Subsamples and the Central Indicators Estimated From the Original Sample. The Areas Represent a Range From 2.5th to 97.5th Quantiles. Abbreviations: DASS.A, Anxiety Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.D, Depression Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DASS.S, Stress Dimension of Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; ERQ.R, Reappraisal Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; ERQ.S, Suppression Dimension of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; SPQ.CP, Cognitive Perceptual Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.D, Disorganized Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; SPQ.I, Interpersonal Dimension of Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire

Bayesian Network

Bayesian network could explore the directional relationship between schizotypal traits and negative affect. Nodes located in the upstream (near the top) of the DAG network were sources of activation to drive other nodes. Supplementary Figure S1.10 shows the DAG for the whole sample. Stress was an influential node to drive the 3 dimensions of schizotypal traits. The depressed mood had an edge pointing toward SPQ-I and SPQ-D. The SPQ-CP node had an edge pointing toward anxiety.

The SPQ-I had an edge pointing toward emotion suppression. In the sensitivity analysis, some findings remained consistent. Stress remained a significant node driving all 3 dimensions of schizotypal traits, depressed mood had an edge pointing to SPQ-D, and SPQ-I had an edge pointing toward emotion suppression. We used an 85% threshold to select edges, and a 50% threshold to determine direction. However, different thresholds were used in previous studies to select edges (eg, 50%,63 75%,40 and 85%57,60,64), which could affect the results. Thus, we provided details for any edge with an arc strength greater than 50% for reference in Supplementary Tables S1.7–S1.11.

Given the significant differences found in the network comparisons between age groups and high/low schizotypy subgroups, we further conducted DAGs for these subgroups separately. In the DAG for the high schizotypy group (n = 452), SPQ-I had 2 edges pointing toward depressed mood and emotion suppression, and influenced stress and anxiety through depressed mood. The depressed mood had an edge pointing toward SPQ-D. In the DAG for the low schizotypy group (n = 482), depressed mood appeared to drive anxiety, and stress (see Supplementary Figure S1.11). Additionally, anxiety had an edge pointing toward SPQ-CP, with an arc strength of 82% (very close to the 85% threshold). In the sensitivity analysis for the high schizotypy subgroup, SPQ-I still had an edge pointing toward depressed mood with an arc strength of 83% (very close to the 85% threshold). Depressed mood still had an edge pointing to SPQ-D. In the sensitivity analysis for the low schizotypal subgroup, depressed mood still appeared to drive anxiety and stress, and anxiety still has an edge pointing toward SPQ-CP.

In the DAG for children aged 9–10, the depressed mood had an edge pointing toward SPQ-D, while stress pointing toward both SPQ-D and SPQ-CP. For children aged 11–12, the DAG showed that depressed mood had an edge pointing toward SPQ-D, anxiety pointing toward SPQ-CP, and both depressed mood and stress pointing toward SPQ-I (see Supplementary Figure S1.12). In the sensitivity analysis, we identified similar directional relationships, with negative affect mainly pointing to the dimensions of schizotypal traits rather than the other way around (see Supplementary Material 2, S2.4 for details).

Discussions

This study revealed the inter-relationship between schizotypal traits, emotion regulation, and negative affect in a large sample of children. The key findings can be summarized as follows. In the regularized partial correlation network, all 3 dimensions of schizotypal traits were positively correlated with negative affect. Emotion suppression was positively correlated with all negative affect nodes, while emotion reappraisal was negatively correlated with depressed mood and stress. Additionally, we found positive correlations of emotion suppression with SPQ-I and SPQ-D nodes. Emotion reappraisal correlated positively with SPQ-CP, and negatively with SPQ-I. Stress was pivotal in the network, as revealed in the node centrality indices. The DAGs revealed that stress plays a role in driving schizotypal traits. Moreover, gender, age, and schizotypy level (high/low) appeared to have an impact on the network patterns.

We examined the relationship between schizotypal traits and negative affect. Positive correlations were found between all dimensions of schizotypal traits and negative affect after controlling other variables in the network, consistent with earlier findings in adolescents10,65 and adults.11,66 This indicates that negative affect and schizotypal traits have been closely related during childhood. Previous research in adults has found that SPQ-D appears to show a stronger association with negative affect.18–20 However, in our sample of children, SPQ-CP rather than other SPQ nodes was more related to anxiety. Such inconsistent results might be due to sampling differences, and the fact that previous studies did not categorize different types of NA. Future research recruits samples with different age ranges, and categorizes NA to further clarify the relationship between schizotypal dimensions and negative affect. Our explorative DAG findings indicated that stress and depressed mood in children appeared to drive schizotypal traits. Additionally, stress exacerbated schizotypal symptoms via depressed mood and anxiety, partially concurring with previous findings on the mediating role of negative affect in the relationship between stress and mental health.30 Previous research has indicated that negative affect may play a role in the development and exacerbation of schizotypal symptoms. From a developmental perspective, schizotypal traits in developing children may be insufficient to determine the development of psychosis,21,67 negative affect may serve as aggravating factors to synergize the impact of schizotypal traits.15,67 However, causality may also be bidirectional between negative affect and schizotypal traits.21 It is important to note that DAGs do not confirm causality,45 and their results should be further clarified in future longitudinal studies.

Regarding the role of emotion regulation, we found that greater use of suppression was associated with more depressed mood, anxiety, and stress, while greater use of emotion reappraisal was associated with less depressed mood and stress. These are consistent with previous findings on suppression26,68 and reappraisal26,69 in children, and further support that suppression is a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy, while reappraisal is adaptive.26,69 Regarding schizotypal traits and emotion regulation, we found that SPQ-I and SPQ-D were positively correlated with emotion suppression, and emotion reappraisal was correlated positively with SPQ-CP but negatively with SPQ-I. A study using experience sampling showed that individuals with psychosis and active delusions had used maladaptive strategies more often than healthy people, and had used reappraisal more frequently.33 Such findings concurred with ours. Having said that, we found that emotion reappraisal had weaker relationships with other nodes in the network, as evidenced by its significantly lowest EI centrality and bridge EI. As children and adolescents grow older, emotion reappraisal might become more frequent and effective.70–72

The extant literature generally suggested that reliability of closeness and betweenness would be poor,73,74 and strength and EI can be better indicators of central nodes. In our study, stress showed significantly higher strength than all other nodes, except depressed mood, and stress also showed the highest EI, suggesting that stress might play an important role in the network. Although some network analysis studies regarded high-centrality and bridge nodes as intervention targets,38,39 recent research advocated a more conservative approach to interpret these indices.75,76 Future research can further explore the roles of these central and bridge nodes to better understand their potential roles.

Our preliminary NCT results also explored the effects of age, gender, and schizotypy levels on the network. Although girls scored significantly higher than boys on SPQ, negative affect, emotion suppression, and reappraisal, the NCT results revealed no significant between-gender differences in the network, except that emotion reappraisal had higher EI in girls than boys. Such findings suggested that reappraisal might be more important for girls, consistently with the notion that girls generally have greater awareness and intention to regulate emotions than boys of the same age.77 However, prior research using children of similar age groups reported mixed results, regarding gender differences.47,78–82 Our findings should be considered preliminary and interpreted with caution. Furthermore, our findings suggested significant effects of age, because the network structure and global strength between the subgroups aged 9–10 and 11–12 differed significantly. Specifically, children aged 11–12 had a stronger connection between SPQ-CP and anxiety compared with those aged 9–10. The DAG results also revealed differences across different age groups. It is noteworthy that our findings remained preliminary. Future research could recruit samples with a wider age range, and utilize longitudinal designs to better understand developmental changes. Additionally, our findings supported that the high and low schizotypy subgroups differed significantly. For instance, the connection between SPQ-D and depressed mood was stronger in the high schizotypy group. The DAG analysis showed an effect of SPQ-I on depressed mood in the high schizotypy group, but not in the low schizotypy group. A previous network analysis in adults also found stronger correlations between schizotypal traits and negative affect in the high schizotypy group.83 Having said that, the use of NCT to explore the effects of age, gender, and high versus low schizotypy level was preliminary, and further research in this area is necessary.

This study has several limitations. First, we did not collect sufficient socioeconomic information, which might have confounded the findings. However, a recent study on Chinese children did not find any significant correlation between the ratings on the SPQ-C and demographics.47 Second, we did not include a lie scale to detect random or dishonest responses, thus undermined the validity of our findings. Third, we used self-report scales only, but many variables of interest, such as emotion regulation, can be measured objectively by laboratory-based paradigms. Fourth, our sample had a narrowed age range, which limited the generalizability. Fifth, we only measured emotion reappraisal and suppression, but other strategies such as rumination and acceptance may also be important in modifying psychopathology. Finally, network analyses have potential limitations, including problems of generalizability and reproducibility of network estimation.76 The centrality indices have been criticized for lacking a theoretical foundation.76 Targeting central and bridging symptoms based on a single time-point identification is not the ideal approach for network-guided interventions.74 DAGs with cross-sectional design precluded the possibility to infer causality, and our findings need to be verified in future longitudinal studies. HC algorithms may yield suboptimal networks and biased results, however, these issues can be partially mitigated with ad hoc computational strategies.60 In our study, some important variables (ie, childhood trauma) were not measured,84,85 limiting both the partial correlation and DAG-based analyses. Future research should consider incorporating these variables in the networks.

To conclude, this network analysis suggested that children with higher levels of schizotypal traits may experience more negative affect and are more likely to use suppression to regulate these emotions. The Bayesian network revealed that stress may serve as a conditional influential node for schizotypal traits. Our preliminary NCT findings suggested that age and schizotypy level (high/low) might influence the network properties. Our work paved the way for future longitudinal or experimental studies, to guide the identification of intervention targets and to understand the pathological mechanisms in children.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material is available at https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/schizophreniabulletin/.

Funding

This study was supported by the Scientific Foundation of Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (E2CX3415CX), and the Philip K. H. Wong Foundation to Raymond Chan.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

References

1.

Lenzenweger
 
MF.
 
Schizotypy, schizotypic psychopathology, and schizophrenia: hearing echoes, leveraging prior advances, and probing new angles
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2018
;
44
(
suppl 2
):
S564
S569
. https://doi.org/

2.

Meehl
 
PE.
 
Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia
.
Am Psychol.
 
1962
;
17
:
827
838
. https://doi.org/

3.

Fonseca-Pedrero
 
E
,
Debbané
 
M
,
Rodríguez-Testal
 
JF
,
Cohen
 
AS
,
Docherty
 
AR
,
Ortuño-Sierra
 
J.
 
Schizotypy: the way ahead
.
Psicothema.
 
2021
;
33
:
16
27
. https://doi.org/

4.

Fonseca Pedrero
 
E
,
Debbané
 
M.
 
Schizotypal traits and psychotic-like experiences during adolescence: an update
.
Psicothema.
 
2017
;
29
:
5
17
. https://doi.org/

5.

Abu-Akel
 
A
,
Testa
 
RR
,
Jones
 
HP
, et al. Attentional set-shifting and social abilities in children with schizotypal and comorbid autism spectrum disorders.
2018
. Accessed
June 13, 2024
. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0004867417708610?url_ver=Z39.88-2003&rfr_id=ori:rid:crossref.org&rfr_dat=cr_pub%20%200pubmed

6.

Jones
 
HP
,
Testa
 
RR
,
Ross
 
N
,
Seal
 
ML
,
Pantelis
 
C
,
Tonge
 
B.
 
The Melbourne assessment of schizotypy in kids: a useful measure of childhood schizotypal personality disorder
.
Biomed Res Int.
 
2015
;
2015
:
635732
. https://doi.org/

7.

Wang
 
Y
,
Harding
 
IH
,
Testa
 
R
, et al.  
Structural and functional brain abnormalities in children with schizotypal disorder: a pilot study
.
npj Schizophr.
 
2020
;
6
:
6
. https://doi.org/

8.

Poulton
 
R
,
Caspi
 
A
,
Moffitt
 
T
, et al.  
Children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
 
2000
;
57
:
1053
. https://doi.org/

9.

Debbané
 
M
,
Eliez
 
S
,
Badoud
 
D
,
Conus
 
P
,
Flückiger
 
R
,
Schultze-Lutter
 
F.
 
Developing psychosis and its risk states through the lens of schizotypy
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2015
;
41
(
suppl 2
):
S396
S407
. https://doi.org/

10.

Yung
 
AR
,
Nelson
 
B
,
Baker
 
K
,
Buckby
 
JA
,
Baksheev
 
G
,
Cosgrave
 
EM.
 
Psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of adolescents: implications for the continuum model of psychosis and prediction of schizophrenia
.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry.
 
2009
;
43
:
118
128
. https://doi.org/

11.

Armando
 
M
,
Nelson
 
B
,
Yung
 
AR
, et al.  
Psychotic-like experiences and correlation with distress and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents and young adults
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2010
;
119
:
258
265
. https://doi.org/

12.

Hall
 
J.
 
Schizophrenia—an anxiety disorder
?
Br J Psychiatry.
 
2017
;
211
:
262
263
. https://doi.org/

13.

Debbané
 
M
,
Van der Linden
 
M
,
Gex-Fabry
 
M
,
Eliez
 
S.
 
Cognitive and emotional associations to positive schizotypy during adolescence
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry.
 
2009
;
50
:
326
334
. https://doi.org/

14.

Horan
 
WP
,
Green
 
MF
,
Kring
 
AM
,
Nuechterlein
 
KH.
 
Does anhedonia in schizophrenia reflect faulty memory for subjectively experienced emotions
?
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2006
;
115
:
496
508
. https://doi.org/

15.

Salokangas
 
RKR
,
Ruhrmann
 
S
,
von Reventlow
 
HG
, et al. ;
EPOS group
.
Axis I diagnoses and transition to psychosis in clinical high-risk patients EPOS project: prospective follow-up of 245 clinical high-risk outpatients in four countries
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2012
;
138
:
192
197
. https://doi.org/

16.

Fusar-Poli
 
P
,
Nelson
 
B
,
Valmaggia
 
L
,
Yung
 
AR
,
McGuire
 
PK.
 
Comorbid depressive and anxiety disorders in 509 individuals with an at-risk mental state: impact on psychopathology and transition to psychosis
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2014
;
40
:
120
131
. https://doi.org/

17.

Lewandowski
 
KE
,
Barrantes-Vidal
 
N
,
Nelson-Gray
 
RO
,
Clancy
 
C
,
Kepley
 
HO
,
Kwapil
 
TR.
 
Anxiety and depression symptoms in psychometrically identified schizotypy
.
Schizophr Res.
 
2006
;
83
:
225
235
. https://doi.org/

18.

Kemp
 
KC
,
Gross
 
GM
,
Barrantes-Vidal
 
N
,
Kwapil
 
TR.
 
Association of positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy dimensions with affective symptoms and experiences
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2018
;
270
:
1143
1149
. https://doi.org/

19.

Kemp
 
KC
,
Sperry
 
SH
,
Hernández
 
L
,
Barrantes-Vidal
 
N
,
Kwapil
 
TR.
 
Affective dynamics in daily life are differentially expressed in positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy
.
J Psychopathol Clin Sci.
 
2023
;
132
:
110
121
. https://doi.org/

20.

Kwapil
 
TR
,
Kemp
 
KC
,
Mielock
 
A
, et al.  
Association of multidimensional schizotypy with psychotic-like experiences, affect, and social functioning in daily life: comparable findings across samples and schizotypy measures
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2020
;
129
:
492
504
. https://doi.org/

21.

Taylor
 
SF
,
Grove
 
TB
,
Ellingrod
 
VL
,
Tso
 
IF.
 
The fragile brain: stress vulnerability, negative affect and GABAergic neurocircuits in psychosis
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2019
;
45
:
1170
1183
. https://doi.org/

22.

Ponizovsky
 
AM
,
Vitenberg
 
E
,
Baumgarten-Katz
 
I
,
Grinshpoon
 
A.
 
Attachment styles and affect regulation among outpatients with schizophrenia: relationships to symptomatology and emotional distress
.
Psychol Psychother.
 
2013
;
86
:
164
182
. https://doi.org/

23.

Muddle
 
S
,
Jones
 
B
,
Taylor
 
G
,
Jacobsen
 
P.
 
A systematic review and meta-analysis of the association between emotional stress reactivity and psychosis
.
Early Interv Psychiatry.
 
2022
;
16
:
958
978
. https://doi.org/

24.

Gross
 
JJ
,
Jazaieri
 
H.
 
Emotion, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: an affective science perspective
.
Clin Psychol Sci.
 
2014
;
2
:
387
401
. https://doi.org/

25.

Gross
 
JJ.
 
Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences
.
Psychophysiology.
 
2002
;
39
:
281
291
. https://doi.org/

26.

Compas
 
BE
,
Jaser
 
SS
,
Bettis
 
AH
, et al.  
Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analysis and narrative review
.
Psychol Bull.
 
2017
;
143
:
939
991
. https://doi.org/

27.

Andrés
 
ML
,
Richaud de Minzi
 
MC
,
Castañeiras
 
C
,
Canet-Juric
 
L
,
Rodríguez-Carvajal
 
R.
 
Neuroticism and depression in children: the role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies
.
J Genet Psychol.
 
2016
;
177
:
55
71
. https://doi.org/

28.

van der Velde
 
J
,
Opmeer
 
EM
,
Liemburg
 
EJ
, et al.  
Lower prefrontal activation during emotion regulation in subjects at ultrahigh risk for psychosis: an fMRI-study
.
npj Schizophr.
 
2015
;
1
:
15026
. https://doi.org/

29.

Hoid
 
D
,
Pan
 
D
,
Wang
 
Y
,
Li
 
X.
 
Implicit emotion regulation deficits in individuals with high schizotypal traits: an ERP study
.
Sci Rep.
 
2020
;
10
:
3882
. https://doi.org/

30.

Henry
 
JD
,
Green
 
MJ
,
Restuccia
 
C
, et al.  
Emotion dysregulation and schizotypy
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2009
;
166
:
116
124
. https://doi.org/

31.

Westermann
 
S
,
Rief
 
W
,
Lincoln
 
TM.
 
Emotion regulation in delusion-proneness: deficits in cognitive reappraisal, but not in expressive suppression
.
Psychol Psychother.
 
2014
;
87
:
1
14
. https://doi.org/

32.

Bahlinger
 
K
,
Lincoln
 
TM
,
Clamor
 
A.
 
Are acute increases and variability in emotion regulation strategies related to negative affect and paranoid thoughts in daily life
?
Cogn Ther Res.
 
2022
;
46
:
62
72
. https://doi.org/

33.

Ludwig
 
L
,
Mehl
 
S
,
Krkovic
 
K
,
Lincoln
 
TM.
 
Effectiveness of emotion regulation in daily life in individuals with psychosis and nonclinical controls—an experience-sampling study
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2020
;
129
:
408
421
. https://doi.org/

34.

Borsboom
 
D
,
Cramer
 
AOJ
,
Kalis
 
A.
 
Brain disorders? Not really: why network structures block reductionism in psychopathology research
.
Behav Brain Sci.
 
2018
;
42
:
e2
. https://doi.org/

35.

Epskamp
 
S
,
Borsboom
 
D
,
Fried
 
EI.
 
Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper
.
Behav Res Methods.
 
2018
;
50
:
195
212
. https://doi.org/

36.

Bringmann
 
LF
,
Albers
 
C
,
Bockting
 
C
, et al.  
Psychopathological networks: theory, methods and practice
.
Behav Res Ther.
 
2022
;
149
:
104011
. https://doi.org/

37.

Bringmann
 
L
,
Eronen
 
M.
 
Don’t blame the model: reconsidering the network approach to psychopathology
.
Psychol Rev.
 
2018
;
125
:
606
615
. https://doi.org/

38.

Borsboom
 
D
,
Cramer
 
AOJ.
 
Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology
.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol.
 
2013
;
9
:
91
121
. https://doi.org/

39.

Jones
 
PJ
,
Ma
 
R
,
McNally
 
RJ.
 
Bridge centrality: a network approach to understanding comorbidity
.
Multivar Behav Res.
 
2021
;
56
:
353
367
. https://doi.org/

40.

Abplanalp
 
SJ
,
Lee
 
J
,
Horan
 
WP
,
Kern
 
RS
,
Penn
 
DL
,
Green
 
MF.
 
A Bayesian network approach to social and nonsocial cognition in schizophrenia: are some domains more fundamental than others
?
Schizophr Bull.
 
2023
;
49
:
997
1006
. https://doi.org/

41.

Moffa
 
G
,
Catone
 
G
,
Kuipers
 
J
, et al.  
Using directed acyclic graphs in epidemiological research in psychosis: an analysis of the role of bullying in psychosis
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2017
;
43
:
1273
1279
. https://doi.org/

42.

Moffa
 
G
,
Kuipers
 
J
,
Carrà
 
G
, et al.  
Longitudinal symptomatic interactions in long-standing schizophrenia: a novel five-point analysis based on directed acyclic graphs
.
Psychol Med.
 
2023
;
53
:
1371
1378
. https://doi.org/

43.

Bird
 
JC
,
Evans
 
R
,
Waite
 
F
,
Loe
 
BS
,
Freeman
 
D.
 
Adolescent paranoia: prevalence, structure, and causal mechanisms
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2019
;
45
:
1134
1142
. https://doi.org/

44.

Moffa
 
G
,
Kuipers
 
J
,
Kuipers
 
E
,
McManus
 
S
,
Bebbington
 
P.
 
Sexual abuse and psychotic phenomena: a directed acyclic graph analysis of affective symptoms using English national psychiatric survey data
.
Psychol Med.
 
2023
;
53
:
7817
7826
. https://doi.org/

45.

Dawid
 
AP.
 
Beware of the DAG!
In: Proceedings of Workshop on Causality: Objectives and Assessment at NIPS 2008.
PMLR
;
2010
:
59
86
. Accessed
March 18, 2024
. https://proceedings.mlr.press/v6/dawid10a.html

46.

Raine
 
A
,
Fung
 
AL
,
Lam
 
BYH.
 
Peer victimization partially mediates the schizotypy-aggression relationship in children and adolescents
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2011
;
37
:
937
945
. https://doi.org/

47.

Liu
 
J
,
Wong
 
KKY
,
Dong
 
F
,
Raine
 
A
,
Tuvblad
 
C.
 
The Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire—Child (SPQ-C): psychometric properties and relations to behavioral problems with multi-informant ratings
.
Psychiatry Res.
 
2019
;
275
:
204
211
. https://doi.org/

48.

Gullone
 
E
,
Taffe
 
J.
 
The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents (ERQ-CA): a psychometric evaluation
.
Psychol Assess.
 
2012
;
24
:
409
417
. https://doi.org/

49.

Chen
 
L
,
Liu
 
W
,
Zhang
 
X.
 
Chinese Revision of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents
.
Chin J Clin Psychol.
 
2016
;
24
:
259
263
. https://doi.org/

50.

Lovibond
 
PF
,
Lovibond
 
SH.
 
The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories
.
Behav Res Ther.
 
1995
;
33
:
335
343
. https://doi.org/

51.

Zhang
 
F
,
Liu
 
Z
,
Ma
 
Z
,
Hou
 
Q.
 
Application of the short version of depression-anxiety-stress scale to stress assessment on students after earthquake
.
Chin J Behav Med Brain Sci.
 
2016
;
25
:
82
85
. https://doi.org/

52.

Leys
 
C
,
Delacre
 
M
,
Mora
 
YL
,
Lakens
 
D
,
Ley
 
C.
 
How to classify, detect, and manage univariate and multivariate outliers, with emphasis on pre-registration
.
2019
;
32
:
5
. https://doi.org/

53.

Epskamp
 
S
,
Fried
 
EI.
 
A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks
.
Psychol Methods.
 
2018
;
23
:
617
634
. https://doi.org/

54.

van Borkulo
 
CD
,
van Bork
 
R
,
Boschloo
 
L
, et al.  
Comparing network structures on three aspects: a permutation test
.
Psychol Methods.
 
2023
;
28
:
1273
1285
. https://doi.org/

55.

Diao
 
M
,
Demchenko
 
I
,
Asare
 
G
,
Quan
 
J
,
Debruille
 
JB.
 
Finding normal-to-better neurocognitive indexes in individuals with schizotypal traits using a social role task
.
Schizophrenia.
 
2023
;
9
:
1
10
. https://doi.org/

56.

Scutari
 
M.
 
Learning Bayesian networks with the bnlearn R package
.
J Stat Softw.
 
2010
;
35
:
1
22
. https://doi.org/

57.

Sachs
 
K
,
Perez
 
O
,
Pe’er
 
D
,
Lauffenburger
 
DA
,
Nolan
 
GP.
 
Causal protein-signaling networks derived from multiparameter single-cell data
.
Science.
 
2005
;
308
:
523
529
. https://doi.org/

58.

Hori
 
H
,
Nagamine
 
M
,
Soshi
 
T
,
Okabe
 
S
,
Kim
 
Y
,
Kunugi
 
H.
 
Schizotypal traits in healthy women predict prefrontal activation patterns during a verbal fluency task: a near-infrared spectroscopy study
.
Neuropsychobiology.
 
2008
;
57
:
61
69
. https://doi.org/

59.

Briganti
 
G
,
Scutari
 
M
,
McNally
 
RJ.
 
A tutorial on Bayesian networks for psychopathology researchers
.
Psychol Methods.
 
2023
;
28
:
947
961
. https://doi.org/

60.

McNally
 
RJ
,
Mair
 
P
,
Mugno
 
BL
,
Riemann
 
BC.
 
Co-morbid obsessive-compulsive disorder and depression: a Bayesian network approach
.
Psychol Med.
 
2017
;
47
:
1204
1214
. https://doi.org/

61.

Scutari
 
M
,
Denis
 
JB.
 
Bayesian Networks: With Examples in R
. 1st ed.
Chapman and Hall/CRC
;
2014
.

62.

Rohrer
 
JM.
 
Thinking clearly about correlations and causation: graphical causal models for observational data
.
Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci.
 
2018
;
1
:
27
42
. https://doi.org/

63.

Crouse
 
JJ
,
Ho
 
N
,
Scott
 
J
, et al.  
Dynamic networks of psychological symptoms, impairment, substance use, and social support: the evolution of psychopathology among emerging adults
.
Eur Psychiatry.
 
2022
;
65
:
e32
. https://doi.org/

64.

Abplanalp
 
SJ
,
Braff
 
DL
,
Light
 
GA
,
Joshi
 
YB
,
Nuechterlein
 
KH
,
Green
 
MF
;
Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia
.
Clarifying directional dependence among measures of early auditory processing and cognition in schizophrenia: leveraging Gaussian graphical models and Bayesian networks
.
Psychol Med.
 
2024
;
54
:
1930
1939
. https://doi.org/

65.

Kovacs
 
M
,
Lopez-Duran
 
N.
 
Prodromal symptoms and atypical affectivity as predictors of major depression in juveniles: implications for prevention
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry.
 
2010
;
51
:
472
496
. https://doi.org/

66.

Lin
 
A
,
Wigman
 
JTW
,
Nelson
 
B
, et al.  
Follow-up factor structure of schizotypy and its clinical associations in a help-seeking sample meeting ultra-high risk for psychosis criteria at baseline
.
Compr Psychiatry.
 
2013
;
54
:
173
180
. https://doi.org/

67.

Neus
 
BV
,
Phillip
 
G
,
Kwapil
 
TR.
 
The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders
.
Schizophr Bull.
 
2015
;
41
(
suppl 2
):
S408
S416
. https://doi.org/

68.

Aldao
 
A
,
Nolen-Hoeksema
 
S
,
Schweizer
 
S.
 
Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review
.
Clin Psychol Rev.
 
2010
;
30
:
217
237
. https://doi.org/

69.

Moore
 
SA
,
Zoellner
 
LA
,
Mollenholt
 
N.
 
Are expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms
?
Behav Res Ther.
 
2008
;
46
:
993
1000
. https://doi.org/

70.

Garnefski
 
N
,
Kraaij
 
V.
 
Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: a comparative study of five specific samples
.
Pers Individ Differ.
 
2006
;
40
:
1659
1669
. https://doi.org/

71.

Luna
 
B
,
Padmanabhan
 
A
,
O’Hearn
 
K.
 
What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence
?
Brain Cogn.
 
2010
;
72
:
101
113
. https://doi.org/

72.

Silvers
 
JA
,
McRae
 
K
,
Gabrieli
 
JDE
,
Gross
 
JJ
,
Remy
 
KA
,
Ochsner
 
KN.
 
Age-related differences in emotional reactivity, regulation, and rejection sensitivity in adolescence
.
Emotion.
 
2012
;
12
:
1235
1247
. https://doi.org/

73.

Hallquist
 
MN
,
Wright
 
AGC
,
Molenaar
 
PCM.
 
Problems with centrality measures in psychopathology symptom networks: why network psychometrics cannot escape psychometric theory
.
Multivar Behav Res.
 
2019
;
56
:
199
223
. https://doi.org/

74.

Castro
 
D
,
Ferreira
 
F
,
de Castro
 
I
, et al.  
The differential role of central and bridge symptoms in deactivating psychopathological networks
.
Front Psychol.
 
2019
;
10
:
2448
. Accessed
January 12, 2024
. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02448

75.

Rodebaugh
 
TL
,
Tonge
 
NA
,
Piccirillo
 
ML
, et al.  
Does centrality in a cross-sectional network suggest intervention targets for social anxiety disorder
?
J Consult Clin Psychol.
 
2018
;
86
:
831
844
. https://doi.org/

76.

Bringmann
 
LF
,
Elmer
 
T
,
Epskamp
 
S
, et al.  
What do centrality measures measure in psychological networks
?
J Abnorm Psychol.
 
2019
;
128
:
892
903
. https://doi.org/

77.

Nolen-Hoeksema
 
S.
 
Emotion regulation and psychopathology: the role of gender
.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol.
 
2012
;
8
:
161
187
. https://doi.org/

78.

Bender
 
PK
,
Reinholdt-Dunne
 
ML
,
Esbjørn
 
BH
,
Pons
 
F.
 
Emotion dysregulation and anxiety in children and adolescents: gender differences
.
Pers Individ Differ.
 
2012
;
53
:
284
288
. https://doi.org/

79.

Chervonsky
 
E
,
Hunt
 
C.
 
Emotion regulation, mental health, and social wellbeing in a young adolescent sample: a concurrent and longitudinal investigation
.
Emotion.
 
2019
;
19
:
270
282
. https://doi.org/

80.

Ericson
 
M
,
Tuvblad
 
C
,
Raine
 
A
,
Young-Wolff
 
K
,
Baker
 
LA.
 
Heritability and longitudinal stability of schizotypal traits during adolescence
.
Behav Genet.
 
2011
;
41
:
499
511
. https://doi.org/

81.

Gullone
 
E
,
Hughes
 
EK
,
King
 
NJ
,
Tonge
 
B.
 
The normative development of emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: a 2-year follow-up study
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry.
 
2010
;
51
:
567
574
. https://doi.org/

82.

Keenan
 
K
,
Hipwell
 
AE.
 
Preadolescent clues to understanding depression in girls
.
Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev.
 
2005
;
8
:
89
105
. https://doi.org/

83.

Wang
 
Y
,
Shi
 
H
,
Liu
 
W
, et al.  
Applying network analysis to investigate the links between dimensional schizotypy and cognitive and affective empathy
.
J Affect Disord.
 
2020
;
277
:
313
321
. https://doi.org/

84.

Thomas
 
EHX
,
Rossell
 
SL
,
Gurvich
 
C.
 
Gender differences in the correlations between childhood trauma, schizotypy and negative emotions in non-clinical individuals
.
Brain Sci.
 
2022
;
12
:
186
. https://doi.org/

85.

Huang
 
Y
,
Hu
 
H
,
Wang
 
L
, et al.  
Relationships between childhood trauma and dimensional schizotypy: a network analysis and replication
.
Asian J Psychiatry.
 
2023
;
85
:
103598
. https://doi.org/

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.