Abstract

Empathy for social pain encompasses both affective and cognitive responses to others’ emotional reactions following negative social encounters, facilitating an understanding of their suffering and promoting prosocial behaviors. This study examined how a scarcity mindset affects empathy for social pain and prosocial intentions at behavioral and neural levels. Sixty participants were randomly assigned to either the scarcity or abundance mindset group. They viewed images of social exclusion or neutral scenarios and subsequently rated the perceived unpleasantness of the target person and their willingness to provide comfort during a stage-game paradigm. The results showed that participants in the scarcity mindset group demonstrated greater differentiation in their ratings of unpleasantness and willingness to comfort when exposed to social exclusion images compared to neutral ones, relative to the abundance mindset group. Electrophysiological data revealed that social exclusion images elicited larger late positive potential (LPP) amplitudes in the scarcity mindset group, but not in the abundance mindset group. Additionally, within the scarcity mindset group, affective empathy trait scores moderated the relationship between LPP amplitudes and willingness to comfort ratings. These findings highlight the amplifying effects of a scarcity mindset on empathy for social pain and prosocial intentions, and emphasize the role of affective empathy traits in this dynamic process.

Introduction

Empathy, often observed in response to negative physical or social experiences of others (Meyer et al. 2013, Wei et al. 2023), is essential for fostering social connections (Singer and Klimecki 2014). It is a multifaceted concept (Cuff et al. 2016) that comprises three key components (Zaki and Ochsner 2012): affective empathy, which involves sharing and indirectly experiencing the emotional states of others (Gallese et al. 2004, Decety and Cowell 2014, Marsh 2018); cognitive empathy, the ability to accurately understand how individuals perceive and experience their situations (Leslie et al. 2004, Decety and Cowell 2014, Marsh 2018, Blanke and Riediger 2019); and prosocial disposition, which reflects the motivation to support the well-being of others or to help alleviate their suffering (Zaki and Ochsner 2012).

Empathy encompasses the ability to understand and share not only others’ primary emotions (Krach et al. 2015), such as physical pain (Singer et al. 2004), but also more complex social emotions including social rejection and embarrassment (Krach et al. 2011, Paulus et al. 2015). Social pain, a specific subtype of emotional pain (Levi et al. 2023), is characterized by intense emotional distress triggered by adverse social experiences, such as social exclusion (Macdonald and Leary 2005). Witnessing instances of social exclusion can lead individuals to vicariously experience associated emotional pain, a phenomenon known as empathy for social pain. This allows them to emotionally resonate with and cognitively comprehend the distress of those who are socially rejected (Atkins et al. 2016).

Empathy is widely recognized as a catalyst for prosocial behavior (Decety et al. 2016). It enhances individuals’ ability to recognize the needs of others, motivating them to engage in prosocial behaviors and respond more readily to requests for assistance (Lockwood et al. 2016, Pang et al. 2022). The empathy-altruism hypothesis posits that empathy for someone in need or distress generates altruistic motivation, which subsequently fosters the desire to engage in prosocial behaviors (Batson et al. 1988, 2015). Supporting empirical studies have consistently demonstrated that feeling empathy significantly enhances individuals’ likelihood of engaging in prosocial behaviors directed toward the target of their empathy (Batson et al. 1981, 1999, 2002). When individuals empathize with the social pain of others, they are more likely to engage in various prosocial behaviors toward ostracized victims, such as sympathy, assistance, and comfort (Masten et al. 2010, 2011, Wesselmann et al. 2013). Extensive research has examined the impact of various social factors on empathic responses to others’ physical pain, as well as the underlying neural mechanisms involved. For instance, specific social contexts can influence neural responses when observing others’ pain. A threatening context enhances early neural reactions to others’ suffering, whereas a friendly context promotes later empathic processing (Cui et al. 2017). Additionally, empathic responses to the suffering of others are affected by one’s relative position in the social hierarchy; individuals tend to exhibit greater empathy toward those of inferior status than toward those of superior status (Feng et al. 2016). Moreover, observing a situation that may be painful for others can elicit feelings of empathy, even when the individuals involved are different from the observers and the situation does not cause any pain to the observers themselves (Perry et al. 2010). Furthermore, beliefs about others’ pain can modulate empathic responses, with empathy being diminished in the absence of such beliefs (Wu and Han 2021). However, research on empathy for social pain across different social contexts is relatively new and limited.

Resource scarcity poses a significant challenge globally, affecting not only the economically disadvantaged but also others. A scarcity mindset is often triggered by feelings of insufficient resources (Shah et al. 2012, Huijsmans et al. 2019). This mindset fosters a self-centered bias (Roux et al. 2015), leading individuals who perceive themselves as having inadequate resources to exhibit reduced empathy toward others’ physical pain (Li et al. 2023) and diminished prosocial intentions (Aarøe and Petersen 2013, Cui et al. 2022, 2023, Hasson et al. 2022, Agneman et al. 2023). However, some authors have reached contrasting conclusions, suggesting that a scarcity mindset can foster other-oriented behaviors (Ye and Mattila 2023), leading to enhanced empathic accuracy (Kraus et al. 2010) and increased prosocial intentions (Piff et al. 2010, Louie and Rieta 2018, Goldsmith et al. 2020, Laforet et al. 2024). These inconsistent findings regarding the impact of a scarcity mindset on prosocial intentions may arise from its varying effects on empathy for physical pain compared to empathy for social pain, given the crucial role that empathy plays in motivating prosocial behaviors.

In the context of resource scarcity, individuals may become acutely aware of their limited resources, often neglecting the physiological needs of others and hesitating to share what little they have. However, alternative perspectives suggest that those facing resource scarcity may seek security in other areas (Cannon et al. 2019), such as by developing a greater preference for cooperation to forge social connections in the face of perceived socio-psychological resource scarcity (Civai et al. 2022). Consequently, they may remain attuned to others’ social and emotional cues. For example, individuals from lower social classes, who possess fewer resources, tend to absorb more contextual information when observing others’ emotions (Kraus et al. 2009). This heightened sensitivity can lead to more accurate judgments of others’ social emotions and greater empathy compared to their upper-class counterparts (Kraus et al. 2010, 2012).

Empathy for social pain is likely to be a common and salient experience (Masten et al. 2010), given the inevitability of encountering social pain in interpersonal relationships and its enduring effects (Chen et al. 2008). Exploring how a scarcity mindset influences empathy for social pain and related prosocial intentions, such as comforting others, can enhance our understanding of individuals’ social behaviors during times of resource scarcity. Furthermore, this study provides insights into whether the effects of a scarcity mindset on empathy for social pain differ from previous findings related to empathy for physical pain.

In recent years, an increasing number of researchers have focused on temporal neural processing of empathy for social pain. Specifically, event-related potential (ERP) studies on adult college students have revealed a pronounced late positive potential (LPP) component during the observation of social exclusion images, indicating heightened emotional responses to social exclusion stimuli (He et al. 2020, Zhao et al. 2021). Additionally, other studies have shown that empathy for social pain in participants aged 18–50 years elicits both the early N2 component over fronto-central electrodes, which is associated with sharing others’ emotions, and the late P3 component over centro-parietal electrodes, which reflects prolonged attentional processing and cognitive appraisal of stimuli with motivational significance (Flasbeck and Brüne 2019, Wang et al. 2021, Rodriguez et al. 2023). A review of electrophysiological studies concluded that observing social exclusion is associated with N2, P3, and late slow-wave components, particularly in studies focusing on adults (Mills et al. 2024). Similar to empathy for physical pain (Fan and Han 2008), empathy for social pain involves both early bottom-up affective components, which reflect automatic emotional resonance with the emotional states of others, and later top-down processes related to attentional engagement and cognitive resource allocation. Building on previous research (Li et al. 2023), we hypothesized that a scarcity mindset may influence both the early bottom-up emotional resonance and the late top-down attentional processes involved in empathy for social pain.

To induce a scarcity mindset in a controlled laboratory setting, we employed a stage-game paradigm to manipulate scarcity and abundance mindsets (Huijsmans et al. 2019). In the scarcity condition, participants consistently faced the risk of not being able to advance, triggering a scarcity mindset. In contrast, participants in the abundance condition were able to proceed without stress, fostering an abundance mindset. This paradigm has been shown to effectively induce sustained feelings of scarcity (Huijsmans et al. 2019, Li et al. 2023). It was seamlessly integrated with the main task, thereby minimizing the likelihood of participant suspicion. Furthermore, the stimulus materials consisted of images depicting college students in various authentic campus social exclusion or neutral situations (Zheng et al. 2022). In this study, two distinct groups of participants viewed picture stimuli while experiencing either scarcity or abundance mindsets. We measured unpleasantness ratings and prosocial intention (i.e. willingness to comfort) ratings for the stimuli under these different conditions.

The present study aimed to investigate the effects of scarcity mindset on both behavioral and neural responses when individuals observe someone experiencing social exclusion. Building on prior literature suggesting that individuals facing resource scarcity exhibit heightened sensitivity to others’ social emotions (Kraus et al. 2010), we hypothesized that, unlike inhibiting empathy for physical pain, a scarcity mindset might amplify empathy for social pain and promote prosocial intentions. Specifically, at the behavioral level, we anticipated that participants in a scarcity mindset would report higher ratings of unpleasantness and a greater willingness to offer comfort when witnessing individuals subjected to social exclusion (as opposed to neutral) scenarios compared to those in an abundance mindset. At the neural level, we expected that the amplitudes of the early N2, as well as the later P3 and LPP components elicited by social exclusion (versus neutral) stimuli, would be greater under a scarcity mindset than under an abundance mindset.

Materials and methods

Participants

The sample size was determined using G*Power 3.1.9 software [Faul et al. 2009; F-test, repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA), within-between interaction]. We conducted a priori power analysis based on a significant interaction effect on the LPP amplitudes observed in a previous ERP study (ηp2 = 0.08; Li et al. 2023). The analysis indicated that the sample of 28 participants provided a statistical power of 95%. To further enhance the statistical power, we recruited 30 participants per group, resulting in a total of 60 participants (see Table 1). All participants were right-handed adults aged 18–25 years with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The exclusion criteria included a history of neurological disorders, brain injuries, developmental disabilities, psychiatric disorders, or current use of psychoactive medications. These criteria were assessed using self-reporting questionnaires and interviews. Written informed consent was obtained from all the participants. The research protocol was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of Shenzhen University, in accordance with all provisions outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Table 1.

Demographic and psychometric variables for two groups of participants (mean ± SD)

 Scarcity groupAbundance groupP
Demographic variables
Age (years)20.83 ± 2.0720.87 ± 2.15.951
Female/male15/1515/15-
Psychometric variables
Cognitive empathy55.00 ± 8.1255.03 ± 6.94.986
Affective empathy32.70 ± 5.0731.77 ± 4.46.452
Trait anxiety40.80 ± 7.6840.13 ± 8.77.755
State anxiety36.27 ± 6.9436.47 ± 9.55.926
Social anxiety47.70 ± 19.5952.50 ± 20.74.361
 Scarcity groupAbundance groupP
Demographic variables
Age (years)20.83 ± 2.0720.87 ± 2.15.951
Female/male15/1515/15-
Psychometric variables
Cognitive empathy55.00 ± 8.1255.03 ± 6.94.986
Affective empathy32.70 ± 5.0731.77 ± 4.46.452
Trait anxiety40.80 ± 7.6840.13 ± 8.77.755
State anxiety36.27 ± 6.9436.47 ± 9.55.926
Social anxiety47.70 ± 19.5952.50 ± 20.74.361

Statistics were obtained using independent-sample t-tests.

Table 1.

Demographic and psychometric variables for two groups of participants (mean ± SD)

 Scarcity groupAbundance groupP
Demographic variables
Age (years)20.83 ± 2.0720.87 ± 2.15.951
Female/male15/1515/15-
Psychometric variables
Cognitive empathy55.00 ± 8.1255.03 ± 6.94.986
Affective empathy32.70 ± 5.0731.77 ± 4.46.452
Trait anxiety40.80 ± 7.6840.13 ± 8.77.755
State anxiety36.27 ± 6.9436.47 ± 9.55.926
Social anxiety47.70 ± 19.5952.50 ± 20.74.361
 Scarcity groupAbundance groupP
Demographic variables
Age (years)20.83 ± 2.0720.87 ± 2.15.951
Female/male15/1515/15-
Psychometric variables
Cognitive empathy55.00 ± 8.1255.03 ± 6.94.986
Affective empathy32.70 ± 5.0731.77 ± 4.46.452
Trait anxiety40.80 ± 7.6840.13 ± 8.77.755
State anxiety36.27 ± 6.9436.47 ± 9.55.926
Social anxiety47.70 ± 19.5952.50 ± 20.74.361

Statistics were obtained using independent-sample t-tests.

Before the study, participants were asked to complete several assessments, including the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al. 1983), Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (Liebowitz 1987), and Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy (Reniers et al. 2011). The levels of anxiety and empathy were comparable between the two groups. The participants completed these assessments prior to the main task, with a brief interval between the completion of these measurements and the start of the electroencephalography (EEG) experiment.

Stimuli

The images used in this study were sourced from the publicly accessible Image Database of Social Inclusion/Exclusion in Young Asian Adults (ISIEA; Zheng et al. 2022). Previous research has demonstrated that the ISIEA effectively presents negative social situations that evoke feelings of social distress in younger adults (Zheng et al. 2022, He et al. 2023, Mo et al. 2023, Yu et al. 2023). For this study, we selected 35 social exclusion images and 35 socially neutral images from the database, as illustrated in Fig. 1a. The social exclusion images depict scenarios in which one individual (the rejectee) displays sad or upset facial and body expressions, whereas three or four rejecters engage in conversation and laughter. In contrast, socially neutral images portray scenes featuring two to five individuals exhibiting neutral expressions, without any social interaction or communication. According to the ratings of physical parameters from the ISIEA (Zheng et al. 2022), significant differences were observed between the social exclusion and neutral images across various dimensions, including valence (3.33 ± 0.48 vs 5.08 ± 0.31, P < .001), arousal (4.61 ± 0.19 vs 4.22 ± 0.16, P < .001), inclusion score (3.22 ± 0.53 vs 5.31 ± 0.28, P < .001), and vicarious feeling (3.14 ± 0.36 vs 4.98 ± 0.30, P < .001). Each stimulus subtended a visual angle of 7.5°× 5.0° (width× height) with a viewing distance of 80 cm.

Experimental design. (a) Examples of social exclusion (left) and social neutral (right) pictures. (b) Illustrations of three stage-games. (c) A flowchart depicting the stage-game paradigm. The red dashed lines indicate the threshold, while the black lines represent the variation in the number of tokens around the threshold for each game under scarcity (top) and abundance (bottom) mindsets. The gray shaded areas highlight the main task stages. (d) A flowchart illustrating the main task. ERP responses were time-locked to the segment marked with a dashed-line box.
Figure 1.

Experimental design. (a) Examples of social exclusion (left) and social neutral (right) pictures. (b) Illustrations of three stage-games. (c) A flowchart depicting the stage-game paradigm. The red dashed lines indicate the threshold, while the black lines represent the variation in the number of tokens around the threshold for each game under scarcity (top) and abundance (bottom) mindsets. The gray shaded areas highlight the main task stages. (d) A flowchart illustrating the main task. ERP responses were time-locked to the segment marked with a dashed-line box.

Design and procedure

The present study utilized a 2 (Group: scarcity mindset, abundance mindset) × 2 (Picture: social exclusion, social neutral) mixed design. Each participant was randomly assigned to one of the mindset groups (scarcity or abundance), with allocation counterbalanced across participants. The experiment was conducted in a quiet, temperature-controlled room. Participants were instructed to focus on the experimental task displayed on a 24-inch computer screen using E-Prime 3.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA). The EEG data were continuously recorded throughout the main task.

Scarcity and abundance mindsets were induced using an experimental manipulation known as three-stage games (Huijsmans et al. 2019). Each stage-game (dot comparison, shape matching, or dot counting; see Fig. 1b) consisted of a block of 90 trials. In this paradigm, the initial number of tokens was manipulated within the game, ensuring an equal number of wins and losses across identical stage games under both mindset conditions. To induce a scarcity mindset, participants were provided with one initial token, and the token count remained at approximately one throughout the study. In contrast, for the abundance mindset manipulation, participants received 10 initial tokens, with the token count fluctuating around 10 during the study (see Fig. 1c). Feedback on the outcome of each keypress was provided, and the current token count was displayed at the end of each stage. All participants were informed that they needed to have at least one token at the end of each game to receive a reward of 100 RMB; otherwise, they would only receive compensation of 10 RMB. A more detailed description of each stage-game can be found in Supplementary material S1.

The main task, including social exclusion stimuli, was intertwined with stage-games. During the interval of each stage-game, the participants engaged in the main task. In each trial of the main task (see Fig. 1d), a fixation cross was displayed on a black screen for 500 ms, followed by a random interval ranging from 500 to 1000 ms. Subsequently, either a social exclusion or neutral picture was presented for 6000 ms, during which participants were instructed to observe attentively. After a random interval of 200–500 ms, a 6-point numerical rating scale appeared. Participants were instructed to respond by pressing a key corresponding to a value from 0 to 5 to evaluate either “The level of unpleasantness of the target person, i.e. the one marked with a red circle (0 = no unpleasantness, 5 = enormous unpleasantness)” or “The level of your willingness to comfort the target person (0 = no willingness, 5 = enormous willingness).” The inter-trial interval varied randomly between 1000 and 2000 ms. Each block comprised 47, 46, and 47 trials, respectively.

Furthermore, to assess the effectiveness of scarcity and abundance mindset induction, participants were asked to provide subjective ratings of their stress, confidence, motivation, and excitement using 9-point Likert scales (see Supplementary material S2). Higher scores indicate greater levels of each rating. These assessments were collected at various time points, including the pre-experiment phase (baseline) and after the completion of each stage-game (posttest: T1, T2, T3).

Electroencephalography data recording

EEG data were recorded from 64 scalp sites using tin electrodes mounted on an actiCHamp system (Brain Vision LLC, Morrisville, NC, USA; passband: 0.01–100 Hz; sampling rate: 1000 Hz). The FCz electrode was used as a recording reference, and the electrode on the medial frontal aspect was used as the ground electrode. All the electrode impedances remained below 5 kΩ.

Electroencephalography data analysis

EEG data were preprocessed offline and analyzed using MATLAB R2019 (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) in conjunction with the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig 2004). Continuous EEG signals underwent band-pass filtering within the 0.1–30 Hz range. The signals were then segmented, starting 500 ms before stimulus onset and extending for 3000 ms, resulting in a total analysis window of 3500 ms. Baseline correction was applied using a 500-ms prestimulus time window. Epochs with amplitude values exceeding ±100 μV at any electrode were excluded from averaging. Additionally, EEG epochs were visually inspected, and trials containing significant noise due to gross movements were removed, accounting for 4.71% of the total number of epochs. Electrooculographic artifacts were corrected using an independent component analysis (ICA) algorithm (Jung et al. 2001). Following ICA and additional baseline correction, EEG trials were re-referenced to the bilateral mastoid electrodes.

For each participant, single-trial ERP waveforms elicited by social exclusion and social neutral pictures were averaged separately for scarcity and abundance mindset conditions. This process yielded four average waveforms per participant, each time-locked to the onset of the pictures for the respective mindset conditions. To obtain group-level ERP waveforms, single-participant average waveforms were averaged across all participants. Group-level scalp topographies were computed using spline interpolation. Based on the topographical distribution of the grand-averaged ERP activity and prior research (Zhao et al. 2021, Li et al. 2023), N2 amplitudes were measured at fronto-central electrodes (F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2) within the time window of 215–255 ms; P3 amplitudes were measured at parietal electrodes (CP3, CPz, CP4, P3, Pz, P4, PO3, POz, PO4) within the time window of 350–390 ms; and LPP amplitudes were measured at centro-parietal electrodes (C3, Cz, C4, CP3, CPz, CP4) within the time window of 500–3000 ms.

Statistical analysis

For behavioral data, statistical analysis was conducted using the lmerTest package (Version 3.1.3; Kuznetsova et al., 2017) in R (Version 4.3.2). Linear mixed models (LMMs) were employed to evaluate ratings of unpleasantness and willingness to comfort. Group (scarcity mindset, abundance mindset) and picture types (social exclusion, social neutral) were defined as fixed effects, while participants and random intercepts were included as random effects to account for variability between participants. The effects of group and picture type on unpleasantness ratings and willingness to comfort were assessed separately. Descriptive statistics are presented as mean ± standard error (SE). Simple effect analyses were performed for any significant interaction. Statistical differences were considered significant at P < .05, with P-values corrected using the false discovery rate procedure.

For neural data and subjective ratings, statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 22 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Descriptive statistics are presented as mean ± SE. Neural responses (ERP components) were analyzed using two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs, including the between-participant factor of Group (scarcity mindset, abundance mindset) and the within-participant factor of Picture (social exclusion, social neutral). Differential subjective ratings (posttest–baseline) were analyzed using two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs, with Group (scarcity mindset, abundance mindset) and Timepoint (T1, T2, T3) as factors. In cases of significant interactions, post hoc pairwise comparisons were conducted, and significance levels (P-values) were Bonferroni-corrected at P < .05. The degrees of freedom for the F-ratios were adjusted using the Greenhouse–Geisser method.

To examine whether empathy traits moderated the relationship between neural and behavioral responses, a moderating effect analysis was conducted using the PROCESS macro in SPSS (Hayes 2012). In these moderating models, the independent variable (X) was the neural response (ERP amplitude), the dependent variable (Y) was the behavioral response (ratings of unpleasantness and willingness to comfort), and the moderator variable (Mo) was the score of empathy traits (affective and cognitive empathy). The conditioning values were set at −1SD, Mean, and +1SD in the PROCESS. If a significant moderating effect was identified, simple slope tests were performed to explore the nature of moderation further. Statistical significance was set at P < .05.

Results

Subjective ratings

For subjective ratings of stress, significant main effects were observed for Group [F(1,58) = 10.97, P = .002, ηp2 = 0.16] and Timepoint [F(2,57) = 7.04, P = .002, ηp2 = 0.20]. Participants reported higher stress ratings under the scarcity mindset compared to the abundance mindset (0.81 ± 0.25 vs −0.36 ± 0.25). Additionally, stress ratings were significantly higher at T1 than at T2 (0.55 ± 0.18 vs 0.08 ± 0.19, P = .002) and T3 (0.55 ± 0.18 vs 0.05 ± 0.20, P = .005). The Group × Timepoint interaction was also significant [F(2,57) = 3.36, P = .042, ηp2 = 0.11]. Post hoc comparisons revealed that stress ratings at all three timepoints were significantly higher under the scarcity mindset compared to the abundance mindset (T1: 1.23 ± 0.25 vs −0.13 ± 0.25, P < .001; T2: 0.50 ± 0.27 vs −0.33 ± 0.27, P = .035; T3: 0.70 ± 0.29 vs −0.60 ± 0.29, P = .002) (see Fig. 2a).

Subjective ratings and behavioral results. (a) Differential ratings of subjective stress under scarcity and abundance mindsets following each stage of the game. (b) Ratings of unpleasantness (left) and willingness to comfort (right) in response to social exclusion and neutral pictures under both scarcity and abundance mindsets. Data are presented as mean ± SE. *P < .05; ** P < .01; *** P < .001.
Figure 2.

Subjective ratings and behavioral results. (a) Differential ratings of subjective stress under scarcity and abundance mindsets following each stage of the game. (b) Ratings of unpleasantness (left) and willingness to comfort (right) in response to social exclusion and neutral pictures under both scarcity and abundance mindsets. Data are presented as mean ± SE. *P < .05; ** P < .01; *** P < .001.

No significant differences were found in the dimensions of confidence (P = .364), motivation (P = .115), or excitement (P = .100) between the scarcity and abundance mindset groups.

Behavioral results

The effect size within the two LMMs was assessed using the MuMIn package (Bartoń 2023). Cohen’s f2 was calculated to quantify the relative explanatory power of fixed effects.

Unpleasantness

The Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) for the LMM was 12892.26. The marginal R2 was calculated as 0.493, indicating that the fixed effects accounted for approximately 49.3% of the variance in the response variable. Consequently, the derived Cohen’s f2 value was approximately 0.973, suggesting a large effect size according to Cohen’s guidelines (Cohen 2013). Detailed results of the fixed effects are presented in Table 2. A significant main effect of Picture was observed, with higher unpleasantness ratings for social exclusion pictures compared to socially neutral pictures (3.56 ± 0.08 vs 1.14 ± 0.08, P < .001). Additionally, the Group × Picture interaction was significant. Simple effects analysis revealed that unpleasantness ratings for social exclusion pictures were significantly higher than those for socially neutral pictures under both mindsets (scarcity: 3.55 ± 0.11 vs 1.04 ± 0.11, P < .001; abundance: 3.58 ± 0.11 vs 1.25 ± 0.11, P < .001). However, the difference in ratings (social exclusion−social neutral) was significantly greater under the scarcity mindset compared to the abundance mindset (2.50 ± 0.05 vs 2.34 ± 0.05, P = .014) (see Fig. 2b).

Table 2.

The detailed results for all fixed effects in two LMMs

Fixed effectsEst.SEtP
Unpleasantness
Intercept5.920.1346.36<.001***
Group0.130.180.71.481
Picture−2.340.05−49.07<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.46.014*
Willingness to comfort
Intercept5.160.1828.89<.001***
Group0.280.251.10.274
Picture−1.930.05−39.22<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.38.017*
Fixed effectsEst.SEtP
Unpleasantness
Intercept5.920.1346.36<.001***
Group0.130.180.71.481
Picture−2.340.05−49.07<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.46.014*
Willingness to comfort
Intercept5.160.1828.89<.001***
Group0.280.251.10.274
Picture−1.930.05−39.22<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.38.017*

*P < .05;

***P < .001.

Table 2.

The detailed results for all fixed effects in two LMMs

Fixed effectsEst.SEtP
Unpleasantness
Intercept5.920.1346.36<.001***
Group0.130.180.71.481
Picture−2.340.05−49.07<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.46.014*
Willingness to comfort
Intercept5.160.1828.89<.001***
Group0.280.251.10.274
Picture−1.930.05−39.22<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.38.017*
Fixed effectsEst.SEtP
Unpleasantness
Intercept5.920.1346.36<.001***
Group0.130.180.71.481
Picture−2.340.05−49.07<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.46.014*
Willingness to comfort
Intercept5.160.1828.89<.001***
Group0.280.251.10.274
Picture−1.930.05−39.22<.001***
Group × Picture−0.170.07−2.38.017*

*P < .05;

***P < .001.

Regarding the random effects structure, the variance of the random intercepts for participants was estimated at 0.32 (SD = 0.57), indicating a significant variation in the baseline levels of the response variable among participants. The residual variance, which accounts for the unexplained variability within participants, was estimated to be 1.19 (SD = 1.10).

Willingness to comfort

The BIC for the LMM was 13 201.42. The marginal R2 was calculated as 0.331, indicating that the fixed effects accounted for approximately 33.1% of the variance in the response variable. Consequently, the derived Cohen’s f2 value was approximately 0.496, suggesting a large effect size. Detailed results of the fixed effects are presented in Table 2. A significant main effect of Picture was observed, with higher willingness to comfort ratings for social exclusion pictures compared to socially neutral pictures (3.29 ± 0.12 vs 1.28 ± 0.12, P < .001). The Group × Picture interaction was also significant. Simple effects analysis revealed that willingness to comfort ratings were significantly higher for social exclusion pictures than for socially neutral pictures under both mindsets (scarcity: 3.34 ± 0.16 vs 1.25 ± 0.16, P < .001; abundance: 3.23 ± 0.16 vs 1.30 ± 0.16, P < .001). Furthermore, the difference in ratings (social exclusion−social neutral) was significantly greater under the scarcity mindset compared to the abundance mindset (2.09 ± 0.05 vs 1.93 ± 0.05, P = .017) (see Fig. 2b).

Regarding the random effects structure, the variance of the random intercepts for participants was estimated to be 0.78 (SD = 0.88), indicating a significant variation in the baseline levels of the response variable among participants. The residual variance, which reflects the unexplained variability within participants, was estimated at 1.27 (SD = 1.13).

Event-related potential results

N2

A significant main effect of Group was observed [F(1,58) = 13.19, P = .001, ηp2 = 0.19], indicating that smaller N2 amplitudes were elicited under the scarcity mindset compared to the abundance mindset (−6.76 ± 0.87 μV vs −11.24 ± 0.87 μV). Additionally, a significant main effect of Picture was found [F(1,58) = 5.20, P = .026, ηp2 = 0.08] with smaller N2 amplitudes elicited by social exclusion pictures compared to neutral pictures (−8.72 ± 0.63 μV vs −9.27 ± 0.63 μV). The interaction effect was not significant [F(1,58) = 0.18, P = .675, ηp2 < 0.01] (see Fig. 3a and c).

ERP results. (a) Grand-averaged ERP waveforms recorded at fronto-central electrodes in response to social exclusion and neutral pictures under scarcity and abundance mindsets. Scalp topographies illustrate the distributions of N2 amplitudes for each condition. (b) Grand-averaged ERP waveforms obtained from centro-parietal electrodes in response to social exclusion and neutral pictures under scarcity and abundance mindsets. Scalp topographies depict the distributions of P3 and LPP amplitudes for each condition. (c) Bar charts displaying the N2, P3, and LPP components. Significant differences in ERP waveforms are indicated by gray shaded squares. Electrodes used for estimating ERP amplitudes are marked with white points on their respective scalp maps. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. n.s.: P > .05; *** P < .001.
Figure 3.

ERP results. (a) Grand-averaged ERP waveforms recorded at fronto-central electrodes in response to social exclusion and neutral pictures under scarcity and abundance mindsets. Scalp topographies illustrate the distributions of N2 amplitudes for each condition. (b) Grand-averaged ERP waveforms obtained from centro-parietal electrodes in response to social exclusion and neutral pictures under scarcity and abundance mindsets. Scalp topographies depict the distributions of P3 and LPP amplitudes for each condition. (c) Bar charts displaying the N2, P3, and LPP components. Significant differences in ERP waveforms are indicated by gray shaded squares. Electrodes used for estimating ERP amplitudes are marked with white points on their respective scalp maps. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. n.s.: P > .05; *** P < .001.

P3

A significant main effect of Group was found [F(1,58) = 5.90, P = .018, ηp2 = 0.09], indicating that greater P3 amplitudes were elicited under the scarcity mindset compared to the abundance mindset (7.36 ± 0.76 μV vs 4.76 ± 0.76 μV). Additionally, a significant main effect of Picture was observed [F(1,58) = 27.61, P < .001, ηp2 = 0.32] with greater P3 amplitudes elicited by social exclusion pictures compared to neutral pictures (6.58 ± 0.57 μV vs 5.54 ± 0.52 μV). The interaction effect was not significant [F(1,58) = 0.10, P = .756, ηp2 < 0.01] (see Fig. 3b and c).

Late positive potential

A significant main effect of Group was found [F(1,58) = 7.63, P = .008, ηp2 = 0.12] indicating that greater LPP amplitudes were elicited under the scarcity mindset compared to the abundance mindset (3.22 ± 0.64 μV vs 0.74 ± 0.64 μV). Additionally, a significant main effect of Picture was observed [F(1,58) = 22.93, P < .001, ηp2 = 0.28], with greater LPP amplitudes elicited by social exclusion pictures compared to neutral pictures (2.52 ± 0.48 μV vs 1.44 ± 0.44 μV). Importantly, a significant Group × Picture interaction was identified [F(1,58) =6.61, P = .013, ηp2 = 0.10]. Post hoc comparisons revealed that under the scarcity mindset, social exclusion pictures elicited significantly greater LPP amplitudes than social neutral pictures (4.06 ± 0.69 μV vs 2.39 ± 0.63 μV, P < .001). In contrast, when participants were under the abundance mindset, LPP amplitudes did not differ significantly between social exclusion and neutral pictures (social exclusion: 0.99 ± 0.69 μV; social neutral: 0.49 ± 0.63 μV, P = .122) (see Fig. 3b and c).

Brain–behavior relationship

We investigated the role of empathy traits in the relationship between neural and behavioral responses, specifically focusing on the LPP, a key ERP component linked to the observation of social exclusion stimuli (He et al. 2020, Wang et al. 2021, Zhao et al. 2021). In our moderation models, the LPP amplitude (calculated as social exclusion minus social neutral) served as the independent variable (X), the rating of willingness to comfort (calculated as social exclusion minus social neutral) was the dependent variable (Y), and the affective empathy trait score acted as the moderator variable (Mo).

The analysis revealed that under the scarcity mindset, the moderation model was statistically significant (R2 = 0.29, P = .030). The regression equation was Y = − 0.48 + 1.44*X + 0.07*Mo − 0.04*X × Mo. Notably, the regression coefficients for the independent variable X (Β = 1.44, t = 2.75, P = .011, 95% CI = [0.364, 2.507]) and the interaction X × Mo (Β = − 0.04, t = − 2.55, P = .017, 95% CI = [−0.072, −0.008]) were both significant. Furthermore, the additional explanatory power gained by including the interaction term was also significant (∆R2 = 0.18, P = .017). Simple slope tests indicated that for individuals with low affective empathy traits, LPP amplitudes significantly predicted the ratings of willingness to comfort (Β = 0.33, t = 2.82, P = .009, 95% CI = [0.089, 0.572]). In contrast, for individuals with high affective empathy traits, LPP amplitudes did not significantly predict the ratings of willingness to comfort (Β = − 0.075, t = − 0.65, P = .521, 95% CI = [−0.314, 0.163]). However, under the abundance mindset, the moderation model did not reach statistical significance (R2 = 0.11, P = .384) (see Fig. 4 and Supplementary material S3 for further details).

Simple slope charts. Sores of affective empathy trait moderated the link between LPP amplitudes (social exclusion–social neutral) and ratings of willingness to comfort (social exclusion–social neutral) under scarcity mindset, but not under abundance mindset, with low and high values of each moderator correspond to +1/−1 SD from the mean.
Figure 4.

Simple slope charts. Sores of affective empathy trait moderated the link between LPP amplitudes (social exclusion–social neutral) and ratings of willingness to comfort (social exclusion–social neutral) under scarcity mindset, but not under abundance mindset, with low and high values of each moderator correspond to +1/−1 SD from the mean.

Discussion

This study investigated the impact of a scarcity mindset on individuals’ empathy for social pain and prosocial intentions toward others. We examined the differences in self-reported measures, neural responses, and the relationship with personality traits related to empathy between groups with scarcity and abundance mindsets. Behaviorally, the scarcity mindset group demonstrated greater differentiation in their ratings of unpleasantness and willingness to comfort between social exclusion and neutral stimuli compared to the abundance mindset group. On the neural level, we observed differences in early N2 and late P3 responses between scarcity and abundance mindsets for both social exclusion and neutral stimuli. Notably, LPP amplitudes in response to social exclusion and neutral stimuli varied significantly under the scarcity mindset, whereas they were comparable under the abundance mindset. Furthermore, when participants experienced a scarcity mindset, their affective empathy emerged as a crucial moderator in the relationship between LPP amplitudes and willingness to comfort ratings.

The stage-game paradigm was utilized to manipulate participants’ feelings of scarcity or abundance. The effectiveness of this manipulation was assessed through subjective ratings collected before the study and after each stage of the game, with a particular emphasis on stress ratings, as highlighted in previous studies (Huijsmans et al. 2019, Li et al. 2023). In this study, we successfully induced a scarcity mindset. Following the manipulation, individuals in the scarcity mindset group consistently reported higher levels of stress throughout the experiment than those in the abundance mindset group.

Evaluating the unpleasantness level of target individuals is a common indicator in the analysis of social exclusion images (Zhao et al. 2021, Mo et al. 2023). This indicator was chosen to assess empathy for social pain, while the willingness to comfort the target individual was included to measure prosocial intentions. Our findings for these two indicators were consistent: both groups reported significantly higher ratings for social exclusion images than for socially neutral ones, with the differences being more pronounced in the scarcity mindset group than in the abundance mindset group. It is important to note that while the unpleasantness ratings for social exclusion images were marginally lower under the scarcity mindset than under the abundance mindset, the scarcity mindset amplified the distinction between the two types of images. This observation aligns with the findings of Kraus et al. (2010), which demonstrated that individuals from lower social classes exhibited heightened empathic accuracy regarding others’ social emotions. Furthermore, a scarcity mindset is considered a form of psychosocial stress (Huijsmans et al. 2019). Our results are consistent with previous research indicating that psychosocial stress can lead to increased self-reported empathic responses (Gonzalez-Liencres et al. 2016) and prosocial behaviors (von Dawans et al. 2012, Tomova et al. 2017). This supports the “Tend and Befriend” model, which suggests that stress fosters affiliative behaviors, prompting individuals to seek protection and support from others (Taylor 2006, von Dawans et al. 2012, Azulay et al. 2022). These findings reveal that a scarcity mindset enhances both empathy for social pain and the intention to engage in prosocial behaviors by increasing sensitivity to others’ social suffering.

Correspondingly, the self-reported empathic ratings were supported by electrophysiological evidence, which revealed increased amplitudes of the LPP component under a scarcity mindset compared with an abundance mindset. Owing to the complexity of social exclusion and neutral images, the selected time window for LPP amplitudes was relatively long (Zhao et al. 2021), diverging from standard ERP analysis practices. Consistent with previous research, we observed a significant LPP component triggered by the passive viewing of social exclusion stimuli (He et al. 2020, Zhao et al. 2021). While the LPP component is typically associated with top-down attentional processing (Hajcak et al. 2010, Hajcak and Foti 2020), it also reflects cognitive engagement with emotionally salient stimuli, including the cognitive appraisal of others’ emotional states (Myruski et al. 2019). Our findings indicate that participants under a scarcity mindset exhibited enhanced LPP amplitudes in response to social exclusion, suggesting an increased cognitive effort in evaluating the emotional states of others. This supports the idea that individuals experiencing scarcity allocate more cognitive resources to appraising social stimuli (Li et al. 2023).

The LPP component is recognized as a reliable electrophysiological indicator of empathy for pain (Coll 2018), reflecting the top-down cognitive processes involved in evaluating others’ affective experiences (Fan and Han 2008). Consequently, the effect of a scarcity mindset on empathic neural responses to social pain primarily manifests in the top-down cognitive processing of ostracized victims’ emotional states. When observers encounter someone experiencing social pain, they may not immediately grasp the intensity of their suffering. Instead, they rely on prolonged observation and appraisal of the affective states to understand the individual’s “pain”. For individuals under a scarcity mindset, cognitive capacity may be diminished due to the constraints of scarcity (Mani et al. 2013). As a result, when instructed to focus on target individuals, they are likely to allocate limited mental resources to appraise the target’s emotional states, leading to a more nuanced evaluation. On the other hand, the LPP component over the central-parietal region is associated with top-down emotion regulation processes (Myruski et al. 2019, Hajcak and Foti 2020). During emotion regulation, LPP amplitudes typically decrease (He et al. 2020; Li et al. 2022). In addition to the mental resources dedicated to evaluation, participants under an abundance mindset may attempt to regulate their emotional responses to the affective states of excluded victims, albeit with limited mental resources. Consequently, the LPP amplitudes under an abundance mindset exhibited a negative shift relative to those under a scarcity mindset. In contrast to the scarcity mindset’s influence on the bottom-up and top-down stages of empathy for physical pain, a scarcity mindset selectively affected the top-down attentional processing stage during empathy for social pain. Empathy for social pain is less automatic than empathy for physical pain (Masten et al. 2010), making it more challenging for individuals to quickly discern whether others are experiencing social pain and to share their unpleasant emotions within a short timeframe.

In summary, a scarcity mindset enhanced the cognitive processing stage of empathy for social pain and the associated comforting intentions. It is important to note that this effect contrasts with previous findings indicating that individuals’ empathy for physical pain (Li et al. 2023) and their intentions to share were diminished under a scarcity mindset (Aarøe and Petersen 2013, Cui et al. 2022, 2023). According to the self-regulatory model of resource scarcity, individuals typically navigate two key psychological pathways in response to resource scarcity: the scarcity-reduction route, which motivates individuals to acquire resources to directly address the resource gap, and the control-restoration route, which encourages individuals to seek security in other domains (Cannon et al. 2019). Those who exhibit reduced empathy for physical pain and diminished intentions to share under a scarcity mindset may follow the scarcity-reduction route. In these situations, resources are often overvalued (Shah et al. 2012), leading individuals to focus on acquiring resources for themselves while neglecting the physiological needs of others. This focus can result in indifference to others’ suffering (e.g. actual physical pain) and a reluctance to share resources. Conversely, individuals in our study who had a scarcity mindset may have been following the control-restoration route. Despite experiencing resource scarcity, these individuals remain attuned to the emotions of others and accurately identify those emotions (Kraus et al. 2010, 2012). They are likely to establish affective bonds through empathy to find a sense of security. As a result, they are more inclined to be concerned about others’ social and emotional needs and to respond to the suffering caused by social interactions (i.e. social pain), demonstrating comforting intentions toward excluded victims. The different pathways individuals take in response to scarcity lead to varying emotional reactions to the suffering of others and differing prosocial intentions. The choice of route may be influenced by various contextual factors. However, these interpretations remain speculative, and further research is needed to explore the underlying mechanisms that contribute to these distinctions in greater detail.

Furthermore, individuals under a scarcity mindset exhibited smaller N2 amplitudes and larger P3 amplitudes than those under an abundance mindset, regardless of the stimulus type. The early N2 component is associated with the automatic emotional contagion stage during empathy processing (Fan and Han 2008, Cui et al. 2016). The reduced negative deflection in N2 amplitudes observed under a scarcity mindset suggests that individuals may struggle to capture automatic neural responses to complex emotional information, leading to a decreased sensitivity to others’ emotions during the early processing stage. On the other hand, the P3 component reflects late cognitive processing of emotional stimuli (Fan and Han 2008, Wei et al. 2023). Previous research has indicated that challenging contexts can enhance P3 responses, which suggests increased cognitive engagement (Wu et al. 2020). Similarly, individuals with a scarcity mindset may encounter cognitive challenges due to resource limitations (Mani et al. 2013, Shah et al. 2015). Consequently, they may demonstrate more positive-going P3 amplitudes compared to those in an abundance mindset, indicating heightened cognitive processing in response to emotional stimuli.

Interestingly, we found evidence of a moderated effect of affective empathy traits on the relationship between neural responses associated with empathy for social pain and behavioral judgments regarding prosocial intentions within the scarcity mindset group. In individuals with low affective empathy scores who experienced a scarcity mindset, greater LPP amplitudes were predictive of a higher willingness to comfort others. This relationship was not observed among those with high affective empathy scores. A scarcity mindset may amplify the facilitating effect of empathic neural responses on subjective prosocial intentions among individuals with low affective empathy. While viewing empathy as a limited resource may diminish empathic responses, engaging in effortful cognitive processing can enhance empathy and prosocial behavior toward others (Hasson et al. 2022). Consequently, individuals with low affective empathy traits may invest greater effort in utilizing mental resources to process and evaluate the affective states of others, resulting in higher prosocial intentions. In contrast, individuals with high affective empathy traits may find it relatively easier to empathize with excluded victims (Tao et al. 2020), even when faced with the mental resource challenges posed by a scarcity mindset. Therefore, when individuals have a scarcity mindset, those with low affective empathy traits may still demonstrate prosocial intentions toward others who are socially excluded, but they may need to mobilize more mental resources compared to their high affective empathy counterparts.

Several limitations should be acknowledged in this study. First, the stimulus materials included only images depicting social exclusion and neutral scenarios without including social inclusion images. As the perception of neutral stimuli can sometimes be biased toward negativity rather than remaining entirely neutral (Tae et al. 2020), future research should incorporate social inclusion images to provide a more comprehensive understanding of empathy in complex social interactions. Second, although our sample size met the standards for achieving sufficient statistical power, it may not have fully captured the entire spectrum of empathy levels present in the population. A larger sample size would not only enhance the reliability of the statistical outcomes but also provide a better representation of individuals with varying levels of empathy. Consequently, the results of this study should be considered preliminary. Future studies with larger sample sizes could further investigate how individuals with high versus low empathy are influenced by a scarcity mindset and how they respond to social exclusion scenarios.

Conclusions

This study revealed that a scarcity mindset enhanced the later cognitive appraisal stage of empathy for social pain, as indicated by the unpleasantness ratings and the LPP component. Additionally, it facilitated prosocial intentions toward excluded victims, as measured by the willingness to comfort ratings. Furthermore, the role of affective empathy traits emerged as pivotal in shaping the relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior in the context of a scarcity mindset. These findings enrich our understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying empathy for social pain and highlight the significant impact of empathy traits, thereby providing deeper insights into individuals’ social behaviors when resources are limited.

Author contributions

Wanchen Li (Methodology, Investigation, Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing), Zilong Wei (Writing—review and editing), Jun Wu (Investigation), Ru Song (Investigation), Jie Liu (Validation, Writing—review and editing, Supervision), Fang Cui (Conceptualization, Validation, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing, Funding acquisition, Supervision)

Supplementary data

Supplementary data is available at SCAN online.

Conflict of interest:

The authors declare no competing interests in relation to the subject of this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (NO. 32171013), and the Science and Technology Innovation Commission of Shenzhen (JCYJ20210324100411032).

Data availability

The data associated with this study can be found at https://pan.baidu.com/s/15Ad9AR2jTyCEvKpu6-js3A?pwd=2023.

References

Aarøe
 
L
,
Petersen
 
MB
 
Hunger games: fluctuations in blood glucose levels influence support for social welfare
.
Psychol Sci
 
2013
;
24
:
2550
56
. doi:

Agneman
 
G
,
Falco
 
P
,
Joel
 
E
 et al.  
The material basis of cooperation: how scarcity reduces trusting behaviour
.
Econ J
 
2023
;
133
:
1265
85
. doi:

Atkins
 
D
,
Uskul
 
AK
,
Cooper
 
NR
 
Culture shapes empathic responses to physical and social pain
.
Emotion
 
2016
;
16
:
587
601
. doi:

Azulay
 
H
,
Guy
 
N
,
Pertzov
 
Y
 et al.  
Empathy modulates the effect of stress reactivity on generous giving
.
Front Neurosci
 
2022
;
16
:814789. doi:

Bartoń
 
K
(
2023
).
MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R Package Version 1.46.5
. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn (
6 May 2004
, date last accessed).

Batson
 
CD
,
Ahmad
 
N
,
Yin
 
J
 et al.  
Two threats to the common good: self-interested egoism and empathy-induced altruism
.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull
 
1999
;
25
:
3
16
. doi:

Batson
 
CD
,
Chang
 
J
,
Orr
 
R
 et al.  
Empathy, attitudes and action: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group
.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull
 
2002
;
28
:
1656
66
. doi:

Batson
 
CD
,
Duncan
 
BD
,
Ackerman
 
P
 et al.  
Is empathic emotion a source of altruistic motivation?
 
J Pers Soc Psychol
 
1981
;
40
:
290
302
. doi:

Batson
 
CD
,
Dyck
 
JL
,
Brandt
 
JR
 et al.  
Five studies testing two new egoistic alternatives to the empathy altruism hypothesis
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
 
1988
;
55
:
52
77
. doi:

Batson
 
CD
Lishner
 
DA
Stocks
 
EL
. The empathy—Altruism hypothesis. In:
Schroeder
 
DA
and
Graziano
 
WG
(eds),
The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial Behavior
.
Oxford, England, UK
:
Oxford University Press
,
2015
,
259
81

Blanke
 
ES
Riediger
 
M
. Reading thoughts and feelings in other people: empathic accuracy across adulthood. In:
Srinivasan
 
N
(ed.),
Progress in Brain Research
.
Amsterdam, Netherlands
:
Elsevier
,
2019
, Vol.
247
,
305
27

Cannon
 
C
,
Goldsmith
 
K
,
Roux
 
C
 
A self-regulatory model of resource scarcity
.
J Consum Psychol
 
2019
;
29
:
104
27
. doi:

Chen
 
Z
,
Williams
 
KD
,
Fitness
 
J
 et al.  
When hurt will not heal: exploring the capacity to relive social and physical pain
.
Psychol Sci
 
2008
;
19
:
789
95
. doi:

Civai
 
C
,
Caserotti
 
M
,
Carrus
 
E
 et al.  
How perceived scarcity predicted cooperation during early pandemic lockdown
.
Front Psychol
 
2022
;
13
:951757. doi:

Cohen
 
J
.
Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences
.
Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA
:
Academic Press
,
2013
.

Coll
 
M-P
 
Meta-analysis of ERP investigations of pain empathy underlines methodological issues in ERP research
.
Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci
 
2018
;
13
:
1003
17
. doi:

Cuff
 
BMP
,
Brown
 
SJ
,
Taylor
 
L
 et al.  
Empathy: a review of the concept
.
Emot Rev
 
2016
;
8
:
144
53
. doi:

Cui
 
F
,
Deng
 
K
,
Liu
 
J
 et al.  
Resource scarcity aggravates ingroup bias: neural mechanisms and cross-scenario validation
.
Br J Psychol
 
2023
;
114
:
778
96
. doi:

Cui
 
F
,
Huang
 
X
,
Jing
 
Y
 et al.  
How resource sharing resists scarcity: the role of cognitive empathy and its neurobiological mechanisms
.
Cereb Cortex
 
2022
;
32
:
5330
42
. doi:

Cui
 
F
,
Ma
 
N
,
Luo
 
Y-J
 
Moral judgment modulates neural responses to the perception of other’s pain: an ERP study
.
Sci Rep
 
2016
;
6
:20851. doi:

Cui
 
F
,
Zhu
 
X
,
Luo
 
Y
 
Social contexts modulate neural responses in the processing of others’ pain: an event-related potential study
.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
 
2017
;
17
:
850
57
. doi:

Decety
 
J
,
Bartal
 
IB-A
,
Uzefovsky
 
F
 et al.  
Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species
.
Philos Trans R Soc London, Ser B
 
2016
;
371
:20150077. doi:

Decety
 
J
,
Cowell
 
JM.
 
The complex relation between morality and empathy
.
Trends Cogn Sci
 
2014
;
18
:
337
39
. doi:

Delorme
 
A
,
Makeig
 
S
 
EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis
.
J Neurosci Methods
 
2004
;
134
:
9
21
. doi:

Fan
 
Y
,
Han
 
S.
 
Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: an event-related brain potential study
.
Neuropsychologia
 
2008
;
46
:
160
73
. doi:

Faul
 
F
,
Erdfelder
 
E
,
Buchner
 
A
 et al.  
Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses
.
Behav Res Methods
 
2009
;
41
:
1149
60
. doi:

Feng
 
C
,
Li
 
Z
,
Feng
 
X
 et al.  
Social hierarchy modulates neural responses of empathy for pain
.
Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci
 
2016
;
11
:
485
95
. doi:

Flasbeck
 
V
,
Brüne
 
M.
 
Neural correlates of empathy for physical and psychological pain
.
J Psychophysiol
 
2019
;
33
:
54
63
. doi:

Gallese
 
V
,
Keysers
 
C
,
Rizzolatti
 
G.
 
A unifying view of the basis of social cognition
.
Trends Cogn Sci
 
2004
;
8
:
396
403
. doi:

Goldsmith
 
K
,
Roux
 
C
,
Wilson
 
AV.
 
Can thoughts of having less ever promote prosocial preferences? The relationship between scarcity, construal level, and sustainable product adoption
.
J Assoc Consum Res
 
2020
;
5
:
70
82
. doi:

Gonzalez-Liencres
 
C
,
Breidenstein
 
A
,
Wolf
 
OT
 et al.  
Sex-dependent effects of stress on brain correlates to empathy for pain
.
Int J Psychophysiol
 
2016
;
105
:
47
56
. doi:

Hajcak
 
G
,
Foti
 
D.
 
Significance?& Significance! Empirical, methodological, and theoretical connections between the late positive potential and P300 as neural responses to stimulus significance: an integrative review
.
Psychophysiology
 
2020
;
57
:e13570. doi:

Hajcak
 
G
,
MacNamara
 
A
,
Olvet
 
DM.
 
Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: an integrative review
.
Dev Neuropsychol
 
2010
;
35
:
129
55
. doi:

Hasson
 
Y
,
Amir
 
E
,
Sobol-Sarag
 
D
 et al.  
Using performance art to promote intergroup prosociality by cultivating the belief that empathy is unlimited
.
Nat Commun
 
2022
;
13
:7786. doi:

Hayes
 
AF
.
PROCESS : A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling
.
University of Kansas
,
2012
.

He
 
Z
,
Li
 
S
,
Mo
 
L
 et al.  
The VLPFC-engaged voluntary emotion regulation: combined TMS-fMRI evidence for the neural circuit of cognitive reappraisal
.
J Neurosci
 
2023
;
43
:
6046
60
. doi:

He
 
Z
,
Zhao
 
J
,
Shen
 
J
 et al.  
The right VLPFC and downregulation of social pain: a TMS study
.
Human Brain Mapp
 
2020
;
41
:
1362
71
. doi:

Huijsmans
 
I
,
Ma
 
I
,
Micheli
 
L
 et al.  
A scarcity mindset alters neural processing underlying consumer decision making
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
 
2019
;
116
:
11699
704
. doi:

Jung
 
T-P
,
Makeig
 
S
,
Westerfield
 
M
 et al.  
Analysis and visualization of single-trial event-related potentials
.
Human Brain Mapp
 
2001
;
14
:
166
85
. doi:

Krach
 
S
,
Cohrs
 
JC
,
de Echeverría Loebell
 
NC
 et al.  
Your flaws are my pain: linking empathy to vicarious embarrassment
.
PloS One
 
2011
;
6
:e18675. doi:

Krach
 
S
,
Kamp‐Becker
 
I
,
Einhäuser
 
W
 et al.  
Evidence from pupillometry and fMRI indicates reduced neural response during vicarious social pain but not physical pain in autism
.
Human Brain Mapp
 
2015
;
36
:
4730
44
. doi:

Kraus
 
MW
,
Côté
 
S
,
Keltner
 
D.
 
Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy
.
Psychol Sci
 
2010
;
21
:
1716
23
. doi:

Kraus
 
MW
,
Piff
 
PK
,
Keltner
 
D.
 
Social class, sense of control, and social explanation
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
 
2009
;
97
:
992
1004
. doi:

Kraus
 
MW
,
Piff
 
PK
,
Mendoza-Denton
 
R
 et al.  
Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: how the rich are different from the poor
.
Psychol Rev
 
2012
;
119
:
546
72
. doi:

Kuznetsova
 
A
,
Brockhoff
 
PB
,
Christensen
 
RH.
 
lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models
.
Journal of Statistical Software
 
2017
;
82
:
1
26
. doi:

Laforet
 
B
,
Carrera
 
P
,
Caballero
 
A.
 
Breaking self-focused orientation in people who perceive economic scarcity: the influence of transcendent motivation to promote an abstract mindset and prosocial behavioral intentions
.
Scand J Psychol
 
2024
;
65
:
607
16
. doi:

Leslie
 
AM
,
Friedman
 
O
,
German
 
TP.
 
Core mechanisms in “theory of mind”
.
Trends Cogn Sci
 
2004
;
8
:
528
33
. doi:

Levi
 
MK
,
Schreuer
 
N
,
Granovsky
 
Y
 et al.  
“Feeling unwanted, when nobody wants you around”: perceptions of social pain among people with autism
.
Am J Occup Ther
 
2023
;
77
:7702185050. doi:

Li
 
S
,
Xie
 
H
,
Zheng
 
Z
 et al.  
The causal role of the bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortices on emotion regulation of social feedback
.
Hum Brain Mapp
 
2022
;
43
:
2898
910
. doi:

Li
 
W
,
Meng
 
J
,
Cui
 
F.
 
Scarcity mindset reduces empathic responses to others’ pain: the behavioral and neural evidence
.
Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci
 
2023
;
18
:
1
10
. doi:

Liebowitz
 
MR.
 
Social phobia
.
Mod Probl Pharmacopsychiatry
 
1987
;
22
:
141
73
. doi:

Lockwood
 
PL
,
Apps
 
MAJ
,
Valton
 
V
 et al.  
Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
 
2016
;
113
:
9763
68
. doi:

Louie
 
TA
,
Rieta
 
RJ.
 
Sharing good fortune: effects of scarcity on small donation requests
.
Soc Influ
 
2018
;
13
:
65
77
. doi:

Macdonald
 
G
,
Leary
 
MR.
 
Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain
.
Psychol Bull
 
2005
;
131
:
202
23
. doi:

Mani
 
A
,
Mullainathan
 
S
,
Shafir
 
E
 et al.  
Poverty impedes cognitive function
.
Science
 
2013
;
341
:
976
80
. doi:

Marsh
 
AA
 
The neuroscience of empathy
.
Curr Opin Behav Sci
 
2018
;
19
:
110
15
. doi:

Masten
 
CL
,
Eisenberger
 
NI
,
Pfeifer
 
JH
 et al.  
Witnessing peer rejection during early adolescence: neural correlates of empathy for experiences of social exclusion
.
Soc Neurosci
 
2010
;
5
:
496
507
. doi:

Masten
 
CL
,
Morelli
 
SA
,
Eisenberger
 
NI.
 
An fMRI investigation of empathy for “social pain” and subsequent prosocial behavior
.
NeuroImage
 
2011
;
55
:
381
88
. doi:

Meyer
 
ML
,
Masten
 
CL
,
Ma
 
Y
 et al.  
Empathy for the social suffering of friends and strangers recruits distinct patterns of brain activation
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
 
2013
;
8
:
446
54
. doi:

Mills
 
L
,
Driver
 
C
,
McLoughlin
 
LT
 et al.  
A systematic review and meta-analysis of electrophysiological studies of online social exclusion: evidence for the neurobiological impacts of cyberbullying
.
Adolesc Res Rev
 
2024
;
9
:
135
63
. doi:

Mo
 
L
,
Li
 
S
,
Cheng
 
S
 et al.  
Emotion regulation of social pain: double dissociation of lateral prefrontal cortices supporting reappraisal and distraction
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
 
2023
;
18
:nsad043. doi:

Myruski
 
S
,
Bonanno
 
GA
,
Cho
 
H
 et al.  
The late positive potential as a neurocognitive index of emotion regulatory flexibility
.
Biol Psychol
 
2019
;
148
:107768. doi:

Pang
 
Y
,
Song
 
C
,
Ma
 
C.
 
Effect of different types of empathy on prosocial behavior: gratitude as mediator
.
Front Psychol
 
2022
;
13
:768827. doi:

Paulus
 
FM
,
Müller-Pinzler
 
L
,
Jansen
 
A
 et al.  
Mentalizing and the role of the posterior superior temporal sulcus in sharing others’ embarrassment
.
Cereb Cortex
 
2015
;
25
:
2065
75
. doi:

Perry
 
A
,
Bentin
 
S
,
Bartal
 
IB-A
 et al.  
“Feeling” the pain of those who are different from us: modulation of EEG in the mu/alpha range
.
Cognit Affective Behav Neurosci
 
2010
;
10
:
493
504
. doi:

Piff
 
PK
,
Kraus
 
MW
,
Côté
 
S
 et al.  
Having less, giving more: the influence of social class on prosocial behavior
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
 
2010
;
99
:
771
84
. doi:

Reniers
 
RLEP
,
Corcoran
 
R
,
Drake
 
R
 et al.  
The QCAE: a questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy
.
J Pers Assess
 
2011
;
93
:
84
95
. doi:

Rodriguez
 
K
,
Plascencia Ibarra
 
I
,
Musick
 
A
 et al.  
Event-related correlates of compassion for social pain
.
Soc Neurosci
 
2023
;
18
:
91
102
. doi:

Roux
 
C
,
Goldsmith
 
K
,
Bonezzi
 
A.
 
On the psychology of scarcity: when reminders of resource scarcity promote selfish (and generous) behavior
.
J Consum Res
 
2015
;
42
:
615
31
. doi:

Shah
 
AK
,
Mullainathan
 
S
,
Shafir
 
E.
 
Some consequences of having too little
.
Science
 
2012
;
338
:
682
85
. doi:

Shah
 
AK
,
Shafir
 
E
,
Mullainathan
 
S.
 
Scarcity frames value
.
Psychol Sci
 
2015
;
26
:
402
12
. doi:

Singer
 
T
,
Klimecki
 
OM.
 
Empathy and compassion
.
Curr Biol
 
2014
;
24
:
R875
R878
. doi:

Singer
 
T
,
Seymour
 
B
,
O’Doherty
 
J
 et al.  
Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain
.
Science
 
2004
;
303
:
1157
62
. doi:

Spielberger
 
CD
,
Gorsuch
 
RL
,
Lushene
 
RV
 et al. .
Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
.
Palo Alto, California, USA
:
Consulting Psychologists Press
,
1983
.

Tae
 
J
,
Nam
 
Y-E
,
Lee
 
Y
 et al.  
Neutral but not in the middle: cross-cultural comparisons of negative bias of “neutral” emotional stimuli
.
Cogn Emot
 
2020
;
34
:
1171
82
. doi:

Tao
 
D
,
Leng
 
Y
,
Ge
 
S
 et al.  
Trait empathy modulates brain response to empathy for social rejection: evidence from electrophysiology
. In: 2020 42nd Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine & Biology Society (EMBC)  
Montreal, Quebec, Canada
, pp.
1027
30
,
2020
.

Taylor
 
SE.
 
Tend and befriend: biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress
.
Curr Dir Psychol Sci
 
2006
;
15
:
273
77
. doi:

Tomova
 
L
,
Majdandžic
 
J
,
Hummer
 
A
 et al.  
Increased neural responses to empathy for pain might explain how acute stress increases prosociality
.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
 
2017
;
12
:
401
08
. doi:

von Dawans
 
B
,
Fischbacher
 
U
,
Kirschbaum
 
C
 et al.  
The social dimension of stress reactivity: acute stress increases prosocial behavior in humans
.
Psychol Sci
 
2012
;
23
:
651
60
. doi:

Wang
 
G
,
Wang
 
P
,
Chen
 
Y.
 
Doctors inhibit social threat empathy in the later stage of cognitive processing: electrophysiological evidence
.
Conscious Cogn
 
2021
;
92
:103130. doi:

Wei
 
ZL
,
Huang
 
YJ
,
Li
 
X
 et al.  
The influence of aggressive exercise on responses to self-perceived and others’ pain
.
Cereb Cortex
 
2023
;
33
:
10802
12
. doi:

Wesselmann
 
ED
,
Wirth
 
JH
,
Pryor
 
JB
 et al.  
When do we ostracize?
 
Soc Psychol Pers Sci
 
2013
;
4
:
108
15
. doi:

Wu
 
T
,
Han
 
S.
 
Neural mechanisms of modulations of empathy and altruism by beliefs of others’ pain
.
eLife
 
2021
;
10
:e66043. doi:

Wu
 
Y
,
Liu
 
Y
,
Yao
 
M
 et al.  
Language contexts modulate instant empathic responses to others’ pain
.
Psychophysiology
 
2020
;
57
:e13562. doi:

Ye
 
T
,
Mattila
 
AS.
 
How do resource scarcity reminders influence consumers’ CSR engagement?
 
Int J Contemp Hosp Manag
 
2023
;
36
:
1963
79
. doi:

Yu
 
W
,
Li
 
Y
,
Cao
 
X
 et al.  
The role of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex on voluntary emotion regulation of social pain
.
Hum Brain Mapp
 
2023
;
44
:
4710
21
. doi:

Zaki
 
J
,
Ochsner
 
KN.
 
The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise
.
Nat Neurosci
 
2012
;
15
:
675
80
. doi:

Zhao
 
J
,
Mo
 
L
,
Bi
 
R
 et al.  
The VLPFC versus the DLPFC in downregulating social pain using reappraisal and distraction strategies
.
J Neurosci
 
2021
;
41
:
1331
39
. doi:

Zheng
 
Z
,
Li
 
S
,
Mo
 
L
 et al.  
ISIEA: an image database of social inclusion and exclusion in young Asian adults
.
Behav Res Methods
 
2022
;
54
:
2409
21
. doi:

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data