Abstract

Introduction

Although studies have suggested that implicit attitudes may predict smoking-related decisions, evidence that changes in implicit attitudes toward smoking are related to changes in smoking behavior is lacking. Using data from a trial comparing interventions to induce quit attempts among unmotivated smokers, this study examined whether changes in implicit attitudes were associated with quit attempts and cessation after controlling for explicit motivation.

Methods

Daily smokers recruited from the community completed measures of implicit attitudes (Implicit Association Test) and explicit measure of motivation to smoke at baseline, mid-intervention (week 12 [W12]) and follow-up (week 26 [W26]). Quit attempts and cessation were assessed at follow-up, and cessation was biochemically verified.

Results

As hypothesized, Implicit Association Test scores became more negative from baseline to W12, a change that was sustained at follow-up. Logistic regression analyses in which implicit attitudes were used to predict smoking outcomes revealed that negative changes in implicit attitudes from baseline to W12 and from baseline to W26 were significantly related to quit attempts (OR = 0.71, 95% CI [0.52, 0.97], p < .05 for both) independent of explicit motivation. Negative changes in implicit attitudes from baseline to W26 were significantly related to cessation (OR = 0.50, 95% CI [0.25, 1.00], p < .05).

Conclusions

Negative changes in implicit attitudes were associated with positive changes in smoking behavior independent of explicit motivation. This result indicates that smoking cessation interventions may be enhanced by incorporating strategies to change implicit attitudes, and that changes in implicit attitudes are also potentially important intervention outcomes.

Implications

Smoking cessation interventions may be improved by going beyond the current focus on explicit psychological constructs and targeting automatic cognitive processes such as implicit attitudes. The results are encouragement to examine how best to manipulate smokers’ implicit attitudes as well as to determine the effect on their smoking behavior.

Introduction

Decision-making regarding health behaviors such as smoking is understood to be a function of individuals’ positive and negative evaluations of cigarette use and its consequences.1 Historically, researchers have measured these evaluations, or attitudes, toward smoking with explicit self-report questionnaires.2 However, these explicit attitudes only modestly predict the level of smoking and changes in cigarette use over time.3,4 Recently, researchers have examined implicit attitudes in relationship to health behavior from a dual-process perspective.5 According to this model, human behavior including health behavior is guided by two independent systems, explicit and implicit processes. These two motivational processes are thought to have unique contributions to smoking behavior.6 Implicit-attitude tests measure fast, parallel, effortless, and uncontrolled processes.7–9 Implicit attitudes are also less susceptible to socially-desirable responding, which is common in self-reported measurement of stigmatized behaviors such as smoking.10,11 More importantly, smoking is an addictive behavior, which is thought to be particularly influenced by automatic processes.12,13

Most studies have found that smokers’ attitudes are significantly less negative than nonsmokers’ attitudes.14–18 In addition, implicit attitudes toward smoking are more negative in light smokers compared to heavy smokers,16 are more negative among individuals lower in nicotine dependence,19,20 and are less negative among smokers during withdrawal.21 Implicit attitudes have also been associated with smoking cessation at an 18-month follow-up22 although implicit attitudes assessed before and after smoking cessation have failed to predict relapse.23 These studies suggest that implicit attitudes can predict smoking behaviors and could be a novel treatment target in cessation interventions.

In order to translate experimental research on implicit attitudes into a clinical context, more evidence is needed of the association between changes in implicit attitudes and smoking behavior in natural contexts. While there is considerable evidence that implicit attitudes are relatively inflexible and difficult to change, especially once they are formed,24,25 there is also evidence from memory and social cognition research that suggests implicit attitudes can change rather quickly in response to contextual factors and motivational states, with relatively little counterattitudinal information.26–30 Furthermore, studies have shown that changes in implicit attitudes are not necessarily accompanied by changes in explicit attitudes.6,26,31

A few studies have observed changes in implicit attitudes due to counterattitudinal messages such as anti-tobacco television advertisements,32 anti-smoking public service announcements,33 warning labels of cigarette packages,34 and approach-avoidance practice interventions.35 However, long-term changes in implicit attitudes following a smoking cessation intervention have not been investigated. More importantly, no study has related changes in implicit attitudes toward smoking to changes in smoking behaviors, such as quit attempts and cessation. Lastly, existing studies on implicit attitudes toward smoking have predominantly recruited young adult (ie, college-aged), Caucasian, non-daily smokers as participants; thus, these results may not generalize to a broader, more heterogeneous smoking population.

The aim of this study was to determine whether changes in implicit attitudes toward smoking were associated with changes in quit attempts and cessation independently from changes in self-reported or explicitly measured motivation to smoke. Data were drawn from a smoking cessation induction trial involving a diverse sample of community residents. We hypothesized that changes in implicit attitudes toward smoking from baseline to mid-intervention, and from baseline to follow-up, would be associated with quit attempts and cessation. We also hypothesized that the prediction of changes in implicit attitudes on changes in smoking behavior would be independent of changes in explicit motivation to smoke.

Methods

Participants

Daily smokers (N = 255) who were not motivated to quit smoking were recruited from a large Midwestern city for a clinical trial to induce quit attempts.36 Eligible participants were at least 18 years of age, smoked at least one cigarette a day, spoke English, were not pregnant, were not using smoking-cessation medication, and were not motivated to quit smoking (≤6 on a 0 to 10 scale of motivation to quit) or not planning to quit smoking in the next 7 days. The study received Institutional Review Board approval, and informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

Assessments of implicit attitudes and explicit motivation were administered at baseline, mid-intervention (week 12 [W12]) and follow-up (week 26 [W26]). Demographics and smoking characteristics were obtained at baseline, and changes in smoking behavior were reported at W26.

Demographics and Smoking Characteristics

The baseline survey included assessment of demographics and smoking characteristics including number of cigarettes smoked per day and age when the first cigarette was smoked. Nicotine dependence was assessed using the “time to first cigarette” item (ie, “How soon after you wake up do you smoke your first cigarette of the day?”) from the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence.37 This single item has been shown to account for much of the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence’s predictive validity for cessation success,38 and was used to reduce survey length.

Implicit Attitudes Toward Smoking

To maximize consistency with prior research on smoking behavior, implicit attitudes were assessed with the Implicit Association Test (IAT39) using stimuli and procedures from prior smoking studies.35,40 Stimuli (presented on a 22” computer monitor with a black background) were eight pictures related to smoking (labeled on the screen as “smoking”; eg, a picture of cigarettes next to a lighter, a picture of a lit cigarette), eight picture of shapes (labeled as “shape”; eg, pictures of line-drawn shapes such as rectangles and ovals), eight adjectives with a positive meaning (labeled as “good”; eg, wonderful, nice, pleasant), and eight adjectives with a negative meaning (labeled as “bad”; eg, stupid, disgusting, horrible). The picture size was set to 20% of the height and width of the computer screen. The background and brightness of the smoking and shape pictures were closely matched. The adjective letter size was set to 5% of the height and width of the screen, and adjectives were presented in green letters.

Participants were asked to categorize the stimuli presented in the center of the screen according to the labels presented in the upper right or left side of the screen, as quickly as possible, by pressing the letter e (left) or i (right) on the keyboard. The label letter size was set to 3.5% of the height and width of the screen. The smoking/shapes categories were presented in white letters, while the good/bad categories were presented in green letters. The IAT consisted of seven phases: (1) 20 practice trials categorizing word stimuli into “good” or “bad”; (2) 20 practice trials categorizing picture stimuli into “smoking” or “shapes”; (3) and (4) 20 practice and 40 experimental trials categorizing word and picture stimulus pairs into one of two combined categories (eg, smoking + good vs. shapes + bad); (5) the same 20 practice trials as phase 2 except the right/left locations of category labels (ie, shapes vs. smoking) were switched on the computer screen; and (6) and (7) the same 20 practice and 40 experimental trials as phase 3 and 4 except the right/left locations of the combined categories were switched on the computer screen (eg, shapes + good vs. smoking + bad). On each trial, the stimulus was presented until the participant pressed a letter on the keyboard. Following a correct response, the next stimulus was presented. Following an incorrect response, an X was presented on the screen and the same stimulus was presented again until the correct response was made. The presentation order of paired categories was counterbalanced. Implicit attitudes were indexed using an IAT D score that was calculated for each participant at each assessment point by subtracting the response latencies when smoking pictures were paired with “good” words from the response latencies when smoking pictures were paired with “bad” words.41 Since longer latencies imply an incongruent association, negative IAT scores indicate longer response latencies in smoking pictures—“good” word pairs and thus negative implicit attitudes toward smoking. Individuals with more than 30% error rates (including too fast trials in which latencies were less than 300 ms and too slow trials in which latencies were above 10 000 ms) in the main IAT task were excluded from data analyses. With this criterion, 27 IAT scores from 18 participants were excluded in the data analyses. For the primary analysis change scores were calculated by subtracting baseline IAT D scores from W12 IAT D scores. Higher scores reflect less of a negative shift in attitudes toward smoking over time. Internal consistency (Chronbach’s α) using difference scores for every experimental trial42 was 0.75 for baseline IAT in this study.

Explicit Motivation to Smoke

The 20-item Decisional Balance Scale43 was used to assess self-reported or explicit motivation to smoke. Participants indicated the extent of agreement with a list of items describing the pros (ie, “I am relaxed and therefore more pleasant when smoking”) and cons (ie, “People close to me disapprove of my smoking”) of smoking on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not important; 5 = extremely important). Explicit motivation to smoke was calculated using a difference score between pro and con items with positive scores indicating positive explicit motivation to smoke. In the primary analysis change scores were calculated by subtracting baseline Decisional Balance Scale scores from W12 scores. Higher scores reflect less of a negative shift in motivation to smoke.

Changes in Smoking Behavior

To assess quit attempts, participants were asked if they had made a serious quit attempt lasting at least 24 hours since the last assessment.44,45 Reports made at W12 and W26 were aggregated into a single dichotomous variable that indicated whether any quit attempt had been made since baseline. Assessment of smoking status at W26 was based on self-reported 7-day point prevalence abstinence.46 Participants were asked if they had smoked “even a puff” in the past 7 days. Reports of cessation were biochemically verified using saliva cotinine.47

Procedure

Smokers were recruited through advertisements targeting “smokers” or “smokers not quite ready to quit” and prospective participants were prescreened for eligibility by phone. Participants were told the goal of the study was to learn how best to talk to smokers about their health. Eligible participants were invited to an in-person visit and rescreened. The participants whose eligibility was reconfirmed were asked to complete measures of demographics, implicit attitudes, and explicit motivation. Then, they were randomly assigned to one of three treatment arms (a one-session brief advice to quit arm, a four-session health education arm, and a four-session Motivational Interviewing arm) and received the first intervention session based on treatment group assignment. The single-session brief advice intervention provided smokers with clear, strong, and personalized advice to quit tobacco use. The four-session health education intervention provided education about the risks and costs of smoking, the benefits of quitting, and the potential solutions to the common obstacles to quitting. The four-session Motivational Interviewing intervention used the style and methods of Motivational Interviewing45 to foster motivation to quit smoking. Across all intervention arms, persons wanting to quit were provided assistance with behavioral strategies and choosing medication, which was offered free of charge. The three additional health education and Motivational Interviewing sessions were offered every 6 weeks after baseline. At W12 (mid-intervention) and W26 (follow-up), the participants completed measures of implicit attitudes, explicit motivation, and smoking behaviors.

Data Analysis

Analyses of the effects of the treatments on smoking outcomes are reported in detail elsewhere.37 The present study analyses focused on whether changes in smoking behavior were associated with changes in implicit attitudes, regardless of treatment received.

Preliminary analyses used one-sample t tests to examine changes in IAT D scores over time, and Pearson’s correlation coefficients were used to examine associations between IAT D and explicit motivation scores at each time point, between changes in IAT D scores and explicit motivation scores, and between changes in IAT D scores and treatment group. The primary analyses used stepwise logistic regression models in which changes in implicit attitudes (from baseline to W12 or W26) were used to predict any quit attempt and cessation at W26. Change in explicit motivation was entered in the first step and the corresponding change in IAT D scores was entered in the second step. To attempt to confirm that IAT D changes preceded changes in smoking behavior (rather than changes in smoking behavior preceding IAT D change), an additional longitudinal stepwise logistic regression was conducted in which change in implicit attitudes and explicit motivation from baseline to W12 was used to predict any quit attempt from W12 to W26. To reduce small-sample bias, the Firth penalized likelihood approach was used for stepwise logistic regression models predicting cessation. Analyses were also repeated with treatment group as a covariate and with the addition of an implicit attitude X explicit motivation interaction effect; however, because results were unchanged we present only the results without treatment group or interaction effects included.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

To be included in any of the main analyses, the participants needed to have (1) IAT D scores at baseline and W12, or at baseline and W26, and (2) either quit attempt or smoking cessation data at W26. Among the 255 participants, 13 did not meet either criterion; 21 participants did not meet criterion 1; and another 11 did not meet criterion 2. Thus, 45 participants were excluded from analyses. Demographic information for participants included in the analyses is presented in Table 1. The mean age of the participants was 46 years (SD = 10.7), and they were predominantly male (57%) and African American (66%). The mean age at first cigarette use was 16 years (SD = 4.8) and the average number of cigarettes smoked per day was 16 (SD = 9.2). There were no significant differences in age, gender, ethnicity, and smoking history between those who were included in the analyses and those who were not included in the analyses.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of the Participants Who Were Included in Analyses (N = 210)

VariablesM (SD) or frequency
Age45.88 (10.68)
Gender
 Female90 (42.9%)
 Male120 (57.1%)
Hispanic origin
 Hispanic7 (3.3%)
 Non-Hispanic203 (96.7%)
Race
 Caucasian70 (33.3%)
 African American139 (66.2%)
 Others1 (0.5%)
Education
 Less than high school35 (16.7%)
 High school degree142 (67.6%)
 College degree29 (13.8%)
 Graduate degree4 (1.9%)
Employment
 Employed full time23 (11.0%)
 Employed part time22 (10.5%)
 Unemployed165 (78.6%)
Marital status
 Single121 (57.6%)
 Divorced/separated/widowed49 (23.3%)
 Married/committed40 (19.1%)
Monthly income
 $0–$1000116 (55.2%)
 $1001–$200043 (20.5%)
 $2001–$300011 (5.2%)
 More than $300014 (6.7%)
 Don’t know/declined to answer26 (12.4%)
Age at first cigarette use16.41 (4.83)
The number of cigarettes smoked per day16.27 (9.21)
First cigarette after waking up100 (47.6%)
 Within 5 min90 (42.9%)
 6–30 min8 (3.8%)
 6–30 min12 (5.7%)
VariablesM (SD) or frequency
Age45.88 (10.68)
Gender
 Female90 (42.9%)
 Male120 (57.1%)
Hispanic origin
 Hispanic7 (3.3%)
 Non-Hispanic203 (96.7%)
Race
 Caucasian70 (33.3%)
 African American139 (66.2%)
 Others1 (0.5%)
Education
 Less than high school35 (16.7%)
 High school degree142 (67.6%)
 College degree29 (13.8%)
 Graduate degree4 (1.9%)
Employment
 Employed full time23 (11.0%)
 Employed part time22 (10.5%)
 Unemployed165 (78.6%)
Marital status
 Single121 (57.6%)
 Divorced/separated/widowed49 (23.3%)
 Married/committed40 (19.1%)
Monthly income
 $0–$1000116 (55.2%)
 $1001–$200043 (20.5%)
 $2001–$300011 (5.2%)
 More than $300014 (6.7%)
 Don’t know/declined to answer26 (12.4%)
Age at first cigarette use16.41 (4.83)
The number of cigarettes smoked per day16.27 (9.21)
First cigarette after waking up100 (47.6%)
 Within 5 min90 (42.9%)
 6–30 min8 (3.8%)
 6–30 min12 (5.7%)
Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of the Participants Who Were Included in Analyses (N = 210)

VariablesM (SD) or frequency
Age45.88 (10.68)
Gender
 Female90 (42.9%)
 Male120 (57.1%)
Hispanic origin
 Hispanic7 (3.3%)
 Non-Hispanic203 (96.7%)
Race
 Caucasian70 (33.3%)
 African American139 (66.2%)
 Others1 (0.5%)
Education
 Less than high school35 (16.7%)
 High school degree142 (67.6%)
 College degree29 (13.8%)
 Graduate degree4 (1.9%)
Employment
 Employed full time23 (11.0%)
 Employed part time22 (10.5%)
 Unemployed165 (78.6%)
Marital status
 Single121 (57.6%)
 Divorced/separated/widowed49 (23.3%)
 Married/committed40 (19.1%)
Monthly income
 $0–$1000116 (55.2%)
 $1001–$200043 (20.5%)
 $2001–$300011 (5.2%)
 More than $300014 (6.7%)
 Don’t know/declined to answer26 (12.4%)
Age at first cigarette use16.41 (4.83)
The number of cigarettes smoked per day16.27 (9.21)
First cigarette after waking up100 (47.6%)
 Within 5 min90 (42.9%)
 6–30 min8 (3.8%)
 6–30 min12 (5.7%)
VariablesM (SD) or frequency
Age45.88 (10.68)
Gender
 Female90 (42.9%)
 Male120 (57.1%)
Hispanic origin
 Hispanic7 (3.3%)
 Non-Hispanic203 (96.7%)
Race
 Caucasian70 (33.3%)
 African American139 (66.2%)
 Others1 (0.5%)
Education
 Less than high school35 (16.7%)
 High school degree142 (67.6%)
 College degree29 (13.8%)
 Graduate degree4 (1.9%)
Employment
 Employed full time23 (11.0%)
 Employed part time22 (10.5%)
 Unemployed165 (78.6%)
Marital status
 Single121 (57.6%)
 Divorced/separated/widowed49 (23.3%)
 Married/committed40 (19.1%)
Monthly income
 $0–$1000116 (55.2%)
 $1001–$200043 (20.5%)
 $2001–$300011 (5.2%)
 More than $300014 (6.7%)
 Don’t know/declined to answer26 (12.4%)
Age at first cigarette use16.41 (4.83)
The number of cigarettes smoked per day16.27 (9.21)
First cigarette after waking up100 (47.6%)
 Within 5 min90 (42.9%)
 6–30 min8 (3.8%)
 6–30 min12 (5.7%)

The mean IAT D and explicit motivation scores at each time point and the mean IAT D and explicit motivation change scores are presented in Table 2. Overall, the IAT D scores tended to be negative, indicating that the participants held negative implicit attitudes toward smoking. Furthermore, IAT D scores became significantly more negative over time (t(206) = −2.46, p < .05 and t(198) = −2.10, p < .05 for W12 and W26, respectively).

Table 2.

Mean of IAT D and Explicit Motivation Scores Across Time

VariablesTotal M (SD)Quit attempt vs. no quit attemptCessation vs. no cessation
Quit attempt M (SD)No quit attempt M (SD)Cessation M (SD)No cessation M (SD)
IAT D
 Baselinea−0.16 (0.75)−0.08 (0.75)−0.27 (0.75)−0.04 (0.67)−0.19 (0.76)
 W12b−0.32 (0.71)−0.35 (0.70)−0.28 (0.74)−0.50 (0.57)−0.32 (0.72)
 W26c−0.32 (0.66)−0.38 (0.66)−0.23 (0.65)−0.80 (0.32)−0.28 (0.66)
IAT D change scores
 Baseline to W12b−0.17 (0.95)−0.27 (0.95)−0.01 (0.96)−0.47 (0.96)−0.13 (0.96)
 Baseline to W26c−0.15 (0.99)−0.27 (1.03)0.03 (0.91)−0.76 (0.83)−0.09 (0.98)
Explicit motivation
 Baselinea2.52 (11.56)0.48 (11.39)5.53 (11.21)6.20 (9.14)2.32 (11.76)
 W12b−3.09 (13.16)−7.31 (12.51)3.22 (11.52)−3.89 (12.30)−2.73 (13.11)
 W26c−3.79 (12.57)−8.03 (11.28)2.24 (11.87)−5.00 (18.40)−3.63 (12.29)
Explicit motivation change scores
 Baseline to W12b−5.55 (11.27)−7.78 (11.79)−2.20 (9.57)−10.56 (19.40)−4.97 (10.40)
 Baseline to W26c−6.60 (11.74)−8.79 (12.19)−3.49 (10.37)−11.20 (22.40)−6.26 (10.80)
VariablesTotal M (SD)Quit attempt vs. no quit attemptCessation vs. no cessation
Quit attempt M (SD)No quit attempt M (SD)Cessation M (SD)No cessation M (SD)
IAT D
 Baselinea−0.16 (0.75)−0.08 (0.75)−0.27 (0.75)−0.04 (0.67)−0.19 (0.76)
 W12b−0.32 (0.71)−0.35 (0.70)−0.28 (0.74)−0.50 (0.57)−0.32 (0.72)
 W26c−0.32 (0.66)−0.38 (0.66)−0.23 (0.65)−0.80 (0.32)−0.28 (0.66)
IAT D change scores
 Baseline to W12b−0.17 (0.95)−0.27 (0.95)−0.01 (0.96)−0.47 (0.96)−0.13 (0.96)
 Baseline to W26c−0.15 (0.99)−0.27 (1.03)0.03 (0.91)−0.76 (0.83)−0.09 (0.98)
Explicit motivation
 Baselinea2.52 (11.56)0.48 (11.39)5.53 (11.21)6.20 (9.14)2.32 (11.76)
 W12b−3.09 (13.16)−7.31 (12.51)3.22 (11.52)−3.89 (12.30)−2.73 (13.11)
 W26c−3.79 (12.57)−8.03 (11.28)2.24 (11.87)−5.00 (18.40)−3.63 (12.29)
Explicit motivation change scores
 Baseline to W12b−5.55 (11.27)−7.78 (11.79)−2.20 (9.57)−10.56 (19.40)−4.97 (10.40)
 Baseline to W26c−6.60 (11.74)−8.79 (12.19)−3.49 (10.37)−11.20 (22.40)−6.26 (10.80)

IAT = Implicit Association Test; W12 = week 12; W26 = week 26.

aIn this row: total N = 210, quit attempt n = 125, no quit attempt n = 85, cessation n = 10, no cessation n = 194.

bIn this row: total N = 207, Quit attempt n = 124, no quit attempt n = 83, cessation n = 9, no cessation n = 192.

cIn this row: total N = 199, quit attempt n = 117, no quit attempt n = 82, cessation n = 10, no cessation n = 190.

Table 2.

Mean of IAT D and Explicit Motivation Scores Across Time

VariablesTotal M (SD)Quit attempt vs. no quit attemptCessation vs. no cessation
Quit attempt M (SD)No quit attempt M (SD)Cessation M (SD)No cessation M (SD)
IAT D
 Baselinea−0.16 (0.75)−0.08 (0.75)−0.27 (0.75)−0.04 (0.67)−0.19 (0.76)
 W12b−0.32 (0.71)−0.35 (0.70)−0.28 (0.74)−0.50 (0.57)−0.32 (0.72)
 W26c−0.32 (0.66)−0.38 (0.66)−0.23 (0.65)−0.80 (0.32)−0.28 (0.66)
IAT D change scores
 Baseline to W12b−0.17 (0.95)−0.27 (0.95)−0.01 (0.96)−0.47 (0.96)−0.13 (0.96)
 Baseline to W26c−0.15 (0.99)−0.27 (1.03)0.03 (0.91)−0.76 (0.83)−0.09 (0.98)
Explicit motivation
 Baselinea2.52 (11.56)0.48 (11.39)5.53 (11.21)6.20 (9.14)2.32 (11.76)
 W12b−3.09 (13.16)−7.31 (12.51)3.22 (11.52)−3.89 (12.30)−2.73 (13.11)
 W26c−3.79 (12.57)−8.03 (11.28)2.24 (11.87)−5.00 (18.40)−3.63 (12.29)
Explicit motivation change scores
 Baseline to W12b−5.55 (11.27)−7.78 (11.79)−2.20 (9.57)−10.56 (19.40)−4.97 (10.40)
 Baseline to W26c−6.60 (11.74)−8.79 (12.19)−3.49 (10.37)−11.20 (22.40)−6.26 (10.80)
VariablesTotal M (SD)Quit attempt vs. no quit attemptCessation vs. no cessation
Quit attempt M (SD)No quit attempt M (SD)Cessation M (SD)No cessation M (SD)
IAT D
 Baselinea−0.16 (0.75)−0.08 (0.75)−0.27 (0.75)−0.04 (0.67)−0.19 (0.76)
 W12b−0.32 (0.71)−0.35 (0.70)−0.28 (0.74)−0.50 (0.57)−0.32 (0.72)
 W26c−0.32 (0.66)−0.38 (0.66)−0.23 (0.65)−0.80 (0.32)−0.28 (0.66)
IAT D change scores
 Baseline to W12b−0.17 (0.95)−0.27 (0.95)−0.01 (0.96)−0.47 (0.96)−0.13 (0.96)
 Baseline to W26c−0.15 (0.99)−0.27 (1.03)0.03 (0.91)−0.76 (0.83)−0.09 (0.98)
Explicit motivation
 Baselinea2.52 (11.56)0.48 (11.39)5.53 (11.21)6.20 (9.14)2.32 (11.76)
 W12b−3.09 (13.16)−7.31 (12.51)3.22 (11.52)−3.89 (12.30)−2.73 (13.11)
 W26c−3.79 (12.57)−8.03 (11.28)2.24 (11.87)−5.00 (18.40)−3.63 (12.29)
Explicit motivation change scores
 Baseline to W12b−5.55 (11.27)−7.78 (11.79)−2.20 (9.57)−10.56 (19.40)−4.97 (10.40)
 Baseline to W26c−6.60 (11.74)−8.79 (12.19)−3.49 (10.37)−11.20 (22.40)−6.26 (10.80)

IAT = Implicit Association Test; W12 = week 12; W26 = week 26.

aIn this row: total N = 210, quit attempt n = 125, no quit attempt n = 85, cessation n = 10, no cessation n = 194.

bIn this row: total N = 207, Quit attempt n = 124, no quit attempt n = 83, cessation n = 9, no cessation n = 192.

cIn this row: total N = 199, quit attempt n = 117, no quit attempt n = 82, cessation n = 10, no cessation n = 190.

Prior to the main analyses intercorrelations between smoking characteristics, IAT D and explicit motivation scores across time were examined (Table 3). Average change scores for IAT D and explicit motivation were generally not significantly related to baseline motivation to quit and nicotine dependence. Results revealed weak positive associations between average IAT D score and explicit motivation measured at the same time point with only one significant association at W26 (r = 0.16, p < .05). The average change scores for IAT D were not significantly associated with the average change scores for explicit motivation. Also, the average change scores for IAT D were not significantly different across treatment groups at any time interval.

Table 3.

Correlations Among Baseline Smoking Characteristics, Smoking Behavior Changes, and IAT D and Explicit Motivation Scores

IAT DIAT D change scoresExplicit motivationExplicit motivation change scores
BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26
Motivation to quit−0.036−0.159*−0.063−0.096−0.015−0.371**−0.215**−0.150*0.1300.207**
Time to first cigarette−0.104−0.227**−0.127−0.084−0.009−0.344**−0.240**−0.244**0.0730.080
Quit attempt (yes/no)0.120−0.050−0.108−0.131−0.153*−0.215**−0.400**−0.412**−0.247**−0.227**
Cessation (yes/no)0.043−0.052−0.175*−0.072−0.150*0.072−0.021−0.019−0.107−0.094
IAT D
 Base0.140*0.016−0.680**−0.750**0.1340.1020.153*−0.0190.032
 W120.236**0.631**0.0510.238**0.1300.172−0.092−0.054
 W260.161*0.650**0.1210.1240.161*0.0200.052
IAT D change scores
 Base to W120.624**0.0760.0180.009−0.058−0.067
 Base to W26−0.018−0.0150.0020.0020.020
Explicit motivation
 Base0.594**0.536**−0.334**−0.417**
 W120.750**0.560**0.200**
 W260.315**0.544**
Explicit motivation change scores
 Base to W120.682**
IAT DIAT D change scoresExplicit motivationExplicit motivation change scores
BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26
Motivation to quit−0.036−0.159*−0.063−0.096−0.015−0.371**−0.215**−0.150*0.1300.207**
Time to first cigarette−0.104−0.227**−0.127−0.084−0.009−0.344**−0.240**−0.244**0.0730.080
Quit attempt (yes/no)0.120−0.050−0.108−0.131−0.153*−0.215**−0.400**−0.412**−0.247**−0.227**
Cessation (yes/no)0.043−0.052−0.175*−0.072−0.150*0.072−0.021−0.019−0.107−0.094
IAT D
 Base0.140*0.016−0.680**−0.750**0.1340.1020.153*−0.0190.032
 W120.236**0.631**0.0510.238**0.1300.172−0.092−0.054
 W260.161*0.650**0.1210.1240.161*0.0200.052
IAT D change scores
 Base to W120.624**0.0760.0180.009−0.058−0.067
 Base to W26−0.018−0.0150.0020.0020.020
Explicit motivation
 Base0.594**0.536**−0.334**−0.417**
 W120.750**0.560**0.200**
 W260.315**0.544**
Explicit motivation change scores
 Base to W120.682**

IAT = Implicit Association Test; W12 = week 12; W26 = week 26.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Table 3.

Correlations Among Baseline Smoking Characteristics, Smoking Behavior Changes, and IAT D and Explicit Motivation Scores

IAT DIAT D change scoresExplicit motivationExplicit motivation change scores
BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26
Motivation to quit−0.036−0.159*−0.063−0.096−0.015−0.371**−0.215**−0.150*0.1300.207**
Time to first cigarette−0.104−0.227**−0.127−0.084−0.009−0.344**−0.240**−0.244**0.0730.080
Quit attempt (yes/no)0.120−0.050−0.108−0.131−0.153*−0.215**−0.400**−0.412**−0.247**−0.227**
Cessation (yes/no)0.043−0.052−0.175*−0.072−0.150*0.072−0.021−0.019−0.107−0.094
IAT D
 Base0.140*0.016−0.680**−0.750**0.1340.1020.153*−0.0190.032
 W120.236**0.631**0.0510.238**0.1300.172−0.092−0.054
 W260.161*0.650**0.1210.1240.161*0.0200.052
IAT D change scores
 Base to W120.624**0.0760.0180.009−0.058−0.067
 Base to W26−0.018−0.0150.0020.0020.020
Explicit motivation
 Base0.594**0.536**−0.334**−0.417**
 W120.750**0.560**0.200**
 W260.315**0.544**
Explicit motivation change scores
 Base to W120.682**
IAT DIAT D change scoresExplicit motivationExplicit motivation change scores
BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26BaseW12W26Base to W12Base to W26
Motivation to quit−0.036−0.159*−0.063−0.096−0.015−0.371**−0.215**−0.150*0.1300.207**
Time to first cigarette−0.104−0.227**−0.127−0.084−0.009−0.344**−0.240**−0.244**0.0730.080
Quit attempt (yes/no)0.120−0.050−0.108−0.131−0.153*−0.215**−0.400**−0.412**−0.247**−0.227**
Cessation (yes/no)0.043−0.052−0.175*−0.072−0.150*0.072−0.021−0.019−0.107−0.094
IAT D
 Base0.140*0.016−0.680**−0.750**0.1340.1020.153*−0.0190.032
 W120.236**0.631**0.0510.238**0.1300.172−0.092−0.054
 W260.161*0.650**0.1210.1240.161*0.0200.052
IAT D change scores
 Base to W120.624**0.0760.0180.009−0.058−0.067
 Base to W26−0.018−0.0150.0020.0020.020
Explicit motivation
 Base0.594**0.536**−0.334**−0.417**
 W120.750**0.560**0.200**
 W260.315**0.544**
Explicit motivation change scores
 Base to W120.682**

IAT = Implicit Association Test; W12 = week 12; W26 = week 26.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Associations Between Implicit Attitude and Explicit Motivation Changes and Smoking Behavior

Simple correlations revealed that IAT D change scores were negatively related to smoking outcomes but only the IAT changes from baseline to W26 reached significance (Table 3). The pattern of IAT D scores across time for the participants who made any quit attempts compared with participants who did not make any quit attempts is presented in the left panel of Figure 1. The results of the stepwise logistic regression models in which explicit motivation change scores and IAT D change scores were used to predict quit attempts are presented in the top 2 sections of Table 4. In both stepwise models predicting any quit attempts, a comparison of the goodness-of-fit revealed that the addition of IAT D change (baseline to W12 or baseline to W26) in step 2 added significantly to the prediction of quit attempts over and above explicit motivation change entered in step 1 (X2 [1, N = 207] = 4.88, p < .05 and X2 [1, N = 199] = 4.91, p < .05, respectively). The final models revealed that for both the baseline to W12 and baseline to W26 models IAT D change and explicit motivation change were significant predictors of any quit attempt when both predictors were in the model. The results indicated that negative shifts in IAT D and explicit motivation over time were associated with higher odds of making a quit attempt.

Changes in Implicit Association Test [IAT] D: quit attempt vs. no quit attempt and cessation vs. no cessation.
Figure 1.

Changes in Implicit Association Test [IAT] D: quit attempt vs. no quit attempt and cessation vs. no cessation.

Table 4.

Results of Logistic Regression Models Using Implicit Attitude and Explicit Motivation Changes to Predict Quit Attempt and Cessation

PredictorsQuit attempt vs. no quit attempt (any)
Model coefficientsNagelkerke R2Odds95% CIp
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 12.855 (p = .000)0.0810.9530.927, 0.980.001
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 17.737 (p = .000)0.1110.9490.923, 0.977.001
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 207) = 4.882 (p = .027)0.7110.523, 0.968.030
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 10.312 (p = .001)0.0680.9590.934, 0.985.032
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 15.218 (p = .000)0.0990.9580.932, 0.984.002
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.906 (p = .027)0.7110.522, 0.967.030
Quit attempt vs. no quit attempt (W12 to W26)
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 204) = 3.940 (p = .047)0.0260.9750.950, 1.000.051
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 204) = 7.551 (p = .023)0.0490.9730.948, 0.999.039
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 3.611 (p = .057)0.7540.561, 1.013.061
Cessation vs. no cessation
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 2.243 (p = .134)0.9580.907, 1.012.122
 Step 2Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 3.340 (p = .188)0.9570.906, 1.012.122
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 1.097 (p = .295)0.6660.324, 1.369.269
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 1.764 (p = .184)0.9660.920, 1.015.167
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 5.825 (p = .054)0.9690.923, 1.018.208
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.061 (p = .044)0.5200.272, 0.994.048
PredictorsQuit attempt vs. no quit attempt (any)
Model coefficientsNagelkerke R2Odds95% CIp
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 12.855 (p = .000)0.0810.9530.927, 0.980.001
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 17.737 (p = .000)0.1110.9490.923, 0.977.001
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 207) = 4.882 (p = .027)0.7110.523, 0.968.030
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 10.312 (p = .001)0.0680.9590.934, 0.985.032
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 15.218 (p = .000)0.0990.9580.932, 0.984.002
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.906 (p = .027)0.7110.522, 0.967.030
Quit attempt vs. no quit attempt (W12 to W26)
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 204) = 3.940 (p = .047)0.0260.9750.950, 1.000.051
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 204) = 7.551 (p = .023)0.0490.9730.948, 0.999.039
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 3.611 (p = .057)0.7540.561, 1.013.061
Cessation vs. no cessation
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 2.243 (p = .134)0.9580.907, 1.012.122
 Step 2Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 3.340 (p = .188)0.9570.906, 1.012.122
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 1.097 (p = .295)0.6660.324, 1.369.269
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 1.764 (p = .184)0.9660.920, 1.015.167
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 5.825 (p = .054)0.9690.923, 1.018.208
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.061 (p = .044)0.5200.272, 0.994.048

CI = confidence interval; IAT = Implicit Association Test; W12 = week 12; W26 = week 26.

Table 4.

Results of Logistic Regression Models Using Implicit Attitude and Explicit Motivation Changes to Predict Quit Attempt and Cessation

PredictorsQuit attempt vs. no quit attempt (any)
Model coefficientsNagelkerke R2Odds95% CIp
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 12.855 (p = .000)0.0810.9530.927, 0.980.001
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 17.737 (p = .000)0.1110.9490.923, 0.977.001
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 207) = 4.882 (p = .027)0.7110.523, 0.968.030
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 10.312 (p = .001)0.0680.9590.934, 0.985.032
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 15.218 (p = .000)0.0990.9580.932, 0.984.002
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.906 (p = .027)0.7110.522, 0.967.030
Quit attempt vs. no quit attempt (W12 to W26)
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 204) = 3.940 (p = .047)0.0260.9750.950, 1.000.051
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 204) = 7.551 (p = .023)0.0490.9730.948, 0.999.039
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 3.611 (p = .057)0.7540.561, 1.013.061
Cessation vs. no cessation
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 2.243 (p = .134)0.9580.907, 1.012.122
 Step 2Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 3.340 (p = .188)0.9570.906, 1.012.122
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 1.097 (p = .295)0.6660.324, 1.369.269
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 1.764 (p = .184)0.9660.920, 1.015.167
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 5.825 (p = .054)0.9690.923, 1.018.208
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.061 (p = .044)0.5200.272, 0.994.048
PredictorsQuit attempt vs. no quit attempt (any)
Model coefficientsNagelkerke R2Odds95% CIp
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 12.855 (p = .000)0.0810.9530.927, 0.980.001
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 17.737 (p = .000)0.1110.9490.923, 0.977.001
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 207) = 4.882 (p = .027)0.7110.523, 0.968.030
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 10.312 (p = .001)0.0680.9590.934, 0.985.032
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 15.218 (p = .000)0.0990.9580.932, 0.984.002
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.906 (p = .027)0.7110.522, 0.967.030
Quit attempt vs. no quit attempt (W12 to W26)
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 204) = 3.940 (p = .047)0.0260.9750.950, 1.000.051
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 204) = 7.551 (p = .023)0.0490.9730.948, 0.999.039
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 3.611 (p = .057)0.7540.561, 1.013.061
Cessation vs. no cessation
Baseline to W12
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 207) = 2.243 (p = .134)0.9580.907, 1.012.122
 Step 2Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 207) = 3.340 (p = .188)0.9570.906, 1.012.122
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 1.097 (p = .295)0.6660.324, 1.369.269
Baseline to W26
 Step 1:Explicit motivation changeX2 (1, N = 199) = 1.764 (p = .184)0.9660.920, 1.015.167
 Step 2:Explicit motivation changeX2 (2, N = 199) = 5.825 (p = .054)0.9690.923, 1.018.208
IAT D changeX2diff (1, N = 199) = 4.061 (p = .044)0.5200.272, 0.994.048

CI = confidence interval; IAT = Implicit Association Test; W12 = week 12; W26 = week 26.

In the longitudinal model predicting quit attempts between W12 and W26 (third section in Table 4) the results were similar although the addition of IAT D change in step 2 did not add significantly to the overall model prediction (X2 [1, N = 204] = 3.61, p > .05), and the effect of IAT D change from baseline to W12 was slightly weaker and was not significant.

The pattern of IAT D scores across time for participants who were abstinent at W26 compared to participants who were not abstinent at W26 is presented in the right panel of Figure 1. The results of the stepwise logistic regression models using Firth penalized likelihood approach in which explicit motivation change scores and IAT D change scores were used to predict cessation are presented in the bottom 2 sections of Table 4. In the two stepwise models predicting cessation, a comparison of the goodness-of-fit revealed that the addition of IAT D change from baseline to W26 in step 2 added significantly to the prediction of cessation over and above explicit motivation change entered in step 1 (X2 [1, N = 199] = 4.06, p < .05). In the final model examining changes in IAT D and explicit motivation from baseline to W12 as predictors of cessation, neither changes in IAT D nor changes in explicit motivation were significant predictors. In the final model examining changes in IAT D and explicit motivation from baseline to W26 as predictors of cessation, IAT D change was a significant predictor but explicit motivation change was not. In these models a negative shifts in implicit attitudes over time was associated with higher odds of cessation.

Discussion

According to the dual-process theory of cognition, explicit and implicit processes are expected to independently contribute to motivating smoking-related behavior. Until recently, most studies investigating the relationship between cognition and smoking behavior have focused on explicit attitudes.5 The focus of this study was on the additional contribution of implicit attitude change to predict changes in smoking behavior over and above explicit motivation change, supporting the dual-process theory of cognition in smoking behavior.

In a large smoking cessation induction trial with predominantly low-income community residents, smokers held somewhat negative implicit attitudes toward smoking, consistent with prior research.14–18 Results indicated that smokers who had greater negative shifts in implicit attitudes at mid-intervention and at follow-up were more likely to have made quit attempts during the study period. Likewise, greater negative shifts in implicit attitudes toward smoking from baseline to W26 were significantly related to cessation at follow-up while explicit motivation failed to predict cessation. Changes in implicit attitudes from baseline to W12 were not significantly associated with cessation at follow-up, perhaps indicating the effects of implicit attitudes are stronger for quit attempts.

These results provide the first evidence of an association between changes in implicit attitudes toward smoking using the IAT and changes in smoking behavior. A recent study that examined changes in the IAT (from baseline to a few days after the quit day) and 6-month self-reported continuous abstinence among 41 smokers found no significant association.23 The reason for the different finding is unclear but could include differences related to the timing of the assessment of change in IAT, differences in the outcome measured (ie, induction of quit attempts and 7-day point prevalence cessation), and the sample size.

The potential association between changes in implicit attitudes and smoking behavior is important because implicit attitude is only valuable as a treatment target if changes in implicit attitudes contribute to changes in behavior. Although our results are correlational, they are consistent with a potential causal association and go beyond existing evidence that has associated implicit attitude scores with smoking behaviors.17–20,22 The present results also extend evidence of short-term effects of counterattitudinal information and practice interventions on implicit attitudes toward smoking32–35 by showing that long-term changes in implicit attitudes during treatment relate to quit attempts and cessation. This helps translate experimental research into clinical research and practice.

Particularly encouraging from a treatment perspective is that, as hypothesized, the relationships between changes in implicit attitudes and smoking behaviors were independent of explicit motivation. We found only weak associations between implicit attitudes and explicit motivation, and there were no significant correlations between changes in implicit attitudes and explicit motivation, similar to previous studies. Changes in explicit motivation were also associated with outcomes both bivariately and in models with implicit attitude. Thus, our results support previous work that indicates implicit attitudes may be a valuable additional target for facilitating smoking behavior change.

The changes in IAT observed in this study are also consistent with the possibility that existing treatments may lead to shifts in implicit attitude. However, there were no differences in IAT changes related to treatment group and there was no pure attention control group. Shifts in IAT may have been due to other factors or changes in smoking behavior. Nevertheless, these results suggest it might be worthwhile to examine implicit attitudes as an outcome in treatment trials to enhance understanding of treatment effects.

One of the strengths of this study is that the participants were adult smokers from the general community, because prior work has tended to focus on young adults.18 Cigarette usage and health consequences differ across demographic groups, with African Americans, on average, being less successful at quitting and suffering from higher smoking-related disease mortality than Caucasian smokers.48,49 Our replication of findings in prior studies in this sample suggests prior implicit attitude findings may be generalizable across diverse groups of smokers.

There are a number of limitations to this study including, as noted, the correlational nature of the study design, which precludes concluding that changes in implicit attitudes caused changes in smoking behavior. We conducted one model that examined if early changes in implicit attitudes were associated with subsequent quit attempts but the effect only approached significance. The reason for this nonsignificant result is unclear but could be due to a weaker effect because of the greater time lag between the occurrence of the attitude change or due to an alternative causal relationship in which quit attempts lead to changes in implicit attitudes. For example, there is evidence that implicit attitudes appear to shift in response to emotional experiences or to maintain cognitive consistency,10 both of which could be relevant when making quit attempts. To clarify these issues studies are needed that examine the effects of implicit attitude manipulation on long-term smoking cessation.

Another limitation of this study is the use of the IAT to assess implicit attitudes. Although it was selected to allow the results from this treatment trial to be compared with prior experimental work in smoking behavior, questions have been raised about the validity of the original IAT50,51 and there is evidence that different measures of implicit attitudes are not highly correlated.17,23 Results should be interpreted with the limitations of the IAT in mind. This study is also limited by the small number of the participants who achieved abstinence. Although this low cessation rate is to be expected in a smoking cessation trial where participants who are unmotivated to quit smoking are recruited, the results with respect to smoking cessation may be subject to some small sample bias. Results may differ in more traditional cessation trials that evaluate treatments among smokers who desire to quit.

In spite of the limitations, this is the first study to examine implicit attitudes in a large treatment trial of diverse adult smokers recruited from the community and to show significant associations between long-term changes in implicit attitudes and smoking behavior. This study provides strong support for continuing to pursue the development of interventions based on implicit attitudes for smoking cessation.

Funding

This study was supported by grant R01CA133068 from the National Cancer Institute of the National Institutes of Health and a University of Kansas Cancer Center Pilot Project Award. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Varenicline (Chantix) was provided by Pfizer through Investigator-Initiated Research Support (No. WS759405).

Declaration of Interests

None declared.

References

1.

Hine
DW
Marks
ADG
O’Neill
G
.
Smoking cessation in adults: a dual process perspective
.
Addict Res Theory
.
2009
;
17
(
2
):
220
229
. doi:
10.1080/16066350802386108

2.

Wiers
RW
Bartholow
BD
van den Wildenberg
E
et al. .
Autonomic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model
.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav
.
2007
;
86
(
2
):
263
283
. doi:
10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021

3.

De Leeuw
RNH
Engels
RCME
Vermulst
AA
Scholte
RH
.
Do smoking attitudes predict behavior? A longitudinal study on the bi-directional relations between adolescents’ smoking attitudes and behaviours
.
Addiction
.
2008
;
103
(
10
):
1713
1721
. doi:
10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02293.x

4.

Stacy
AW
Bentler
PM
Flay
BR
.
Attitudes and health behavior in diverse populations: drunk driving, alcohol use, binge eating, marijuana use, and cigarette use
.
Health Psychol
.
1994
;
13
(
1
):
73
85
. doi:
10.1037//0278-6133.13.1.73

5.

Wiers
RW
Stacy
AW.
Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction
.
Thousand Oaks, CA
:
Sage Publications, Inc
;
2006
.

6.

Rydell
RJ
McConnell
AR
.
Understanding implicit and explicit attitude change: a systems of reasoning analysis
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2006
;
91
(
6
):
995
1008
. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.995

7.

Dovidio
JF
Kawakami
K
Beach
KR
.
Implicit and explicit attitudes: examination of the relations between measures of intergroup bias
. In:
Brown
R
Gaertner
S
, eds.
Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes
.
Oxford, UK
:
Backwell Publishing
;
2001
.

8.

Greenwald
AG
Banaji
MR
.
Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes
.
Psychol Rev
.
1995
;
102
(
1
):
4
27
. doi:
10.1037//0033-295X.102.1.4

9.

Stacy
AW
Ames
SL
Knowlton
BJ
.
Neurologically plausible distinctions in cognition relevant to drug use etiology and prevention
.
Subst Use Misuse
.
2004
;
39
(
10–12
):
1571
1623
. doi:
10.1081/LSUM-200033204

10.

Rudman
LA
.
Sources of implicit attitudes
.
Curr Dir Psychol Sci
.
2004
;
13
(
2
):
79
82
. doi:
10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00279.x

11.

Wiers
RW
de Jong
PJ
Havermans
R
Jelicic
M
.
How to change implicit drug use-related cognitions in prevention: a transdisciplinary integration of findings from experimental psychopathology, social cognition, memory, and experimental learning psychology
.
Subst Use Misuse
.
2004
;
39
(
10–12
):
1625
1684
. doi:
10.1081/JA-200033206

12.

Baxter
BW
Hinson
RE
.
Is smoking automatic? Demands of smoking behavior on attentional resources
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
2001
;
110
(
1
):
59
66
. doi:
10.1037//0021-843X.110.1.59

13.

Field
M
Mogg
K
Bradley
BP
.
Automaticity of smoking behaviour: the relationship between dual-task performance, daily cigarette intake and subjective nicotine effects
.
J Psychopharmacol
.
2006
;
20
(
6
):
799
805
. doi:
10.1177/0269881106063997

14.

Chassin
L
Presson
C
Rose
J
Sherman
SJ
Prost
J
.
Parental smoking cessation and adolescent smoking
.
J Pediatr Psychol
.
2002
;
27
(
6
):
485
496
. doi:
10.1093/jpepsy/27.6.485

15.

Huijding
J
de Jong
PJ
Wiers
RW
Verkooijen
K
.
Implicit and explicit attitudes toward smoking in a smoking and a nonsmoking setting
.
Addict Behav
.
2005
;
30
(
5
):
949
961
. doi:
10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.09.014

16.

Robinson
MD
Meier
BP
Zetocha
KJ
McCaul
KD
.
Smoking and the Implicit Association Test: when the contrast category determines the theoretical conclusions
.
Basic Appl Soc Psych
.
2005
;
27
(
3
):
201
212
. doi:
10.1207/s15324834basp2703_2

17.

Sherman
SJ
Rose
JS
Koch
K
Presson
CC
Chassin
L
.
Implicit and explicit attitudes toward cigarette smoking: the effects of context and motivation
.
J Soc Clin Psychol
.
2003
;
22
(
1
):
13
39
. doi:
10.1521/jscp.22.1.13.22766

18.

Swanson
JE
Rudman
LA
Greenwald
AG
.
Using the Implicit Association Test to investigate attitude-behaviour consistency for stigmatized behavior
.
Cognition Emotion
.
2001
;
15
(
2
):
207
230
. doi:
10.1080/0269993004200060

19.

Huijing
J
de Jong
PJ
.
Automatic associations with the sensory aspects of smoking: positive in habitual smokers but negative in non-smokers
.
Addict Behav
.
2006
;
31
(
1
):
182
186
. doi:
10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.04.014

20.

Waters
A
Carter
BL
Robinson
JD
Wetter
DW
Lam
CY
Cinciripini
PM
.
Implicit attitudes to smoking are associated with craving and dependence
.
Drug Alcohol Depend
.
2007
;
91
(
2–3
):
178
186
. doi:
10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.024

21.

Payne
BK
McClernon
FJ
Dobbins
IG
.
Automatic affective responses to smoking cues
.
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol
.
2007
;
15
(
4
):
400
409
. doi:
10.1037/1064-1297.15.4.400

22.

Chassin
L
Presson
CC
Sherman
SJ
Seo
D-C
Macy
JT
.
Implicit and explicit attitudes predict smoking cessation: moderating effects of experienced failure to control smoking and plans to quit
.
Psychol Addict Behav
.
2010
;
24
(
4
):
670
679
. doi:
10.1037/a0021722

23.

Spruyt
A
Lemaigre
V
Salhi
B
et al. .
Implicit attitudes towards smoking predict long-term relapse in abstinent smokers
.
Psychopharmacol
.
2015
;
232
(
14
):
2551
2561
. doi:
10.1007/s00213-015-3893-2

24.

Gregg
AP
Seibt
B
Banaji
MR
.
Easier done than undone: asymmetry in the malleability of implicit preferences
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2006
;
90
(
1
):
1
20
. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.1

25.

Petty
RE
Tormala
Z
Briñol
P
Jarmis
WBG
.
Implicit ambivalence from attitude change: an exploration of the PAST Model
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2006
;
90
(
1
):
21
41
. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.21

26.

Dasgupta
N
Greenwald
AG
.
On the malleability of automatic attitudes: combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2001
;
81
(
5
):
800
814
. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.800

27.

Gawronski
B
Rydell
RJ
Vervliet
B
de Houwer
J
.
Generalization versus contextualization in automatic evaluation
.
J Exp Psychol Gen
.
2010
;
139
(
4
):
682
701
. doi:
10.1037/a0020315

28.

Rudman
LA
Ashmore
RD
Gary
ML
.
“Unlearning” automatic biases: the malleability of implicit prejudice and stereotypes
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2001
;
81
(
5
):
856
868
. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.856

29.

Rydell
RJ
Gawronski
B
.
I like you, I like you not: understanding the formation of context dependent automatic evaluations
.
Cognition Emotion
.
2009
;
23
(
6
):
1118
1152
. doi:
10.1080/02699930802355255

30.

Wittenbrink
B
Judd
CM
Park
B
.
Spontaneous prejudice in context: variability in automatically activated attitudes
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2001
;
81
(
5
):
815
827
. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.815

31.

Rydell
RJ
McConnell
AR
Mackie
DM
Strain
LM
.
Of two minds: forming and changing valence-inconsistent implicit and explicit attitudes
.
Psychol Sci
.
2006
;
17
(
11
):
954
958
. doi:
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01811.x

32.

Czyzewska
M
Ginsburg
HJ
.
Explicit and implicit effects of anti-marijuana and anti-tobacco TV advertisements
.
Addict Behav
.
2007
;
32
(
1
):
114
127
. doi:
10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.03.025

33.

Rydell
RJ
Sherman
SJ
Boucher
KL
Macy
JT
.
The role of motivational and persuasive message factors in changing implicit attitudes toward smoking
.
Basic Appl Soc Psych
.
2012
;
34
(
1
):
1
7
. doi:
10.1080/01973533.2011.637847

34.

Macy
JT
Chassin
L
Presson
C
Yeung
E
.
Exposure to graphic warning labels on cigarette packages: effects on implicit and explicit attitudes towards smoking among young adults
.
Psychol Health
.
2016
;
31
(
3
):
349
363
. doi:
10.1080/08870446.2015.1104309

35.

Macy
JT
Chassin
L
Presson
C
Sherman
JW
.
Changing implicit attitudes toward smoking: results from a web-based approach-avoidance practice intervention
.
J Behav Med
.
2014
;
38
(
1
):
143
152
. doi:
10.1007/s10865-014-9585-2

36.

Catley
D
Goggin
K
Harris
KJ
et al. .
Motivational Interviewing for inducing quit attempts and cessation among unmotivated smokers: a Randomized Clinical Trial
.
Am J Prev Med
.
2016
;
50
(
5
):
573
583
. doi:
10.1016/j.amepre.2015.10.013

37.

Fagerström
K
.
Time to first cigarette; the best single indicator of tobacco dependence
?
Monaldi Arch Chest Dis
.
2003
;
59
(
1
):
91
94
.

38.

Baker
TB
Piper
ME
McCarthy
DE
et al. .
Time to first cigarette in the morning as an index of ability to quit smoking: implications for nicotine dependence
.
Nocotine Tob Res
.
2007
;
9
(
suppl 4
):
S555
S570
. doi:
10.1080/14622200701673480
.

39.

Greenwald
AG
McGhee
DE
Schwartz
JL
.
Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
1998
;
74
(
6
):
1464
1480
. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464

40.

Sherman
SJ
Chassin
L
Presson
C
Seo
D-C
Macy
JT
.
The intergenerational transmission of implicit and explicit attitudes toward smoking: prediction adolescent smoking initiation
.
J Exp Soc Psychol
.
2009
;
45
(
2
):
313
319
. doi:
10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.012

41.

Greenwald
AG
Nosek
BA
Banaji
MR
.
Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: an improved scoring algorithm
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2003
;
85
(
2
):
197
216
. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197

42.

Schnabel
K
Asendorpf
JB
Greenwald
AG
.
Using Implicit Association Tests for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept
. In:
Boyle
GJ
Matthews
G
Saklofske
H
, eds.
Handbook of Personality Theory and Testing
.
London, UK
:
Sage
;
2007
.

43.

Velicer
WF
DiClemente
CC
Prochaska
JO
Brandenburg
N
.
Decisional balance measure for assessing and predicting smoking status
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
1985
;
48
(
5
):
1279
1289
. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.48.5.1279

44.

Carpenter
MJ
Hughes
JR
Solomon
LJ
Callas
PW
.
Both smoking reduction with nicotine replacement therapy and motivational advice increase future cessation among smokers unmotivated to quit
.
J Consult Clin Psych
.
2004
;
72
(
3
):
371
381
. doi:
10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.371

45.

Zhou
X
Nonnemaker
J
Sherrill
B
Gilsenan
AW
Coste
F
West
R
.
Attempts to quit smoking and relapse: factors associated with success or failure from the ATTEMPT cohort study
.
Addict Behav
.
2009
;
34
(
4
):
365
373
. doi:
10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.11.013

46.

Hughes
JR
Keely
JP
Niaura
RS
Ossip-Klein
DJ
Richmond
RL
Swan
GE
.
Measures of abstinence in clinical trials: issues and recommendations
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2003
;
5
(
1
):
13
25
. doi:
10.1080/1462220031000070552

47.

Cooke
F
Bullen
C
Whittaker
R
McRobbie
H
Chen
M-H
Walker
N
.
Diagnostic accuracy of nicalert cotinine test strips in saliva for verifying smoking status
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2008
;
10
(
4
):
607
612
. doi:
10.1080/14622200801978680

48.

Gandhi
KK
Foulds
J
Steinberg
MB
Lu
SE
Williams
JM
.
Lower quit rates among African American and Latino menthol cigarette smokers at a tobacco treatment clinic
.
Int J Clin Pract
.
2009
;
63
(
3
):
360
370
. doi:
10.1111/j.1742-1241.2008.01969.x

49.

Pamuk
E
Makuc
D
Heck
K
Reuben
C
Lochner
K.
Socioeconomic Status and Health Chartbook
.
Hyattsville, MD
:
National Center for Health Statistics
;
1998
.

50.

Houben
K
Nosek
BA
Wiers
RW
.
Seeing the forest through the trees: a comparison of different IAT variants measuring implicit alcohol associations
.
Drug Alcohol Depend
.
2010
;
106
(
2–3
):
204
211
. doi:
10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.08.016

51.

Houben
K
Wiers
RW
Roefs
A
.
Reaction time measures of substance-related associations
. In:
Wiers
RW
Stacy
W
, eds.
Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction
.
Thousand Oaks, CA
:
Sage Publication
;
2006
.

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.