Abstract

Objective

The objective of this study was to systematically review the standardized neurodevelopmental assessments used to study preschool-aged children’s cognitive development in Spanish-speaking Latin America.

Methods

The authors systematically searched PubMed, PsycINFO, and ERIC databases for peer-reviewed articles from Spanish-speaking Latin American countries. Articles were included if they measured cognitive development among children aged 2–6 years using at least one standardized assessment tool; 97 articles were included and reviewed in accordance with PRISMA guidelines to assess their use of these tools.

Results

Ninety-seven studies across 13 countries used a total of 41 assessments to measure cognitive development; most widely used were the Wechsler intelligence scales (n = 46/97), particularly the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (n = 23 and 29, respectively). Other common assessments included the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (n = 9), Raven’s Progressive Matrices (n = 9), Child Neuropsychological Assessment (n = 8), and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (n = 7). In regions where normative data for a given assessment were unpublished, authors commonly used norms from the United States, Mexico, or Spain or did not report standard scores in their analyses.

Conclusions

The wide range of tools used in these studies presents a challenge for generalizing results when measuring the neurodevelopment of Latin American preschool-aged children. The low availability of normative data for specific regions reveals concerns if some tools are culturally and linguistically appropriate even when Spanish is a common language, particularly in low-resource settings. Future work to forge greater consistency in the use of validated measures, clarity in reporting research methods, and publication of regional normative data would benefit the field.

The use of standardized cognitive assessments to quantify and compare child developmental outcomes is a vital component of conducting rigorous child neurodevelopmental outcome research. To assess neurodevelopment and cognitive function within a study cohort and effectively determine whether children are developing on track with their peers, researchers need to consider both the cultural suitability of standardized assessments that were developed and normed in different areas of the world as well as the prior application of these assessments in similar samples (Gladstone et al., 2017). However, validated measures or information about commonly used measures may be limited or unavailable depending upon the region in which a study is conducted. Despite the high importance of assessing child development globally, conducting research that includes participants from different countries and with different primary languages can enhance the complexity of finding cognitive measures that can be used and compared between these cohorts. While it has been acknowledged that “remarkable progress” has been made regarding child neurodevelopmental testing and research in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) in recent years (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2017), the need to accurately measure and report on child cognitive development within the context of factors specific to lower-resource settings is ongoing.

Like those in other LMICs, families in Spanish-speaking Latin America face a high burden of social inequities including infectious illness, inadequate access to proper nutrition, toxic environmental exposures, and violence, all of which place children at risk of abnormal neurodevelopment (Bitta et al., 2017; Dix-Cooper et al., 2012; Dórea, 2020; Flores-Mendoza et al., 2021; Manrique Millones et al., 2015; Schady, 2006; Velez van Meerbeke et al., 2007). Latin America has been identified by organizations such as the World Bank (see Schady, 2006), United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO, 2010), and UNICEF (Hartwig, 2020; Lumpkin, 2023) as a region requiring unique and intentional investment in early childhood care and development. As advocates, policymakers, and other stakeholders in these regions invest in high-quality programs to reduce inequity and promote nurturing care for young children, it is important to monitor cognitive outcomes to understand children’s shifting trajectories, identify unique needs of certain subgroups of children, and find opportunities for continued improvement (Hartwig, 2020; Lumpkin, 2023; UNESCO, 2010). Identifying standardized cognitive tests that are used to monitor neurodevelopment in Latin America is an important step to help researchers maximize the relevance, validity, and impact of their child outcome research in these regions (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2017).

We conducted a systematic review to determine the standardized neurodevelopmental assessments used to study child cognitive development in Spanish-speaking Latin America. We defined cognitive development broadly to include a broad range of skills that the authors of included studies defined as their measure of a neurodevelopmental or cognitive outcome. As such, these included measures of intelligence, language, nonverbal reasoning, learning, and other skills. The objective of the systematic review was to describe the most frequently used assessments in Spanish-speaking Latin America and to present an overview of how these cognitive assessments have been used in the literature to date. We hope that the systematic review will inform multinational and multiregional child cognitive outcome studies now and in the future.

Methods

A systematic review was conducted in accordance with PRISMA Guidelines for Systematic Reviews (Page et al., 2021); the final PRISMA checklist is available in Supplementary File S1. Beginning in July 2022, the authors systematically searched PubMed, PsycINFO, and ERIC databases for relevant articles. An initial search was conducted in October 2022 using database-specific search strategies and initial search terms, identified following a standard protocol as suggested by a research librarian at Children’s National Hospital, Washington, DC. Additional search terms (e.g., adding “-ing” or “-s” to terms when applicable, and listing all Spanish-speaking countries in Latin America) were later added to capture as many articles as possible, and the searches were re-run in July and August 2023. The search terms are outlined below, and the final reproducible searches for each database are available in the Supplementary Material:

  • (Cognition OR neurodevelopment OR cognitive OR neurodevelopmental OR intelligence OR comprehension OR IQ OR FSIQ) AND (test OR tests OR assessment OR assessments OR evaluation OR evaluations OR delay OR delays OR score OR scores) AND (“South America” OR “South American” OR “Latin America” OR “Latin American” OR “Central America” OR “Central American” OR Mexico OR Mexican OR Colombia OR Colombian OR Argentina OR Argentinian OR Bolivia OR Bolivian OR Chile OR Chilean OR Ecuador OR Ecuadorian OR Paraguay OR Paraguayan OR Peru OR Peruvian OR Uruguay OR Uruguayan OR Venezuela OR Venezuelan OR Panama OR Panamanian OR Nicaragua OR Nicaraguan OR Honduras OR Honduran OR Costa Rica OR Costa Rican OR Cuba OR Cuban OR Dominican OR El Salvador OR El Salvadorian OR Guatemala OR Guatemalan)

Inclusion and exclusion criteria were identical in all searches. Results were filtered to include only peer-reviewed articles published in English or Spanish from Spanish-speaking countries in Latin America. We included studies assessing children between 2 and 6 years of age to reflect the diversity in ages and experiences of children in preschool programs in Latin America (UNESCO, 2010; World Education Network, n.d.); additional database-specific filters were applied to limit results to articles assessing children within this age group.

The searches yielded 4,949 articles for further review (Figure 1). All articles were uploaded into Covidence, a systematic review management software (Covidence, n.d.). Once duplicate records were removed, all titles and abstracts were screened using Covidence (by M.E.W. and S.M.R.); another author (E.A.C.) independently resolved any conflict between the ratings. Articles were selected for inclusion if the studies directly assessed the cognitive development of children aged 2–6 years using a standardized assessment tool, or select subtest(s) of a standardized tool, in Spanish-speaking Latin American countries. We did not exclude articles based on year of publication. Systematic reviews, meta-analyses, and articles reporting only on psychometric properties or validation of assessment tools were excluded. Articles were also excluded if they only reported data on children older or younger than 2–6 years of age or utilized proxy standardized assessments only (e.g., parent questionnaires), assessed children from geographic regions outside of Spanish-speaking Latin America, or were published in a language besides English or Spanish. After the initial title and abstract screening, the authors (M.E.W., E.C., S.M.R., and R.A.-S.) retrieved the remaining articles and assessed the full text of each to determine whether it met the above-mentioned inclusion criteria. Similarly, each full text article required at least two investigators to agree on its inclusion. If multiple articles reported on data from the same cohort, only the article that was published most recently was included. Once all included articles were selected, M.E.W., E.C., S.M.R., and R.A.S. extracted relevant data on the methods of each study (i.e., study design, location, number of participants, participant age ranges, participant health conditions, and inclusion criteria; and outcome measures used to assess cognitive development) and grouped studies based on the assessments used to measure cognitive development. Initial and full-text screening and data extraction were performed in Covidence. Because the goal of this systematic review was to describe studies’ utilization of standardized assessment tools and not to harmonize their results or appraise instruments based on psychometric properties, a meta-analysis and assessment of each study’s methodological quality and risk of bias were not appropriate for this review. Study investigators were not contacted to obtain or confirm unclear or missing data due to limited resources; the authors of this review did not make assumptions about unclear or missing information and only reported data available in the full texts. This review was not registered and the protocol is not published.

PRISMA flow diagram (Page et al., 2021). PRISMA flow diagram adapted from: Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 372, n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n7.
Figure 1.

PRISMA flow diagram (Page et al., 2021). PRISMA flow diagram adapted from: Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 372, n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n7.

Results

Figure 1 shows the study selection and process of the systematic review. Our search identified 97 studies for inclusion (Table 1). Across the 97 studies, we identified 41 standardized assessments used to measure cognitive development; information about these standardized assessments is outlined in Table 2. The most widely used assessments, defined as those utilized by greater than 5 studies, are described in further detail below.

Table 1.

Studies identified in the systematic review (N = 97).

Lead author (year)Study designCountryParticipants
(N, details)
Participant age range (years)Standardized cognitive assessment(s) used for children aged 2–6 years
1. Aguilar et al. (1993)Nonrandomized experimentalMexico95 children with developmental delay due to perinatal hypoxia1–15WISC-R (Mexican version)
2. Aizawa (2021)CohortPeru (also India, Vietnam, Ethiopia)6,072 children (1,624 from Peru) in Young Lives longitudinal study1–15PPVT
3. Álvarez-González et al. (2004)CohortCuba100 children diagnosed with congenital hypothyroidism3 months–14 yearsWPPSI
4. Alvarez-Ortega et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia554 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–16K-BIT
5. Ardila & Rosselli (1994)Cross sectionalColombia233 typically developing children5–12BNT, ROCF, Token Test, WMS
6. Ardila et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia, Mexico788 typically developing children5–16ENI
7. Baker et al. (2019)CohortChile (also Australia)126 participants with fragile X syndrome and no other significant conditions1–43WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
8. Baroni et al. (2020)CohortArgentina29 children treated for infant medulloblastoma0–4Stanford–Binet, WISC-IV and V, WPPSI-III and IV
9. Bauer et al. (2015)Cross sectionalMexico33 healthy children, 18 of normal weight and 15 overweight/obese6–8ENI
10. Bautista et al. (1982)RCTBolivia200 Quechuan children with thyroid enlargement5–12Stanford–Binet
11. Bedregal et al. (2016)Cross sectionalChile1,045 children who receive healthcare in the public or private sector2–4BDI
12. Benito-Sánchez et al. (2020)Cross sectional
  • Chile, Colombia,

  • Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, Puerto Rico (also Spain)

6,030 healthy, typically developing children with an IQ of ≥80 (according to TONI-2)6–17ROCF, TAMV-I, TONI-2
13. Bernal and Fernandez (2013)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia10,173 children in a subsidized childcare program for low-income families3–6TVIP, WMB
14. Blouin et al. (2018)CohortPeru880 healthy, typically developing children1–5WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
15. Bojorque et al. (2018)RCTEcuador355 typically developing children in an experimental or regular (control) mathematics program5–6WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
16. Braun et al. (2012)CohortMexico1,035 children of low or moderate SES mothers enrolled during pregnancy1–4MSCA
17. Cando et al. (2019)Cross sectionalEcuador37 typically developing children5–6WPPSI-IV (Mexican version)
18. Caraballo et al. (2004)CohortArgentina51 children with poly-microgyria or closed-lip schizencephaly3–20Wechsler Intelligence (tests and versions not specified), Terman–Merril
19. Castro et al. (2022)Cross sectionalPeru137 typically developing children from a poor urban neighborhood6–8WMLS-S (Spanish version)
20. Caulfield et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPeru165 typically developing children of mothers in a zinc supplement trial during pregnancy54 monthsWPPSI (Spanish version)
21. Chasiotis et al. (2006)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Germany, Cameroon)

314 typically developing children (82 from urban kindergartens and nursery schools in Costa Rica)3–5WPPSI-R (Costa Rican version)
22. Chinome Torres et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalColombia38 children without sensory or motor limitations in a rural preschool3–6CUMANIN
23. Clark et al. (2006)RCTChile784 typically developing urban children without health complications at 4 months old5WPPSI-R, PLS-3, WMB-R
24. Contini de González (2000)Cross sectionalArgentina88 urban children, 44 private school and high SES and 44 public school and low SES6–12WISC-III
25. Counter et al. (1998)Cross sectionalEcuador77 children with varying levels of lead exposure4–15RCPM
26. Counter et al. (2005)Cross sectionalEcuador188 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–11RCPM
27. Cruz & Magallanes (2012)Cross sectionalMexico39 typically developing children4–6TAVE-CI
28. De los Reyes-Aragon et al. (2016)Cross sectionalColombia629 typically developing children0–5BDI
29. Diuk et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina90 low-income children with or without reading difficulties
  • Control group: 6–8

  • Others: mean age = 10 years

WISC-R, K-BIT, RPM, PPVT
30. Fernandes et al. (2023)Secondary analysis of RCT dataGuatemala (also India, Pakistan, Democratic Republic of the Congo)667 children whose mothers participated in a nutrition supplementation trial2INTER-NDA
31. Flórez et al. (1978)Cross sectionalColombia7 children hospitalized for osteoarticular lesions without serious conditions5–14GHT
32. Fox-Fuller et al. (2021)CohortColombia1,354 children with or without autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease variant6–16WISC-IV (Spanish version)
33. Friedman et al. (2020)Cross sectionalEcuador307 children with varying levels of pesticide exposure4–9NEPSY
34. Galván et al. (2014)CohortChile215 typically developing children without conditions that could affect growth4–5WPPSI-R
35. Gatica et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile94 typically developing children6–8Factor g
36. Gerez et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico75 children with behavioral, academic, or medical problems, and 20 typically developing children6–17WISC-R
37. Gomez et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalChile1,418 children who were or were not exposed to an earthquake in February 2010Mean age = 4 yearsWMLS-R, WMB-III
38. Gonzalez-Casanova et al. (2021)RCTMexico622 children of mothers enrolled in a DHA supplementation trial during pregnancy5MSCA
39. González-Cortés et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico203 children with autism spectrum disorder1–12WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
40. Grandjean et al. (2022)CohortMexico (also Canada)1,010 children (498 from Mexico) of mothers in various birth cohort studies4, 6–12MSCA, WASI (Spanish version)
41. Hermida et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina131 typically developing children who attend either a very urban or very rural school5CUMANIN, TONI-4, WISC-III, WPPSI-III
42. Herrera-Chinchay et al. (2021)CohortPeru26 children with congenital hypo-thyroidism3–11WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
43. Hill et al. (2014)Cross sectionalBolivia82 typically developing children aged 4–10, 32 adolescents, 77 adults4–10, 13–16, 25–40, 50–85CDR, RCPM, RSPM, WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
44. Jofrě et al. (2007)Cross sectionalArgentina26 healthy, typically developing children in public school6K-ABC
45. Johnston et al. (1987)CohortGuatemala459 children from a low SES urban community4–9WISC (Guatemalan modification)
46. Juric et al. (2013)Cross sectionalArgentina274 children enrolled in private school, middle-class SES6–8ENI
47. Knauer et al. (2018)CohortMexico603 children whose parent participated in a parenting program4–18 months, 3–5 yearsMSCA
48. Korzeniowski et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalArgentina178 typically developing, low- and middle-SES children from public school6–10WMB, WMB-III
49. Kozulin et al. (2010)Quasi-experimental (Chilean group was not randomized)Chile (also Italy, Israel, Belgium, and Canada)188 children with “an identified neurological developmental disturbance” and mental age of 5–74–20WISC-R, RCPM
50. Kupsco et al. (2020)CohortMexico571 children of mothers receiving prenatal care from a clinic4–6MSCA
51. Laborda et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico1,804 children from a nationally and regionally representative sample5–11RCPM
52. Lacunza et al. (2010)Cross sectionalArgentina283 children with or without diagnoses of malnutrition3–5K-ABC
53. Lamminmäki et al. (1997)Quasi- experimentalChile94 children with learning difficulties and IQ > 806–11WISC-R, WMB
54. Leyva et al. (2015)Cross sectionalChile1,868 children in public pre-Kindergarten4DCCS, PT, WMB, WMLS
55. Lohndorf et al. (2019)CohortChile77 children of either Chilean or Mapuche ethnicity1–5DCCS, TVIP
56. Lozoff et al. (2006)CohortCosta Rica161 typically developing children, with or without iron deficiencyInfant, 5, 11–14, 15–18, 19WPPSI, WMB
57. Lu et al. (2009)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 children of organic or conventional coffee farmers4–10BARS
58. Marincovich et al. (2000)Cross sectionalChile4,258 children in a regionally representative sample5–22GHT, RPM (Escala Especial)
59. Martínez-Cruz et al. (2006)Cross sectionalMexico184 children with no neurological sequelae from the Prospective Study of High-Risk Infants6–12Terman–Merril, Stanford–Binet
60. McInnes et al. (2005)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 cases with documented developmental delay and signs of autism3–13Leiter-R, MSCA, WPPSI-III, WISC-III
61. McKay et al. (1978)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia449 children without serious neurological or sensory dysfunctions; ∼92% of subnormal height and weight with clinical signs of malnutrition, low SES3–7WISC, WISC-R, WPPSI
62. Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)CohortMexico9 typically developing children with recurrent strabismus, without systemic diseases4–10WISC-R, WPPSI
63. Morales et al. (2013)Cross sectionalMexico26 children with or without hemophilia5–16ENI, WISC-RM (Mexican Revision)
64. Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico100 typically developing children of mothers who received a kidney transplant and immune-suppression drugs during pregnancy4+WISC-IV, WPPSI
65. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico27 children with nonsyndromic craniosynostosis3–5MSCA, WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
66. Nogueira et al. (2005)Cross sectionalArgentina401 typically developing children in primary school6, 12 (first and seventh grades)PMA, Terman Tests, WISC-II
67. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)CohortArgentina478 children attending an undernutrition prevention center5–17RSPM
68. Osorio-Valencia et al. (2018)CohortMexico148 children of mothers enrolled into a perinatal cohort exposed to DDT
  • 1–30 months,

  • 3 years, 5 years

MSCA
69. Otero et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico66 children with or without iron deficiency6–12WISC-R
70. Pineda et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia288 children of Paisa descent with or without ADHD6–16WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
71. Pineda et al. (2007)Cross sectionalColombia621 children with and without ADHD6–11WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
72. Pinto-Dussán et al. (2010)Cross sectionalColombia31 children without complications during pregnancy or other risk factors6–15TAVE-CI, ENI, K-ABC, WISC-IV (Spanish version)
73. Porras-Kattz et al. (2011)RCTMexico18 children with learning difficulties without epilepsy, ADHD, or visual or hearing impairments6–11WISC-RM
74. Reynolds (2022)CohortChile1,606 children whose families participated in the ELPI in 2010 and 20121–3BDI, TADI, PPVT
75. Rico et al. (2006)RCTMexico515 children who attended school near a site of lead exposure6–8PPVT, WISC-R (Mexican version)
76. Rindermann et al. (2015)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Austria)

131 children (80 from Costa Rica who attend public school)
  • 5–6, 9–11,

  • 15–16

RCPM
77. Romero et al. (2011)Cross sectionalMexico69 patients with congenital hypo-thyroidism4–10WISC-IV
78. Rosales-Rueda (2018)CohortEcuador7,989 children of low-SES families who received a cash transfer1–6PPVT
79. Rosselli et al. (2001)Cross sectionalColombia290 children without documented mental disability6–11WMB
80. Rubio-Codina et al. (2019)RCTColombia952 children without mental disabilities from low-income regions1–8WISC-IV
81. Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)RCTPeru108 typically developing children from the Programa No Escolarizado de Educación Inicial3–5GHT, WPPSI
82. Sánchez et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPanama229 children in a public elementary school6–11WISC-III
83. Sánchez-Escudero et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia223 randomly chosen children from three public schools4–13WJ
84. Schnaas et al. (2000)CohortMexico112 healthy children of healthy women3–5MSCA
85. Segretin et al. (2014)RCT and quasi-experimentalArgentina333 children in a group cognitive training program3–5NEPSY, Day-Night Stroop
86. Talero-Gutierrez et al. (2012)Cross sectionalColombia834 children with symptoms consistent with ADHD and/or learning disorders5–12BADyG
87. Torrado et al. (2007)CohortArgentina91 children with Prader-Willi syndrome0–17Terman–Merril
88. Treviño et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico, Colombia, Guatemala643 monolingual Spanish- speaking children, inclusion criteria not provided2.5–6ENI, ENI-P
89. Ugarte et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile26 Mapuche children with normal intellectual functioning and language development4–7TEVI-R
90. Urazán-Torres et al. (2013)Case-controlColombia78 children who did or did not have a conduct disorder6–12K-BIT, ENI
91. Van Wendel de Joode et al. (2016)Cross sectionalCosta Rica140 healthy children living near banana and plantain plantations6–9WISC-IV (Mexican version), ROCF
92. Varela et al. (2020)CohortChile20 children without cognitive impairments who were or were not premature5–7ENI-2
93. Vega-Dienstmaier et al. (2006)Cross sectionalPeru134 children with varying levels of lead exposure6–8GTR, KBDT
94. Vera Noriega et al. (2005)Cross sectionalMexico115 children from “extremely impoverished” communities1–5EDIN
95. Virués-Ortega et al. (2011)Cross sectionalBolivia
  • 62 healthy, typically developing children in private schools living at high altitude

6–9, 13–16WISC-IV, WMS
96. Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)RCTPeru184 typically developing children from a study by the Instituto de Investigación Nutricional3–4Day–Night Stroop, WPPSI-R (Spanish version)
97. Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)Cross sectionalMexico162 healthy, typically developing children from low-SES families4–6WPPSI (Spanish version)
Lead author (year)Study designCountryParticipants
(N, details)
Participant age range (years)Standardized cognitive assessment(s) used for children aged 2–6 years
1. Aguilar et al. (1993)Nonrandomized experimentalMexico95 children with developmental delay due to perinatal hypoxia1–15WISC-R (Mexican version)
2. Aizawa (2021)CohortPeru (also India, Vietnam, Ethiopia)6,072 children (1,624 from Peru) in Young Lives longitudinal study1–15PPVT
3. Álvarez-González et al. (2004)CohortCuba100 children diagnosed with congenital hypothyroidism3 months–14 yearsWPPSI
4. Alvarez-Ortega et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia554 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–16K-BIT
5. Ardila & Rosselli (1994)Cross sectionalColombia233 typically developing children5–12BNT, ROCF, Token Test, WMS
6. Ardila et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia, Mexico788 typically developing children5–16ENI
7. Baker et al. (2019)CohortChile (also Australia)126 participants with fragile X syndrome and no other significant conditions1–43WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
8. Baroni et al. (2020)CohortArgentina29 children treated for infant medulloblastoma0–4Stanford–Binet, WISC-IV and V, WPPSI-III and IV
9. Bauer et al. (2015)Cross sectionalMexico33 healthy children, 18 of normal weight and 15 overweight/obese6–8ENI
10. Bautista et al. (1982)RCTBolivia200 Quechuan children with thyroid enlargement5–12Stanford–Binet
11. Bedregal et al. (2016)Cross sectionalChile1,045 children who receive healthcare in the public or private sector2–4BDI
12. Benito-Sánchez et al. (2020)Cross sectional
  • Chile, Colombia,

  • Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, Puerto Rico (also Spain)

6,030 healthy, typically developing children with an IQ of ≥80 (according to TONI-2)6–17ROCF, TAMV-I, TONI-2
13. Bernal and Fernandez (2013)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia10,173 children in a subsidized childcare program for low-income families3–6TVIP, WMB
14. Blouin et al. (2018)CohortPeru880 healthy, typically developing children1–5WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
15. Bojorque et al. (2018)RCTEcuador355 typically developing children in an experimental or regular (control) mathematics program5–6WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
16. Braun et al. (2012)CohortMexico1,035 children of low or moderate SES mothers enrolled during pregnancy1–4MSCA
17. Cando et al. (2019)Cross sectionalEcuador37 typically developing children5–6WPPSI-IV (Mexican version)
18. Caraballo et al. (2004)CohortArgentina51 children with poly-microgyria or closed-lip schizencephaly3–20Wechsler Intelligence (tests and versions not specified), Terman–Merril
19. Castro et al. (2022)Cross sectionalPeru137 typically developing children from a poor urban neighborhood6–8WMLS-S (Spanish version)
20. Caulfield et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPeru165 typically developing children of mothers in a zinc supplement trial during pregnancy54 monthsWPPSI (Spanish version)
21. Chasiotis et al. (2006)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Germany, Cameroon)

314 typically developing children (82 from urban kindergartens and nursery schools in Costa Rica)3–5WPPSI-R (Costa Rican version)
22. Chinome Torres et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalColombia38 children without sensory or motor limitations in a rural preschool3–6CUMANIN
23. Clark et al. (2006)RCTChile784 typically developing urban children without health complications at 4 months old5WPPSI-R, PLS-3, WMB-R
24. Contini de González (2000)Cross sectionalArgentina88 urban children, 44 private school and high SES and 44 public school and low SES6–12WISC-III
25. Counter et al. (1998)Cross sectionalEcuador77 children with varying levels of lead exposure4–15RCPM
26. Counter et al. (2005)Cross sectionalEcuador188 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–11RCPM
27. Cruz & Magallanes (2012)Cross sectionalMexico39 typically developing children4–6TAVE-CI
28. De los Reyes-Aragon et al. (2016)Cross sectionalColombia629 typically developing children0–5BDI
29. Diuk et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina90 low-income children with or without reading difficulties
  • Control group: 6–8

  • Others: mean age = 10 years

WISC-R, K-BIT, RPM, PPVT
30. Fernandes et al. (2023)Secondary analysis of RCT dataGuatemala (also India, Pakistan, Democratic Republic of the Congo)667 children whose mothers participated in a nutrition supplementation trial2INTER-NDA
31. Flórez et al. (1978)Cross sectionalColombia7 children hospitalized for osteoarticular lesions without serious conditions5–14GHT
32. Fox-Fuller et al. (2021)CohortColombia1,354 children with or without autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease variant6–16WISC-IV (Spanish version)
33. Friedman et al. (2020)Cross sectionalEcuador307 children with varying levels of pesticide exposure4–9NEPSY
34. Galván et al. (2014)CohortChile215 typically developing children without conditions that could affect growth4–5WPPSI-R
35. Gatica et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile94 typically developing children6–8Factor g
36. Gerez et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico75 children with behavioral, academic, or medical problems, and 20 typically developing children6–17WISC-R
37. Gomez et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalChile1,418 children who were or were not exposed to an earthquake in February 2010Mean age = 4 yearsWMLS-R, WMB-III
38. Gonzalez-Casanova et al. (2021)RCTMexico622 children of mothers enrolled in a DHA supplementation trial during pregnancy5MSCA
39. González-Cortés et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico203 children with autism spectrum disorder1–12WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
40. Grandjean et al. (2022)CohortMexico (also Canada)1,010 children (498 from Mexico) of mothers in various birth cohort studies4, 6–12MSCA, WASI (Spanish version)
41. Hermida et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina131 typically developing children who attend either a very urban or very rural school5CUMANIN, TONI-4, WISC-III, WPPSI-III
42. Herrera-Chinchay et al. (2021)CohortPeru26 children with congenital hypo-thyroidism3–11WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
43. Hill et al. (2014)Cross sectionalBolivia82 typically developing children aged 4–10, 32 adolescents, 77 adults4–10, 13–16, 25–40, 50–85CDR, RCPM, RSPM, WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
44. Jofrě et al. (2007)Cross sectionalArgentina26 healthy, typically developing children in public school6K-ABC
45. Johnston et al. (1987)CohortGuatemala459 children from a low SES urban community4–9WISC (Guatemalan modification)
46. Juric et al. (2013)Cross sectionalArgentina274 children enrolled in private school, middle-class SES6–8ENI
47. Knauer et al. (2018)CohortMexico603 children whose parent participated in a parenting program4–18 months, 3–5 yearsMSCA
48. Korzeniowski et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalArgentina178 typically developing, low- and middle-SES children from public school6–10WMB, WMB-III
49. Kozulin et al. (2010)Quasi-experimental (Chilean group was not randomized)Chile (also Italy, Israel, Belgium, and Canada)188 children with “an identified neurological developmental disturbance” and mental age of 5–74–20WISC-R, RCPM
50. Kupsco et al. (2020)CohortMexico571 children of mothers receiving prenatal care from a clinic4–6MSCA
51. Laborda et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico1,804 children from a nationally and regionally representative sample5–11RCPM
52. Lacunza et al. (2010)Cross sectionalArgentina283 children with or without diagnoses of malnutrition3–5K-ABC
53. Lamminmäki et al. (1997)Quasi- experimentalChile94 children with learning difficulties and IQ > 806–11WISC-R, WMB
54. Leyva et al. (2015)Cross sectionalChile1,868 children in public pre-Kindergarten4DCCS, PT, WMB, WMLS
55. Lohndorf et al. (2019)CohortChile77 children of either Chilean or Mapuche ethnicity1–5DCCS, TVIP
56. Lozoff et al. (2006)CohortCosta Rica161 typically developing children, with or without iron deficiencyInfant, 5, 11–14, 15–18, 19WPPSI, WMB
57. Lu et al. (2009)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 children of organic or conventional coffee farmers4–10BARS
58. Marincovich et al. (2000)Cross sectionalChile4,258 children in a regionally representative sample5–22GHT, RPM (Escala Especial)
59. Martínez-Cruz et al. (2006)Cross sectionalMexico184 children with no neurological sequelae from the Prospective Study of High-Risk Infants6–12Terman–Merril, Stanford–Binet
60. McInnes et al. (2005)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 cases with documented developmental delay and signs of autism3–13Leiter-R, MSCA, WPPSI-III, WISC-III
61. McKay et al. (1978)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia449 children without serious neurological or sensory dysfunctions; ∼92% of subnormal height and weight with clinical signs of malnutrition, low SES3–7WISC, WISC-R, WPPSI
62. Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)CohortMexico9 typically developing children with recurrent strabismus, without systemic diseases4–10WISC-R, WPPSI
63. Morales et al. (2013)Cross sectionalMexico26 children with or without hemophilia5–16ENI, WISC-RM (Mexican Revision)
64. Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico100 typically developing children of mothers who received a kidney transplant and immune-suppression drugs during pregnancy4+WISC-IV, WPPSI
65. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico27 children with nonsyndromic craniosynostosis3–5MSCA, WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
66. Nogueira et al. (2005)Cross sectionalArgentina401 typically developing children in primary school6, 12 (first and seventh grades)PMA, Terman Tests, WISC-II
67. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)CohortArgentina478 children attending an undernutrition prevention center5–17RSPM
68. Osorio-Valencia et al. (2018)CohortMexico148 children of mothers enrolled into a perinatal cohort exposed to DDT
  • 1–30 months,

  • 3 years, 5 years

MSCA
69. Otero et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico66 children with or without iron deficiency6–12WISC-R
70. Pineda et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia288 children of Paisa descent with or without ADHD6–16WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
71. Pineda et al. (2007)Cross sectionalColombia621 children with and without ADHD6–11WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
72. Pinto-Dussán et al. (2010)Cross sectionalColombia31 children without complications during pregnancy or other risk factors6–15TAVE-CI, ENI, K-ABC, WISC-IV (Spanish version)
73. Porras-Kattz et al. (2011)RCTMexico18 children with learning difficulties without epilepsy, ADHD, or visual or hearing impairments6–11WISC-RM
74. Reynolds (2022)CohortChile1,606 children whose families participated in the ELPI in 2010 and 20121–3BDI, TADI, PPVT
75. Rico et al. (2006)RCTMexico515 children who attended school near a site of lead exposure6–8PPVT, WISC-R (Mexican version)
76. Rindermann et al. (2015)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Austria)

131 children (80 from Costa Rica who attend public school)
  • 5–6, 9–11,

  • 15–16

RCPM
77. Romero et al. (2011)Cross sectionalMexico69 patients with congenital hypo-thyroidism4–10WISC-IV
78. Rosales-Rueda (2018)CohortEcuador7,989 children of low-SES families who received a cash transfer1–6PPVT
79. Rosselli et al. (2001)Cross sectionalColombia290 children without documented mental disability6–11WMB
80. Rubio-Codina et al. (2019)RCTColombia952 children without mental disabilities from low-income regions1–8WISC-IV
81. Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)RCTPeru108 typically developing children from the Programa No Escolarizado de Educación Inicial3–5GHT, WPPSI
82. Sánchez et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPanama229 children in a public elementary school6–11WISC-III
83. Sánchez-Escudero et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia223 randomly chosen children from three public schools4–13WJ
84. Schnaas et al. (2000)CohortMexico112 healthy children of healthy women3–5MSCA
85. Segretin et al. (2014)RCT and quasi-experimentalArgentina333 children in a group cognitive training program3–5NEPSY, Day-Night Stroop
86. Talero-Gutierrez et al. (2012)Cross sectionalColombia834 children with symptoms consistent with ADHD and/or learning disorders5–12BADyG
87. Torrado et al. (2007)CohortArgentina91 children with Prader-Willi syndrome0–17Terman–Merril
88. Treviño et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico, Colombia, Guatemala643 monolingual Spanish- speaking children, inclusion criteria not provided2.5–6ENI, ENI-P
89. Ugarte et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile26 Mapuche children with normal intellectual functioning and language development4–7TEVI-R
90. Urazán-Torres et al. (2013)Case-controlColombia78 children who did or did not have a conduct disorder6–12K-BIT, ENI
91. Van Wendel de Joode et al. (2016)Cross sectionalCosta Rica140 healthy children living near banana and plantain plantations6–9WISC-IV (Mexican version), ROCF
92. Varela et al. (2020)CohortChile20 children without cognitive impairments who were or were not premature5–7ENI-2
93. Vega-Dienstmaier et al. (2006)Cross sectionalPeru134 children with varying levels of lead exposure6–8GTR, KBDT
94. Vera Noriega et al. (2005)Cross sectionalMexico115 children from “extremely impoverished” communities1–5EDIN
95. Virués-Ortega et al. (2011)Cross sectionalBolivia
  • 62 healthy, typically developing children in private schools living at high altitude

6–9, 13–16WISC-IV, WMS
96. Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)RCTPeru184 typically developing children from a study by the Instituto de Investigación Nutricional3–4Day–Night Stroop, WPPSI-R (Spanish version)
97. Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)Cross sectionalMexico162 healthy, typically developing children from low-SES families4–6WPPSI (Spanish version)

Note. BADyG = Batería de Aptitudes Diferenciales y Generales (General and Differential Aptitudes Battery); BARS = The Behavioral Assessment and Research System; BDI = Battelle Developmental Inventory Screening Test; BNT = Boston Naming Test; CDR = Cognitive Drug Research Battery; CUMANIN = Cuestionario de Madurez Neuropsicológica Infantil (Questionnaire for Children’s Neuropsychological Maturity); DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; EDIN = Escala del Desarrollo Integral del Niño (Scale of Integral Child Development); ENI = Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil (Child Neuropsychological Assessment); GHT = Goodenough–Harris Draw-a-Man Test; GTR = Graphic Test of Reasoning; INTER-NDA = INTERGROWTH-21st Neurodevelopment Assessment; K-ABC = Kaufman Brief Assessment for Children; KBDT = Kohs Block Design Test; K-BIT = Kaufman’s Brief Intelligence Test; MSCA = McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities; NEPSY = A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment; PLS-3 = Preschool Language Scale-3; PMA = Thurstone’s Primary Mental Abilities Test; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; PT = Pencil Tapping; RCPM = Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices; RPM = Raven’s Progressive Matrices; RSPM = Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices; ROCF = Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; TADI = Test de Aprendizaje y Desarrollo Infantil; TAVE-CI = Test de Aprendizaje Verbal. España-Complutense Infantil (Verbal Learning Test); TEVI: Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes; TVIP = Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (Spanish version of PPVT); TONI = Test of Nonverbal Intelligence; WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WJ = Woodcock–Johnson; WMB = Woodcock–Muñoz Battery; WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale; WMLS = Woodcock–Muñoz Language Survey; WPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence.

Table 1.

Studies identified in the systematic review (N = 97).

Lead author (year)Study designCountryParticipants
(N, details)
Participant age range (years)Standardized cognitive assessment(s) used for children aged 2–6 years
1. Aguilar et al. (1993)Nonrandomized experimentalMexico95 children with developmental delay due to perinatal hypoxia1–15WISC-R (Mexican version)
2. Aizawa (2021)CohortPeru (also India, Vietnam, Ethiopia)6,072 children (1,624 from Peru) in Young Lives longitudinal study1–15PPVT
3. Álvarez-González et al. (2004)CohortCuba100 children diagnosed with congenital hypothyroidism3 months–14 yearsWPPSI
4. Alvarez-Ortega et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia554 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–16K-BIT
5. Ardila & Rosselli (1994)Cross sectionalColombia233 typically developing children5–12BNT, ROCF, Token Test, WMS
6. Ardila et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia, Mexico788 typically developing children5–16ENI
7. Baker et al. (2019)CohortChile (also Australia)126 participants with fragile X syndrome and no other significant conditions1–43WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
8. Baroni et al. (2020)CohortArgentina29 children treated for infant medulloblastoma0–4Stanford–Binet, WISC-IV and V, WPPSI-III and IV
9. Bauer et al. (2015)Cross sectionalMexico33 healthy children, 18 of normal weight and 15 overweight/obese6–8ENI
10. Bautista et al. (1982)RCTBolivia200 Quechuan children with thyroid enlargement5–12Stanford–Binet
11. Bedregal et al. (2016)Cross sectionalChile1,045 children who receive healthcare in the public or private sector2–4BDI
12. Benito-Sánchez et al. (2020)Cross sectional
  • Chile, Colombia,

  • Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, Puerto Rico (also Spain)

6,030 healthy, typically developing children with an IQ of ≥80 (according to TONI-2)6–17ROCF, TAMV-I, TONI-2
13. Bernal and Fernandez (2013)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia10,173 children in a subsidized childcare program for low-income families3–6TVIP, WMB
14. Blouin et al. (2018)CohortPeru880 healthy, typically developing children1–5WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
15. Bojorque et al. (2018)RCTEcuador355 typically developing children in an experimental or regular (control) mathematics program5–6WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
16. Braun et al. (2012)CohortMexico1,035 children of low or moderate SES mothers enrolled during pregnancy1–4MSCA
17. Cando et al. (2019)Cross sectionalEcuador37 typically developing children5–6WPPSI-IV (Mexican version)
18. Caraballo et al. (2004)CohortArgentina51 children with poly-microgyria or closed-lip schizencephaly3–20Wechsler Intelligence (tests and versions not specified), Terman–Merril
19. Castro et al. (2022)Cross sectionalPeru137 typically developing children from a poor urban neighborhood6–8WMLS-S (Spanish version)
20. Caulfield et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPeru165 typically developing children of mothers in a zinc supplement trial during pregnancy54 monthsWPPSI (Spanish version)
21. Chasiotis et al. (2006)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Germany, Cameroon)

314 typically developing children (82 from urban kindergartens and nursery schools in Costa Rica)3–5WPPSI-R (Costa Rican version)
22. Chinome Torres et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalColombia38 children without sensory or motor limitations in a rural preschool3–6CUMANIN
23. Clark et al. (2006)RCTChile784 typically developing urban children without health complications at 4 months old5WPPSI-R, PLS-3, WMB-R
24. Contini de González (2000)Cross sectionalArgentina88 urban children, 44 private school and high SES and 44 public school and low SES6–12WISC-III
25. Counter et al. (1998)Cross sectionalEcuador77 children with varying levels of lead exposure4–15RCPM
26. Counter et al. (2005)Cross sectionalEcuador188 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–11RCPM
27. Cruz & Magallanes (2012)Cross sectionalMexico39 typically developing children4–6TAVE-CI
28. De los Reyes-Aragon et al. (2016)Cross sectionalColombia629 typically developing children0–5BDI
29. Diuk et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina90 low-income children with or without reading difficulties
  • Control group: 6–8

  • Others: mean age = 10 years

WISC-R, K-BIT, RPM, PPVT
30. Fernandes et al. (2023)Secondary analysis of RCT dataGuatemala (also India, Pakistan, Democratic Republic of the Congo)667 children whose mothers participated in a nutrition supplementation trial2INTER-NDA
31. Flórez et al. (1978)Cross sectionalColombia7 children hospitalized for osteoarticular lesions without serious conditions5–14GHT
32. Fox-Fuller et al. (2021)CohortColombia1,354 children with or without autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease variant6–16WISC-IV (Spanish version)
33. Friedman et al. (2020)Cross sectionalEcuador307 children with varying levels of pesticide exposure4–9NEPSY
34. Galván et al. (2014)CohortChile215 typically developing children without conditions that could affect growth4–5WPPSI-R
35. Gatica et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile94 typically developing children6–8Factor g
36. Gerez et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico75 children with behavioral, academic, or medical problems, and 20 typically developing children6–17WISC-R
37. Gomez et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalChile1,418 children who were or were not exposed to an earthquake in February 2010Mean age = 4 yearsWMLS-R, WMB-III
38. Gonzalez-Casanova et al. (2021)RCTMexico622 children of mothers enrolled in a DHA supplementation trial during pregnancy5MSCA
39. González-Cortés et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico203 children with autism spectrum disorder1–12WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
40. Grandjean et al. (2022)CohortMexico (also Canada)1,010 children (498 from Mexico) of mothers in various birth cohort studies4, 6–12MSCA, WASI (Spanish version)
41. Hermida et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina131 typically developing children who attend either a very urban or very rural school5CUMANIN, TONI-4, WISC-III, WPPSI-III
42. Herrera-Chinchay et al. (2021)CohortPeru26 children with congenital hypo-thyroidism3–11WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
43. Hill et al. (2014)Cross sectionalBolivia82 typically developing children aged 4–10, 32 adolescents, 77 adults4–10, 13–16, 25–40, 50–85CDR, RCPM, RSPM, WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
44. Jofrě et al. (2007)Cross sectionalArgentina26 healthy, typically developing children in public school6K-ABC
45. Johnston et al. (1987)CohortGuatemala459 children from a low SES urban community4–9WISC (Guatemalan modification)
46. Juric et al. (2013)Cross sectionalArgentina274 children enrolled in private school, middle-class SES6–8ENI
47. Knauer et al. (2018)CohortMexico603 children whose parent participated in a parenting program4–18 months, 3–5 yearsMSCA
48. Korzeniowski et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalArgentina178 typically developing, low- and middle-SES children from public school6–10WMB, WMB-III
49. Kozulin et al. (2010)Quasi-experimental (Chilean group was not randomized)Chile (also Italy, Israel, Belgium, and Canada)188 children with “an identified neurological developmental disturbance” and mental age of 5–74–20WISC-R, RCPM
50. Kupsco et al. (2020)CohortMexico571 children of mothers receiving prenatal care from a clinic4–6MSCA
51. Laborda et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico1,804 children from a nationally and regionally representative sample5–11RCPM
52. Lacunza et al. (2010)Cross sectionalArgentina283 children with or without diagnoses of malnutrition3–5K-ABC
53. Lamminmäki et al. (1997)Quasi- experimentalChile94 children with learning difficulties and IQ > 806–11WISC-R, WMB
54. Leyva et al. (2015)Cross sectionalChile1,868 children in public pre-Kindergarten4DCCS, PT, WMB, WMLS
55. Lohndorf et al. (2019)CohortChile77 children of either Chilean or Mapuche ethnicity1–5DCCS, TVIP
56. Lozoff et al. (2006)CohortCosta Rica161 typically developing children, with or without iron deficiencyInfant, 5, 11–14, 15–18, 19WPPSI, WMB
57. Lu et al. (2009)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 children of organic or conventional coffee farmers4–10BARS
58. Marincovich et al. (2000)Cross sectionalChile4,258 children in a regionally representative sample5–22GHT, RPM (Escala Especial)
59. Martínez-Cruz et al. (2006)Cross sectionalMexico184 children with no neurological sequelae from the Prospective Study of High-Risk Infants6–12Terman–Merril, Stanford–Binet
60. McInnes et al. (2005)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 cases with documented developmental delay and signs of autism3–13Leiter-R, MSCA, WPPSI-III, WISC-III
61. McKay et al. (1978)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia449 children without serious neurological or sensory dysfunctions; ∼92% of subnormal height and weight with clinical signs of malnutrition, low SES3–7WISC, WISC-R, WPPSI
62. Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)CohortMexico9 typically developing children with recurrent strabismus, without systemic diseases4–10WISC-R, WPPSI
63. Morales et al. (2013)Cross sectionalMexico26 children with or without hemophilia5–16ENI, WISC-RM (Mexican Revision)
64. Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico100 typically developing children of mothers who received a kidney transplant and immune-suppression drugs during pregnancy4+WISC-IV, WPPSI
65. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico27 children with nonsyndromic craniosynostosis3–5MSCA, WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
66. Nogueira et al. (2005)Cross sectionalArgentina401 typically developing children in primary school6, 12 (first and seventh grades)PMA, Terman Tests, WISC-II
67. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)CohortArgentina478 children attending an undernutrition prevention center5–17RSPM
68. Osorio-Valencia et al. (2018)CohortMexico148 children of mothers enrolled into a perinatal cohort exposed to DDT
  • 1–30 months,

  • 3 years, 5 years

MSCA
69. Otero et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico66 children with or without iron deficiency6–12WISC-R
70. Pineda et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia288 children of Paisa descent with or without ADHD6–16WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
71. Pineda et al. (2007)Cross sectionalColombia621 children with and without ADHD6–11WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
72. Pinto-Dussán et al. (2010)Cross sectionalColombia31 children without complications during pregnancy or other risk factors6–15TAVE-CI, ENI, K-ABC, WISC-IV (Spanish version)
73. Porras-Kattz et al. (2011)RCTMexico18 children with learning difficulties without epilepsy, ADHD, or visual or hearing impairments6–11WISC-RM
74. Reynolds (2022)CohortChile1,606 children whose families participated in the ELPI in 2010 and 20121–3BDI, TADI, PPVT
75. Rico et al. (2006)RCTMexico515 children who attended school near a site of lead exposure6–8PPVT, WISC-R (Mexican version)
76. Rindermann et al. (2015)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Austria)

131 children (80 from Costa Rica who attend public school)
  • 5–6, 9–11,

  • 15–16

RCPM
77. Romero et al. (2011)Cross sectionalMexico69 patients with congenital hypo-thyroidism4–10WISC-IV
78. Rosales-Rueda (2018)CohortEcuador7,989 children of low-SES families who received a cash transfer1–6PPVT
79. Rosselli et al. (2001)Cross sectionalColombia290 children without documented mental disability6–11WMB
80. Rubio-Codina et al. (2019)RCTColombia952 children without mental disabilities from low-income regions1–8WISC-IV
81. Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)RCTPeru108 typically developing children from the Programa No Escolarizado de Educación Inicial3–5GHT, WPPSI
82. Sánchez et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPanama229 children in a public elementary school6–11WISC-III
83. Sánchez-Escudero et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia223 randomly chosen children from three public schools4–13WJ
84. Schnaas et al. (2000)CohortMexico112 healthy children of healthy women3–5MSCA
85. Segretin et al. (2014)RCT and quasi-experimentalArgentina333 children in a group cognitive training program3–5NEPSY, Day-Night Stroop
86. Talero-Gutierrez et al. (2012)Cross sectionalColombia834 children with symptoms consistent with ADHD and/or learning disorders5–12BADyG
87. Torrado et al. (2007)CohortArgentina91 children with Prader-Willi syndrome0–17Terman–Merril
88. Treviño et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico, Colombia, Guatemala643 monolingual Spanish- speaking children, inclusion criteria not provided2.5–6ENI, ENI-P
89. Ugarte et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile26 Mapuche children with normal intellectual functioning and language development4–7TEVI-R
90. Urazán-Torres et al. (2013)Case-controlColombia78 children who did or did not have a conduct disorder6–12K-BIT, ENI
91. Van Wendel de Joode et al. (2016)Cross sectionalCosta Rica140 healthy children living near banana and plantain plantations6–9WISC-IV (Mexican version), ROCF
92. Varela et al. (2020)CohortChile20 children without cognitive impairments who were or were not premature5–7ENI-2
93. Vega-Dienstmaier et al. (2006)Cross sectionalPeru134 children with varying levels of lead exposure6–8GTR, KBDT
94. Vera Noriega et al. (2005)Cross sectionalMexico115 children from “extremely impoverished” communities1–5EDIN
95. Virués-Ortega et al. (2011)Cross sectionalBolivia
  • 62 healthy, typically developing children in private schools living at high altitude

6–9, 13–16WISC-IV, WMS
96. Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)RCTPeru184 typically developing children from a study by the Instituto de Investigación Nutricional3–4Day–Night Stroop, WPPSI-R (Spanish version)
97. Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)Cross sectionalMexico162 healthy, typically developing children from low-SES families4–6WPPSI (Spanish version)
Lead author (year)Study designCountryParticipants
(N, details)
Participant age range (years)Standardized cognitive assessment(s) used for children aged 2–6 years
1. Aguilar et al. (1993)Nonrandomized experimentalMexico95 children with developmental delay due to perinatal hypoxia1–15WISC-R (Mexican version)
2. Aizawa (2021)CohortPeru (also India, Vietnam, Ethiopia)6,072 children (1,624 from Peru) in Young Lives longitudinal study1–15PPVT
3. Álvarez-González et al. (2004)CohortCuba100 children diagnosed with congenital hypothyroidism3 months–14 yearsWPPSI
4. Alvarez-Ortega et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia554 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–16K-BIT
5. Ardila & Rosselli (1994)Cross sectionalColombia233 typically developing children5–12BNT, ROCF, Token Test, WMS
6. Ardila et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia, Mexico788 typically developing children5–16ENI
7. Baker et al. (2019)CohortChile (also Australia)126 participants with fragile X syndrome and no other significant conditions1–43WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
8. Baroni et al. (2020)CohortArgentina29 children treated for infant medulloblastoma0–4Stanford–Binet, WISC-IV and V, WPPSI-III and IV
9. Bauer et al. (2015)Cross sectionalMexico33 healthy children, 18 of normal weight and 15 overweight/obese6–8ENI
10. Bautista et al. (1982)RCTBolivia200 Quechuan children with thyroid enlargement5–12Stanford–Binet
11. Bedregal et al. (2016)Cross sectionalChile1,045 children who receive healthcare in the public or private sector2–4BDI
12. Benito-Sánchez et al. (2020)Cross sectional
  • Chile, Colombia,

  • Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, Puerto Rico (also Spain)

6,030 healthy, typically developing children with an IQ of ≥80 (according to TONI-2)6–17ROCF, TAMV-I, TONI-2
13. Bernal and Fernandez (2013)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia10,173 children in a subsidized childcare program for low-income families3–6TVIP, WMB
14. Blouin et al. (2018)CohortPeru880 healthy, typically developing children1–5WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
15. Bojorque et al. (2018)RCTEcuador355 typically developing children in an experimental or regular (control) mathematics program5–6WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
16. Braun et al. (2012)CohortMexico1,035 children of low or moderate SES mothers enrolled during pregnancy1–4MSCA
17. Cando et al. (2019)Cross sectionalEcuador37 typically developing children5–6WPPSI-IV (Mexican version)
18. Caraballo et al. (2004)CohortArgentina51 children with poly-microgyria or closed-lip schizencephaly3–20Wechsler Intelligence (tests and versions not specified), Terman–Merril
19. Castro et al. (2022)Cross sectionalPeru137 typically developing children from a poor urban neighborhood6–8WMLS-S (Spanish version)
20. Caulfield et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPeru165 typically developing children of mothers in a zinc supplement trial during pregnancy54 monthsWPPSI (Spanish version)
21. Chasiotis et al. (2006)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Germany, Cameroon)

314 typically developing children (82 from urban kindergartens and nursery schools in Costa Rica)3–5WPPSI-R (Costa Rican version)
22. Chinome Torres et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalColombia38 children without sensory or motor limitations in a rural preschool3–6CUMANIN
23. Clark et al. (2006)RCTChile784 typically developing urban children without health complications at 4 months old5WPPSI-R, PLS-3, WMB-R
24. Contini de González (2000)Cross sectionalArgentina88 urban children, 44 private school and high SES and 44 public school and low SES6–12WISC-III
25. Counter et al. (1998)Cross sectionalEcuador77 children with varying levels of lead exposure4–15RCPM
26. Counter et al. (2005)Cross sectionalEcuador188 children with varying levels of lead exposure5–11RCPM
27. Cruz & Magallanes (2012)Cross sectionalMexico39 typically developing children4–6TAVE-CI
28. De los Reyes-Aragon et al. (2016)Cross sectionalColombia629 typically developing children0–5BDI
29. Diuk et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina90 low-income children with or without reading difficulties
  • Control group: 6–8

  • Others: mean age = 10 years

WISC-R, K-BIT, RPM, PPVT
30. Fernandes et al. (2023)Secondary analysis of RCT dataGuatemala (also India, Pakistan, Democratic Republic of the Congo)667 children whose mothers participated in a nutrition supplementation trial2INTER-NDA
31. Flórez et al. (1978)Cross sectionalColombia7 children hospitalized for osteoarticular lesions without serious conditions5–14GHT
32. Fox-Fuller et al. (2021)CohortColombia1,354 children with or without autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease variant6–16WISC-IV (Spanish version)
33. Friedman et al. (2020)Cross sectionalEcuador307 children with varying levels of pesticide exposure4–9NEPSY
34. Galván et al. (2014)CohortChile215 typically developing children without conditions that could affect growth4–5WPPSI-R
35. Gatica et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile94 typically developing children6–8Factor g
36. Gerez et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico75 children with behavioral, academic, or medical problems, and 20 typically developing children6–17WISC-R
37. Gomez et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalChile1,418 children who were or were not exposed to an earthquake in February 2010Mean age = 4 yearsWMLS-R, WMB-III
38. Gonzalez-Casanova et al. (2021)RCTMexico622 children of mothers enrolled in a DHA supplementation trial during pregnancy5MSCA
39. González-Cortés et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico203 children with autism spectrum disorder1–12WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
40. Grandjean et al. (2022)CohortMexico (also Canada)1,010 children (498 from Mexico) of mothers in various birth cohort studies4, 6–12MSCA, WASI (Spanish version)
41. Hermida et al. (2019)Cross sectionalArgentina131 typically developing children who attend either a very urban or very rural school5CUMANIN, TONI-4, WISC-III, WPPSI-III
42. Herrera-Chinchay et al. (2021)CohortPeru26 children with congenital hypo-thyroidism3–11WISC-IV, WPPSI-IV
43. Hill et al. (2014)Cross sectionalBolivia82 typically developing children aged 4–10, 32 adolescents, 77 adults4–10, 13–16, 25–40, 50–85CDR, RCPM, RSPM, WPPSI-III (Spanish version)
44. Jofrě et al. (2007)Cross sectionalArgentina26 healthy, typically developing children in public school6K-ABC
45. Johnston et al. (1987)CohortGuatemala459 children from a low SES urban community4–9WISC (Guatemalan modification)
46. Juric et al. (2013)Cross sectionalArgentina274 children enrolled in private school, middle-class SES6–8ENI
47. Knauer et al. (2018)CohortMexico603 children whose parent participated in a parenting program4–18 months, 3–5 yearsMSCA
48. Korzeniowski et al. (2017)Quasi-experimentalArgentina178 typically developing, low- and middle-SES children from public school6–10WMB, WMB-III
49. Kozulin et al. (2010)Quasi-experimental (Chilean group was not randomized)Chile (also Italy, Israel, Belgium, and Canada)188 children with “an identified neurological developmental disturbance” and mental age of 5–74–20WISC-R, RCPM
50. Kupsco et al. (2020)CohortMexico571 children of mothers receiving prenatal care from a clinic4–6MSCA
51. Laborda et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico1,804 children from a nationally and regionally representative sample5–11RCPM
52. Lacunza et al. (2010)Cross sectionalArgentina283 children with or without diagnoses of malnutrition3–5K-ABC
53. Lamminmäki et al. (1997)Quasi- experimentalChile94 children with learning difficulties and IQ > 806–11WISC-R, WMB
54. Leyva et al. (2015)Cross sectionalChile1,868 children in public pre-Kindergarten4DCCS, PT, WMB, WMLS
55. Lohndorf et al. (2019)CohortChile77 children of either Chilean or Mapuche ethnicity1–5DCCS, TVIP
56. Lozoff et al. (2006)CohortCosta Rica161 typically developing children, with or without iron deficiencyInfant, 5, 11–14, 15–18, 19WPPSI, WMB
57. Lu et al. (2009)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 children of organic or conventional coffee farmers4–10BARS
58. Marincovich et al. (2000)Cross sectionalChile4,258 children in a regionally representative sample5–22GHT, RPM (Escala Especial)
59. Martínez-Cruz et al. (2006)Cross sectionalMexico184 children with no neurological sequelae from the Prospective Study of High-Risk Infants6–12Terman–Merril, Stanford–Binet
60. McInnes et al. (2005)Cross sectionalCosta Rica35 cases with documented developmental delay and signs of autism3–13Leiter-R, MSCA, WPPSI-III, WISC-III
61. McKay et al. (1978)Nonrandomized experimentalColombia449 children without serious neurological or sensory dysfunctions; ∼92% of subnormal height and weight with clinical signs of malnutrition, low SES3–7WISC, WISC-R, WPPSI
62. Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)CohortMexico9 typically developing children with recurrent strabismus, without systemic diseases4–10WISC-R, WPPSI
63. Morales et al. (2013)Cross sectionalMexico26 children with or without hemophilia5–16ENI, WISC-RM (Mexican Revision)
64. Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)Cross sectionalMexico100 typically developing children of mothers who received a kidney transplant and immune-suppression drugs during pregnancy4+WISC-IV, WPPSI
65. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico27 children with nonsyndromic craniosynostosis3–5MSCA, WPPSI-III (Mexican version)
66. Nogueira et al. (2005)Cross sectionalArgentina401 typically developing children in primary school6, 12 (first and seventh grades)PMA, Terman Tests, WISC-II
67. Moreno-Villagómez et al. (2021)CohortArgentina478 children attending an undernutrition prevention center5–17RSPM
68. Osorio-Valencia et al. (2018)CohortMexico148 children of mothers enrolled into a perinatal cohort exposed to DDT
  • 1–30 months,

  • 3 years, 5 years

MSCA
69. Otero et al. (1999)Cross sectionalMexico66 children with or without iron deficiency6–12WISC-R
70. Pineda et al. (2011)Cross sectionalColombia288 children of Paisa descent with or without ADHD6–16WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
71. Pineda et al. (2007)Cross sectionalColombia621 children with and without ADHD6–11WISC-R, WMS, ROCF, Token Test
72. Pinto-Dussán et al. (2010)Cross sectionalColombia31 children without complications during pregnancy or other risk factors6–15TAVE-CI, ENI, K-ABC, WISC-IV (Spanish version)
73. Porras-Kattz et al. (2011)RCTMexico18 children with learning difficulties without epilepsy, ADHD, or visual or hearing impairments6–11WISC-RM
74. Reynolds (2022)CohortChile1,606 children whose families participated in the ELPI in 2010 and 20121–3BDI, TADI, PPVT
75. Rico et al. (2006)RCTMexico515 children who attended school near a site of lead exposure6–8PPVT, WISC-R (Mexican version)
76. Rindermann et al. (2015)Cross sectional
  • Costa Rica

  • (also Austria)

131 children (80 from Costa Rica who attend public school)
  • 5–6, 9–11,

  • 15–16

RCPM
77. Romero et al. (2011)Cross sectionalMexico69 patients with congenital hypo-thyroidism4–10WISC-IV
78. Rosales-Rueda (2018)CohortEcuador7,989 children of low-SES families who received a cash transfer1–6PPVT
79. Rosselli et al. (2001)Cross sectionalColombia290 children without documented mental disability6–11WMB
80. Rubio-Codina et al. (2019)RCTColombia952 children without mental disabilities from low-income regions1–8WISC-IV
81. Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)RCTPeru108 typically developing children from the Programa No Escolarizado de Educación Inicial3–5GHT, WPPSI
82. Sánchez et al. (2011)Cross sectionalPanama229 children in a public elementary school6–11WISC-III
83. Sánchez-Escudero et al. (2019)Cross sectionalColombia223 randomly chosen children from three public schools4–13WJ
84. Schnaas et al. (2000)CohortMexico112 healthy children of healthy women3–5MSCA
85. Segretin et al. (2014)RCT and quasi-experimentalArgentina333 children in a group cognitive training program3–5NEPSY, Day-Night Stroop
86. Talero-Gutierrez et al. (2012)Cross sectionalColombia834 children with symptoms consistent with ADHD and/or learning disorders5–12BADyG
87. Torrado et al. (2007)CohortArgentina91 children with Prader-Willi syndrome0–17Terman–Merril
88. Treviño et al. (2021)Cross sectionalMexico, Colombia, Guatemala643 monolingual Spanish- speaking children, inclusion criteria not provided2.5–6ENI, ENI-P
89. Ugarte et al. (2019)Cross sectionalChile26 Mapuche children with normal intellectual functioning and language development4–7TEVI-R
90. Urazán-Torres et al. (2013)Case-controlColombia78 children who did or did not have a conduct disorder6–12K-BIT, ENI
91. Van Wendel de Joode et al. (2016)Cross sectionalCosta Rica140 healthy children living near banana and plantain plantations6–9WISC-IV (Mexican version), ROCF
92. Varela et al. (2020)CohortChile20 children without cognitive impairments who were or were not premature5–7ENI-2
93. Vega-Dienstmaier et al. (2006)Cross sectionalPeru134 children with varying levels of lead exposure6–8GTR, KBDT
94. Vera Noriega et al. (2005)Cross sectionalMexico115 children from “extremely impoverished” communities1–5EDIN
95. Virués-Ortega et al. (2011)Cross sectionalBolivia
  • 62 healthy, typically developing children in private schools living at high altitude

6–9, 13–16WISC-IV, WMS
96. Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)RCTPeru184 typically developing children from a study by the Instituto de Investigación Nutricional3–4Day–Night Stroop, WPPSI-R (Spanish version)
97. Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)Cross sectionalMexico162 healthy, typically developing children from low-SES families4–6WPPSI (Spanish version)

Note. BADyG = Batería de Aptitudes Diferenciales y Generales (General and Differential Aptitudes Battery); BARS = The Behavioral Assessment and Research System; BDI = Battelle Developmental Inventory Screening Test; BNT = Boston Naming Test; CDR = Cognitive Drug Research Battery; CUMANIN = Cuestionario de Madurez Neuropsicológica Infantil (Questionnaire for Children’s Neuropsychological Maturity); DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; EDIN = Escala del Desarrollo Integral del Niño (Scale of Integral Child Development); ENI = Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil (Child Neuropsychological Assessment); GHT = Goodenough–Harris Draw-a-Man Test; GTR = Graphic Test of Reasoning; INTER-NDA = INTERGROWTH-21st Neurodevelopment Assessment; K-ABC = Kaufman Brief Assessment for Children; KBDT = Kohs Block Design Test; K-BIT = Kaufman’s Brief Intelligence Test; MSCA = McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities; NEPSY = A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment; PLS-3 = Preschool Language Scale-3; PMA = Thurstone’s Primary Mental Abilities Test; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; PT = Pencil Tapping; RCPM = Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices; RPM = Raven’s Progressive Matrices; RSPM = Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices; ROCF = Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; TADI = Test de Aprendizaje y Desarrollo Infantil; TAVE-CI = Test de Aprendizaje Verbal. España-Complutense Infantil (Verbal Learning Test); TEVI: Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes; TVIP = Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (Spanish version of PPVT); TONI = Test of Nonverbal Intelligence; WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WJ = Woodcock–Johnson; WMB = Woodcock–Muñoz Battery; WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale; WMLS = Woodcock–Muñoz Language Survey; WPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence.

Table 2.

Child neurodevelopmental assessments used for research in Latin America.

AssessmentAppropriate age groupsaNo. of Studies (out of 97)
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)School age, Adolescence29
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI)Preschool/early childhood23
Woodcock–Muñoz Battery (WMB), Woodcock–Muñoz Language Survey (WMLS), and Woodcock–Johnson (WJ)All ages10
Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM), Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM)All ages9
McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA)Preschool/early childhood, School age9
Child Neuropsychological Assessment (ENI); Child Neuropsychological Assessment for Preschoolers (ENI-P)ENI: Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence
ENI-P: Preschool/early childhood
8
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT/TVIP)All ages7
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF)All ages5
Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS)Adolescence, Adult4
Battelle Developmental Inventory Screening Test (BDI)Preschool/early childhood3
Goodenough–Harris Draw-a-Man Test (GHT)All ages3
Kaufman Brief Assessment for Children (K-ABC)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
Kaufman’s Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT)All ages3
Stanford–Binet Intelligence ScaleAll ages3
Token Test for ChildrenPreschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment (NEPSY)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS)All ages2
Questionnaire for Children’s Neuropsychological Maturity (CUMANIN)Preschool/early childhood2
Spanish Complutense Verbal Learning Test—Children (TAVE-CI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), Day–Night Stroop TestAll ages2
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI)All ages2
Behavioral Assessment and Research System (BARS)All ages1
Boston Naming Test (BNT)All ages1
Cognitive Drug Research Battery (CDR)All ages1
General and Differential Aptitudes Battery (BADyG)All ages1
Graphic Test of Reasoning (GTR)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
INTERGROWTH-21st Neurodevelopment Assessment (INTER-NDA)Preschool/early childhood1
Kohs Block Design Test (KBDT)All ages1
Pencil Tapping (PT)Preschool/early childhood1
Preschool Language Scale-3 (PLS-3)Preschool/early childhood1
Scale of Integral Child Development (EDIN)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Aprendizaje y Desarrollo Infantil (Test of Child Learning and Development) (TADI)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes (TEVI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Thurstone’s Primary Mental Abilities Test (PMA)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI)All ages1
AssessmentAppropriate age groupsaNo. of Studies (out of 97)
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)School age, Adolescence29
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI)Preschool/early childhood23
Woodcock–Muñoz Battery (WMB), Woodcock–Muñoz Language Survey (WMLS), and Woodcock–Johnson (WJ)All ages10
Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM), Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM)All ages9
McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA)Preschool/early childhood, School age9
Child Neuropsychological Assessment (ENI); Child Neuropsychological Assessment for Preschoolers (ENI-P)ENI: Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence
ENI-P: Preschool/early childhood
8
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT/TVIP)All ages7
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF)All ages5
Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS)Adolescence, Adult4
Battelle Developmental Inventory Screening Test (BDI)Preschool/early childhood3
Goodenough–Harris Draw-a-Man Test (GHT)All ages3
Kaufman Brief Assessment for Children (K-ABC)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
Kaufman’s Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT)All ages3
Stanford–Binet Intelligence ScaleAll ages3
Token Test for ChildrenPreschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment (NEPSY)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS)All ages2
Questionnaire for Children’s Neuropsychological Maturity (CUMANIN)Preschool/early childhood2
Spanish Complutense Verbal Learning Test—Children (TAVE-CI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), Day–Night Stroop TestAll ages2
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI)All ages2
Behavioral Assessment and Research System (BARS)All ages1
Boston Naming Test (BNT)All ages1
Cognitive Drug Research Battery (CDR)All ages1
General and Differential Aptitudes Battery (BADyG)All ages1
Graphic Test of Reasoning (GTR)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
INTERGROWTH-21st Neurodevelopment Assessment (INTER-NDA)Preschool/early childhood1
Kohs Block Design Test (KBDT)All ages1
Pencil Tapping (PT)Preschool/early childhood1
Preschool Language Scale-3 (PLS-3)Preschool/early childhood1
Scale of Integral Child Development (EDIN)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Aprendizaje y Desarrollo Infantil (Test of Child Learning and Development) (TADI)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes (TEVI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Thurstone’s Primary Mental Abilities Test (PMA)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI)All ages1
a

Categories include: Preschool/early childhood (ages 2–7), School age (8–11), Adolescence (12–19), Adult (20+), All ages.

Table 2.

Child neurodevelopmental assessments used for research in Latin America.

AssessmentAppropriate age groupsaNo. of Studies (out of 97)
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)School age, Adolescence29
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI)Preschool/early childhood23
Woodcock–Muñoz Battery (WMB), Woodcock–Muñoz Language Survey (WMLS), and Woodcock–Johnson (WJ)All ages10
Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM), Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM)All ages9
McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA)Preschool/early childhood, School age9
Child Neuropsychological Assessment (ENI); Child Neuropsychological Assessment for Preschoolers (ENI-P)ENI: Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence
ENI-P: Preschool/early childhood
8
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT/TVIP)All ages7
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF)All ages5
Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS)Adolescence, Adult4
Battelle Developmental Inventory Screening Test (BDI)Preschool/early childhood3
Goodenough–Harris Draw-a-Man Test (GHT)All ages3
Kaufman Brief Assessment for Children (K-ABC)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
Kaufman’s Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT)All ages3
Stanford–Binet Intelligence ScaleAll ages3
Token Test for ChildrenPreschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment (NEPSY)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS)All ages2
Questionnaire for Children’s Neuropsychological Maturity (CUMANIN)Preschool/early childhood2
Spanish Complutense Verbal Learning Test—Children (TAVE-CI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), Day–Night Stroop TestAll ages2
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI)All ages2
Behavioral Assessment and Research System (BARS)All ages1
Boston Naming Test (BNT)All ages1
Cognitive Drug Research Battery (CDR)All ages1
General and Differential Aptitudes Battery (BADyG)All ages1
Graphic Test of Reasoning (GTR)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
INTERGROWTH-21st Neurodevelopment Assessment (INTER-NDA)Preschool/early childhood1
Kohs Block Design Test (KBDT)All ages1
Pencil Tapping (PT)Preschool/early childhood1
Preschool Language Scale-3 (PLS-3)Preschool/early childhood1
Scale of Integral Child Development (EDIN)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Aprendizaje y Desarrollo Infantil (Test of Child Learning and Development) (TADI)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes (TEVI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Thurstone’s Primary Mental Abilities Test (PMA)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI)All ages1
AssessmentAppropriate age groupsaNo. of Studies (out of 97)
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)School age, Adolescence29
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI)Preschool/early childhood23
Woodcock–Muñoz Battery (WMB), Woodcock–Muñoz Language Survey (WMLS), and Woodcock–Johnson (WJ)All ages10
Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM), Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM)All ages9
McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA)Preschool/early childhood, School age9
Child Neuropsychological Assessment (ENI); Child Neuropsychological Assessment for Preschoolers (ENI-P)ENI: Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence
ENI-P: Preschool/early childhood
8
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT/TVIP)All ages7
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF)All ages5
Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS)Adolescence, Adult4
Battelle Developmental Inventory Screening Test (BDI)Preschool/early childhood3
Goodenough–Harris Draw-a-Man Test (GHT)All ages3
Kaufman Brief Assessment for Children (K-ABC)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
Kaufman’s Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT)All ages3
Stanford–Binet Intelligence ScaleAll ages3
Token Test for ChildrenPreschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence3
A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment (NEPSY)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS)All ages2
Questionnaire for Children’s Neuropsychological Maturity (CUMANIN)Preschool/early childhood2
Spanish Complutense Verbal Learning Test—Children (TAVE-CI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence2
Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), Day–Night Stroop TestAll ages2
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI)All ages2
Behavioral Assessment and Research System (BARS)All ages1
Boston Naming Test (BNT)All ages1
Cognitive Drug Research Battery (CDR)All ages1
General and Differential Aptitudes Battery (BADyG)All ages1
Graphic Test of Reasoning (GTR)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
INTERGROWTH-21st Neurodevelopment Assessment (INTER-NDA)Preschool/early childhood1
Kohs Block Design Test (KBDT)All ages1
Pencil Tapping (PT)Preschool/early childhood1
Preschool Language Scale-3 (PLS-3)Preschool/early childhood1
Scale of Integral Child Development (EDIN)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Aprendizaje y Desarrollo Infantil (Test of Child Learning and Development) (TADI)Preschool/early childhood1
Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes (TEVI)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Thurstone’s Primary Mental Abilities Test (PMA)Preschool/early childhood, School age, Adolescence1
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI)All ages1
a

Categories include: Preschool/early childhood (ages 2–7), School age (8–11), Adolescence (12–19), Adult (20+), All ages.

Wechsler Intelligence Scales

The Wechsler intelligence scales, including the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI), the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI), are some of the most well-known standard measures of intelligence. Each scale assesses different cognitive indices, such as verbal and nonverbal reasoning skills, working memory, and/or processing speed, which render index scores including a Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) as well as verbal IQ (VIQ) and nonverbal IQ (NVIQ). Other indices and supplementary scores are also options depending on how many and which subtests are administered. Forty-six studies used at least one Wechsler scale; 23 used the WPPSI, 29 used the WISC, and 1 used the WASI. Some authors that studied broad age ranges of participants, such as González-Cortés (2019) and Herrera-Chinchay et al. (2021), used more than one Wechsler scale to assess intelligence using the age-appropriate Wechsler test (e.g., WPPSI or WISC). For example, Herrera-Chinchay et al. (2021) studied children aged 2–16 years; investigators administered the WPPSI to children aged 2–7 years, and the WISC to other children from 6 up to 16 years of age. Studies that used the Wechsler scales were conducted in 11 countries: Mexico (n = 13), Argentina (n = 6), Colombia (n = 6), Chile (n = 5), Peru (n = 5), Costa Rica (n = 4), Ecuador (n = 2), Bolivia (n = 2), Panama (n = 1), Cuba (n = 1), and Guatemala (n = 1).

The WPPSI was originally developed in 1967 and has had three new editions since: the WPPSI-R in 1989 (Wechsler, 1989), the WPPSI-III in 2002 (Wechsler, 2002), and the WPPSI-IV in 2012 (Wechsler, 2012). Each new edition of the WPPSI has encompassed a broader age range, the most recent version intended for use in children between 2 years 6 months of age and 7 years 7 months of age (Wechsler, 2012). The Spanish language version of the WPPSI (Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para preescolar y primaria [Pearson]) has been validated for use in Mexico and Spain with large normative samples, translation of language from the English version, and adaptation of language to cultural differences (Sánchez-Escobedo et al., 2016). Some studies in this review specifically reported use of the Mexican edition of the test (e.g., Cando et al., 2019) or the Spain-normed version of the test (e.g., Caulfield, 2011). Chasiotis et al. (2006) report use of a “Costa Rican version of the verbal IQ scale,” and McKay (1978) used a local adaptation of the original WPPSI, although it is unclear how these versions were modified from the original. Others (e.g., Warthon-Medina, 2015; Zambrano-Sánchez et al., 2010) report using the “Spanish version” of the test; however, this way of reporting is unclear whether this refers to the version normed in Spain or the Spanish-language edition with standardization in a Mexican population (e.g., Blouin et al., 2018 mention using “the Spanish version” in their methods, but cite a version of the WPPSI-III standardized in Mexico; Caulfield et al. [2011] also report using “the Spanish adaptation”, but cite a 1993 version from Argentina). Eleven studies (48%) that used the WPPSI (Álvarez González et al., 2004; Baroni et al., 2020; Clark, 2006; Galván et al., 2014; Hermida et al., 2019; Herrera-Chinchay et al. 2021; Lozoff et al., 2006; McInnes, 2005; Moguel-Ancheita et al., 2008; Morales-Buenrostro et al., 2019; Salinas-Piélago et al., 1998) did not specifically mention or cite a Spanish language edition, and Caraballo et al. (2004) did not specify in their methods which of the “Wechsler Intelligence” tests were used. While the assumption is that all tests were administered in the Spanish language, seven studies (30%) were published in English and did not mention the language of administration of the WPPSI for their participants (Baroni et al., 2020; Clark et al., 2006; Galván et al., 2014; Hermida et al., 2019; Lozoff et al., 2006; McInnes et al., 2005; McKay, 1978; Morales-Buenrostro et al., 2019).

Some studies, such as McKay et al. (1978) and Chasiotis et al. (2006), administered select subtests of the WPPSI thus were not able to render any composite index scores. Table 3 summarizes the WPPSI results across studies that assessed intelligence of typically developing children; of these 15 studies, nine reported mean IQ scores including FSIQ as well as VIQ and NVIQ. Most did not report their range of scores, only standard deviation. Mean FSIQ scores ranged from 86.61 (Warthon-Medina, 2015) to 108.56 (Morales-Buenrostro et al., 2019), VIQ scores from 83.31 (Warthon-Medina, 2015) to 104.33 (Morales-Buenrostro et al., 2019), and NVIQ scores from 96.39 (Cando et al., 2019) to 112.22 (Morales-Buenrostro et al., 2019). Six of nine (67.7%) of the studies with reported FSIQ scores had mean FSIQ under 95 which is below the measurement error of the WPPSI and falls in the lower end of the average range. Six studies did not report index scores, which was largely because the study only administered select subtests of the WPPSI (e.g., Chasiotis et al., 2006; McKay et al., 1978).

Table 3.

WPPSI methods and results among typically developing preschool-age children in Spanish-speaking Latin America.

Lead author (year)EditionLanguage administered inTotal No. of participantsAge of participants completing WPPSIIndex scores (FSIQ, VIQ, NVIQ): Mean (SD)
Blouin et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish8804–5No index scores reported
Bojorque et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish3555–6No index scores reported
Cando et al. (2019)WPPSI (Mexican version)Spanish375–6FSIQ: 89.25 (10.43)
VIQ: 86.46 (11.55)
NVIQ: 96.39 (9.96)
Caulfield et al. (2011)WPPSI (Spanish version) cited 1993Spanish165 (subset of larger sample, n = 184)4No index scores reported for sub-sample of 165 children; scores below reflect original sample (n = 184):
FSIQ:
Group 1: 91.9 (10.0)
Group 2: 92.3 (10.1)
VIQ:
Group 1: 85.3 (9.8)
Group 2: 86.2 (10.3)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 100.6 (11.7)
Group 2: 100.0 (12.8)
Chasiotis et al. (2006)WPPSI (Costa Rican version, cites Echeverría 1980)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish3143–5No index scores reported
Clark et al. (2006)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish7845FSIQ:
Breastfed <2 months: 87.1 (0.6)
Breastfed 2–8 months: 88.4 (0.05)
Breastfed >8 months: 88.1 (1.2)
Galván et al. (2014)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish2154–5FSIQ:
Obese: 92.0 (8.94)
Nonobese: 91.69 (8.57)
Hermida et al. (2019)WPPSI-IIINot mentioned; presumed Spanish1315No index scores reported
Hill et al. (2014)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish1914–6No index scores reported
Lozoff et al. (2006)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish1615No index scores reported for WPPSI (Figure 2 shows FSIQ range of ∼86–115)
Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)WPPSI (not specified or cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish95–6FSIQ: 88.1 (12)
VIQ: 89.8 (11)
NVIQ: 84.7 (10)
Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Spanish1004–6FSIQ:
Unexposed: 108.56 (11.59)
Exposed: 100.2 (9.79)
VIQ:
Unexposed: 104.33 (11.4)
Exposed: 99.9 (10.54)
NVIQ:
Unexposed: 112.22 (13.18)
Exposed: 99.5 (12.06)
Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish1083–5FSIQ:
Controls (initial evaluation): 102.63 (10.27)
Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)WPPSI-R (Spanish version)Spanish1843–4FSIQ:
Group 1: 86.61 (12.23)
Group 2: 87.9 (12.91)
VIQ:
Group 1: 83.31 (9.86)
Group 2: 83.38 (11.09)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 93.95 (14.80)
Group 2: 94.48 (16.75)
Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)WPPSI (Spanish version; not cited)Spanish1624–6FSIQ: 99 (12.9)
VIQ: 93 (11.7)
NVIQ: 99 (14.9)
Lead author (year)EditionLanguage administered inTotal No. of participantsAge of participants completing WPPSIIndex scores (FSIQ, VIQ, NVIQ): Mean (SD)
Blouin et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish8804–5No index scores reported
Bojorque et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish3555–6No index scores reported
Cando et al. (2019)WPPSI (Mexican version)Spanish375–6FSIQ: 89.25 (10.43)
VIQ: 86.46 (11.55)
NVIQ: 96.39 (9.96)
Caulfield et al. (2011)WPPSI (Spanish version) cited 1993Spanish165 (subset of larger sample, n = 184)4No index scores reported for sub-sample of 165 children; scores below reflect original sample (n = 184):
FSIQ:
Group 1: 91.9 (10.0)
Group 2: 92.3 (10.1)
VIQ:
Group 1: 85.3 (9.8)
Group 2: 86.2 (10.3)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 100.6 (11.7)
Group 2: 100.0 (12.8)
Chasiotis et al. (2006)WPPSI (Costa Rican version, cites Echeverría 1980)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish3143–5No index scores reported
Clark et al. (2006)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish7845FSIQ:
Breastfed <2 months: 87.1 (0.6)
Breastfed 2–8 months: 88.4 (0.05)
Breastfed >8 months: 88.1 (1.2)
Galván et al. (2014)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish2154–5FSIQ:
Obese: 92.0 (8.94)
Nonobese: 91.69 (8.57)
Hermida et al. (2019)WPPSI-IIINot mentioned; presumed Spanish1315No index scores reported
Hill et al. (2014)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish1914–6No index scores reported
Lozoff et al. (2006)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish1615No index scores reported for WPPSI (Figure 2 shows FSIQ range of ∼86–115)
Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)WPPSI (not specified or cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish95–6FSIQ: 88.1 (12)
VIQ: 89.8 (11)
NVIQ: 84.7 (10)
Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Spanish1004–6FSIQ:
Unexposed: 108.56 (11.59)
Exposed: 100.2 (9.79)
VIQ:
Unexposed: 104.33 (11.4)
Exposed: 99.9 (10.54)
NVIQ:
Unexposed: 112.22 (13.18)
Exposed: 99.5 (12.06)
Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish1083–5FSIQ:
Controls (initial evaluation): 102.63 (10.27)
Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)WPPSI-R (Spanish version)Spanish1843–4FSIQ:
Group 1: 86.61 (12.23)
Group 2: 87.9 (12.91)
VIQ:
Group 1: 83.31 (9.86)
Group 2: 83.38 (11.09)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 93.95 (14.80)
Group 2: 94.48 (16.75)
Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)WPPSI (Spanish version; not cited)Spanish1624–6FSIQ: 99 (12.9)
VIQ: 93 (11.7)
NVIQ: 99 (14.9)

Note. Although the WISC was used in a higher number of studies in this review than the WPPSI (29 used the WISC compared to the WPPSI with 23), the studies using the WISC used the assessment to measure development of children at the highest end of our intended age range (6+) and generally focused on school age children. The WPPSI was selected for this table because it was the most frequently used assessment that measured cognitive development among our target population of preschool-aged children between ages 2–6 and allows us to interpret findings more accurately to this target population. FSIQ = full Scale IQ, or total IQ; VIQ = verbal IQ; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ or other nonverbal index scores (executive IQ, manipulative IQ, performance IQ).

Table 3.

WPPSI methods and results among typically developing preschool-age children in Spanish-speaking Latin America.

Lead author (year)EditionLanguage administered inTotal No. of participantsAge of participants completing WPPSIIndex scores (FSIQ, VIQ, NVIQ): Mean (SD)
Blouin et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish8804–5No index scores reported
Bojorque et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish3555–6No index scores reported
Cando et al. (2019)WPPSI (Mexican version)Spanish375–6FSIQ: 89.25 (10.43)
VIQ: 86.46 (11.55)
NVIQ: 96.39 (9.96)
Caulfield et al. (2011)WPPSI (Spanish version) cited 1993Spanish165 (subset of larger sample, n = 184)4No index scores reported for sub-sample of 165 children; scores below reflect original sample (n = 184):
FSIQ:
Group 1: 91.9 (10.0)
Group 2: 92.3 (10.1)
VIQ:
Group 1: 85.3 (9.8)
Group 2: 86.2 (10.3)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 100.6 (11.7)
Group 2: 100.0 (12.8)
Chasiotis et al. (2006)WPPSI (Costa Rican version, cites Echeverría 1980)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish3143–5No index scores reported
Clark et al. (2006)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish7845FSIQ:
Breastfed <2 months: 87.1 (0.6)
Breastfed 2–8 months: 88.4 (0.05)
Breastfed >8 months: 88.1 (1.2)
Galván et al. (2014)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish2154–5FSIQ:
Obese: 92.0 (8.94)
Nonobese: 91.69 (8.57)
Hermida et al. (2019)WPPSI-IIINot mentioned; presumed Spanish1315No index scores reported
Hill et al. (2014)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish1914–6No index scores reported
Lozoff et al. (2006)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish1615No index scores reported for WPPSI (Figure 2 shows FSIQ range of ∼86–115)
Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)WPPSI (not specified or cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish95–6FSIQ: 88.1 (12)
VIQ: 89.8 (11)
NVIQ: 84.7 (10)
Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Spanish1004–6FSIQ:
Unexposed: 108.56 (11.59)
Exposed: 100.2 (9.79)
VIQ:
Unexposed: 104.33 (11.4)
Exposed: 99.9 (10.54)
NVIQ:
Unexposed: 112.22 (13.18)
Exposed: 99.5 (12.06)
Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish1083–5FSIQ:
Controls (initial evaluation): 102.63 (10.27)
Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)WPPSI-R (Spanish version)Spanish1843–4FSIQ:
Group 1: 86.61 (12.23)
Group 2: 87.9 (12.91)
VIQ:
Group 1: 83.31 (9.86)
Group 2: 83.38 (11.09)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 93.95 (14.80)
Group 2: 94.48 (16.75)
Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)WPPSI (Spanish version; not cited)Spanish1624–6FSIQ: 99 (12.9)
VIQ: 93 (11.7)
NVIQ: 99 (14.9)
Lead author (year)EditionLanguage administered inTotal No. of participantsAge of participants completing WPPSIIndex scores (FSIQ, VIQ, NVIQ): Mean (SD)
Blouin et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish8804–5No index scores reported
Bojorque et al. (2018)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish3555–6No index scores reported
Cando et al. (2019)WPPSI (Mexican version)Spanish375–6FSIQ: 89.25 (10.43)
VIQ: 86.46 (11.55)
NVIQ: 96.39 (9.96)
Caulfield et al. (2011)WPPSI (Spanish version) cited 1993Spanish165 (subset of larger sample, n = 184)4No index scores reported for sub-sample of 165 children; scores below reflect original sample (n = 184):
FSIQ:
Group 1: 91.9 (10.0)
Group 2: 92.3 (10.1)
VIQ:
Group 1: 85.3 (9.8)
Group 2: 86.2 (10.3)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 100.6 (11.7)
Group 2: 100.0 (12.8)
Chasiotis et al. (2006)WPPSI (Costa Rican version, cites Echeverría 1980)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish3143–5No index scores reported
Clark et al. (2006)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish7845FSIQ:
Breastfed <2 months: 87.1 (0.6)
Breastfed 2–8 months: 88.4 (0.05)
Breastfed >8 months: 88.1 (1.2)
Galván et al. (2014)WPPSI-RNot mentioned; presumed Spanish2154–5FSIQ:
Obese: 92.0 (8.94)
Nonobese: 91.69 (8.57)
Hermida et al. (2019)WPPSI-IIINot mentioned; presumed Spanish1315No index scores reported
Hill et al. (2014)WPPSI-III (Spanish version)Spanish1914–6No index scores reported
Lozoff et al. (2006)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish1615No index scores reported for WPPSI (Figure 2 shows FSIQ range of ∼86–115)
Moguel-Ancheita et al. (2008)WPPSI (not specified or cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish95–6FSIQ: 88.1 (12)
VIQ: 89.8 (11)
NVIQ: 84.7 (10)
Morales-Buenrostro et al. (2019)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Spanish1004–6FSIQ:
Unexposed: 108.56 (11.59)
Exposed: 100.2 (9.79)
VIQ:
Unexposed: 104.33 (11.4)
Exposed: 99.9 (10.54)
NVIQ:
Unexposed: 112.22 (13.18)
Exposed: 99.5 (12.06)
Salinas-Piélago et al. (1998)WPPSI (not specified; 1967 version cited)Not mentioned; presumed Spanish and article written in Spanish1083–5FSIQ:
Controls (initial evaluation): 102.63 (10.27)
Warthon-Medina et al. (2015)WPPSI-R (Spanish version)Spanish1843–4FSIQ:
Group 1: 86.61 (12.23)
Group 2: 87.9 (12.91)
VIQ:
Group 1: 83.31 (9.86)
Group 2: 83.38 (11.09)
NVIQ:
Group 1: 93.95 (14.80)
Group 2: 94.48 (16.75)
Zambrano-Sánchez et al. (2010)WPPSI (Spanish version; not cited)Spanish1624–6FSIQ: 99 (12.9)
VIQ: 93 (11.7)
NVIQ: 99 (14.9)

Note. Although the WISC was used in a higher number of studies in this review than the WPPSI (29 used the WISC compared to the WPPSI with 23), the studies using the WISC used the assessment to measure development of children at the highest end of our intended age range (6+) and generally focused on school age children. The WPPSI was selected for this table because it was the most frequently used assessment that measured cognitive development among our target population of preschool-aged children between ages 2–6 and allows us to interpret findings more accurately to this target population. FSIQ = full Scale IQ, or total IQ; VIQ = verbal IQ; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ or other nonverbal index scores (executive IQ, manipulative IQ, performance IQ).

The WISC was originally developed in 1949 and, like the WPPSI, has been updated with the revised edition in 1974 (WISC-R), the third edition in 1991 (WISC-III), the fourth edition in 2003 (WISC-IV), and the fifth edition in 2014 (WISC-V). The most recent version of the test is intended to evaluate children from 6 years through 16 years 11 months of age (Kezer & Arik, 2012). Five studies in our analysis specifically report using the Mexican version of the WISC (Aguilar, 1993; Morales, 2013; Porras-Kattz, 2011; Rico, 2006; Van Wendel de Joode, 2016). Johnston et al. (1987) used a version of the WISC that was modified for a Guatemalan population but did not report standard scores due to lack of local norming. Many other remaining studies did not mention the edition and/or norms that were used.

McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities

The McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA) were developed in 1972 to measure cognitive development among children aged 2 years 6 months to 8 years 6 months by assessing their perceptual performance; memory; and motor, quantitative, verbal, and general cognitive skills (Sands & D’Amato, 2017). Each domain is evaluated using between three and seven subtests that include both cognitive and motor activities. Nine studies in this review used the MSCA; they all took place in Mexico except one in Costa Rica (McInnes et al., 2005). Four studies (Gonzalez-Casanova et al., 2021; Grandjean, 2022; Osorio-Valencia, 2018; Schnaas et al., 2000) used a validated Spanish version of the MSCA (McCarthy, 2004) which was administered by trained psychologists at their respective institutions. Three studies (Braun, 2012; Knauer et al., 2018; Kupsco, 2020) used a translated and adapted version of the test from researchers at the Instituto Nacional de Perinatología in Mexico City. Osorio-Valencia et al. (2018) and Braun et al. (2012) reported use of the U.S. norms given the lack of official Mexican normative data for the test.

Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM, RCPM, RSPM)

The Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM) are a series of tests originally developed in the 1930s that measure cognitive ability through a nonverbal reasoning task. RPM was originally designed to limit the influence of cultural bias by employing a nonverbal measure of intelligence (Counter et al., 1998). The basic version of the test, Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), relies on problem-solving using a series of black-and-white matrices that increase in difficulty and is validated for use in individuals aged 6 years through adulthood (Raven, 1936). Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM) is a shorter version of the test and is validated for use in children aged 5–11, the elderly, and adults with intellectual disabilities. Nine studies used the Raven’s Matrices tests in the countries of Argentina (n = 2), Chile (n = 2), Ecuador (n = 2), Bolivia (n = 1), Costa Rica (n = 1), and Mexico (n = 1). Population norms for the RPM tests have been established in Latin American regions such as Peru, Puerto Rico, Argentina, Guatemala, and Uruguay (Counter et al., 2019; Kihn, 2005). Rindermann et al. (2015) described the specific Austria and U.K.-based norms utilized for analysis of RCPM data in their cohort of Costa Rican children. Hill et al. (2014) acknowledged the lack of local Bolivian norms for their study population and reported using z-score analysis based on the mean scores of their own local patient samples in place of standard scores.

Child Psychological Assessment/Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil

The Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil (ENI) was published in 2007 and is the first neuropsychological battery designed specifically for use with Latin American children (Matute et al., 2007; Matute et al., 2022). The ENI was developed by the Laboratorio de Neuropsicología y Neurolingüística (Neuropsychology and Neurolinguistics Laboratory) at the University of Guadalajara, Mexico and normed using children from Colombia and Mexico between the ages of 5 and 16 (Matute et al., 2022). The ENI evaluates numerous cognitive and academic domains such as memory, perception, language, attention, reading, writing, arithmetic, visuospatial abilities, and executive function. In total, there are 13 domains, 36 subdomains, and 79 tasks in the battery (Matute et al., 2022).

Eight studies in this review assessed child cognitive development using the ENI. Two of these studies took place exclusively in Mexico (Bauer, 2014; Morales, 2013), two in Colombia (Pinto-Dussán et al., 2010; Urazán-Torres et al., 2013), and one each in Argentina (Juric, 2013) and Chile (Varela et al., 2020). The other two studies recruited participants from multiple countries; Ardila et al. (2011) assessed children in Colombia and Mexico, while Treviño et al. (2021) conducted assessments in Mexico, Colombia, and Guatemala. A second edition of the ENI (ENI-2) was published in 2013 (Matute et al., 2013) and a version of the ENI adapted for preschool-aged children aged 2–5 (ENI-P) was released in 2021 (Matute et al., 2021). Five of the studies in this review cite use of the first edition of the ENI published in 2007 (Ardila et al., 2011; Bauer, 2014; Juric, 2013; Morales, 2013; Urazán-Torres et al., 2013). Pinto-Dussán et al. (2010) do not explicitly cite an ENI manual, but based on the date of study publication the first edition must have been used, as no other edition had yet been published. Treviño et al. (2021) cite use of both the 2007 edition of the ENI as well as the ENI-P. Varela et al. (2020) was the only study to use the ENI-2.

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test/Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody

The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) was published in 1959 as a test of vocabulary developed for individuals between the ages of 2 years 6 months and 90 years of age. The PPVT was revised as the PPVT-R in 1981 (Dunn & Dunn, 1981), the PPVT-III in 1997 (Dunn & Dunn, 1997), the PPVT-IV in 2007 (Dunn & Dunn, 2007), and the PPVT-V in 2018 (Dunn, 2018). The Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (TVIP) is the Spanish version of the test that was adapted from the PPVT-R in 1986 and has been used extensively in Spanish-speaking populations in the United States and Latin America (Bernal & Fernández, 2013). The Latin American version of the TVIP was standardized in both Mexican and Puerto Rican populations and includes a composite Hispanic set of norms that was developed from both samples of participants (Dunn & Dunn, 2007). A Spain edition based on the PPVT-III was published in 2010 (Dunn et al., 2010).

Of the seven studies that used any version of the PPVT/TVIP, three of them used the Latin American adaptation of the TVIP published in 1986 (Bernal & Fernández, 2013; Lohndorf, 2019; Rico, 2006). Two describe using a Spanish-language versions of the 1997 PPVT-III (Reynolds, 2022; Rosales-Rueda, 2018). Rosales-Rueda (2018) specifies that norms from Mexican and Puerto Rican children were used for scoring, and Reynolds (2022) notes that their study team also adapted some words to the Chilean context. Diuk et al. (2019) cite a 1986 version of the PPVT but do not make note of which Spanish adaptation was used. Aizawa (2021) uses versions of the PPVT that were adapted by local members of the study team as part of the Young Lives Study of poverty and inequality that has been tracking the development of 12,000 children across Ethiopia, India, Peru, and Vietnam since 2001.

Woodcock–Johnson, Woodcock Muñoz Battery, Woodcock Muñoz Language Survey

The Woodcock–Johnson (WJ) Tests of Cognitive Abilities assess general cognition, language, and academic achievement (Grizzle & Davis, 2011). The WJ was developed initially in 1977 and has been revised in 1989 (WJ-R), 2001 (WJ-III), and 2014 (WJ-IV). The Batería Woodcock–Muñoz (WMB) is the Spanish edition of the Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities. The test is validated in ages 5–95, but some subtests are appropriate for children as young as 2 years of age. The normative data for the WMB were acquired in Spanish-speaking individuals of Hispanic origin living in the United States (LaForte et al., 2019). Eight studies in this analysis utilized the WMB tests or its subtests (Bernal & Fernández, 2013; Clark, 2006; Gomez & Yoshikawa, 2017; Korzeniowski et al., 2017; Lamminmäki et al., 1997; Leyva et al., 2015; Lozoff et al., 2006; Rosselli et al., 2001); one study cites WJ (Sánchez-Escobedo et al., 2019). Leyva et al. (2015) rely on the WMB along with the Woodcock–Muñoz Language Survey (WMLS) to assess language skills and literacy in Chilean kindergarteners. The WMLS is a test that focuses specifically on language proficiency (Muñoz-Sandoval et al., 2015); the Spanish version of the WMLS was also used by Castro et al. (2002).

Discussion

This systematic review illustrates the breadth of standardized neurodevelopmental assessments of cognition used in preschool-aged children in Latin America. We found large variability of measures across publications, even in a relatively narrow age range of preschool children. We identified 41 neurodevelopmental tests used to assess children’s abilities across 97 studies from 13 countries. The Wechsler intelligence scales were the most widely and consistently used assessments, appearing in studies from every decade of the results, in 11 countries, and half of the studies. Other common measures included the WJ tests, MSCA, RPM test, Child Neuropsychological Assessment (Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil), and the PPVT, which are all measures of cognitive development but have distinct strengths and weaknesses that differentiate them from one another. Twenty-eight additional measures were used in between 1 and 5 studies in this review. Researchers and clinicians working with Spanish-speaking preschoolers may use the results of this review as a starting point as they select assessments that are most appropriate for their particular samples, regions, and goals.

Our review supports a need for continued research to validate, standardize, and ensure cultural relevance of cognitive assessments for this population. International studies on child neurodevelopment are complicated by a lack of normative data for all regions; findings from this review echo existing concerns in the field of neuropsychology about the appropriateness of internationally developed standardized assessments available for use in multicultural settings without additional research (Shuttleworth-Edwards, 2016; Statucka et al., 2021). For instance, the Wechsler scales have norms available for children from Mexico and Spain, but not other Spanish-speaking areas of the world. Many researchers in Latin America have utilized the Spain edition of the test; however, it remains unclear whether these norms and even the items are culturally and linguistically appropriate for populations of children in Latin America, particularly in low-income and low-resource areas. For example, the Similarities subtest of the WPPSI requires respondents to understand the connection between two seasons (winter and spring), which relies on an understanding of four seasons in the way that those in Spain or the United States do; the common understanding in equatorial countries in Latin America may be that there is only a dry season and a rainy season (Louison, 2016). Additionally, the Spanish language has numerous regional dialects; any testing of specific knowledge on standardized assessments would require that vocabulary words be specific to the regions in which testing occurs. Critiques that these norms are not appropriate were supported by our descriptive analysis of studies assessing typically developing children using the WPPSI. Of the three studies assessed with a reported mean FSIQ scores above 95, two were from Mexico using normative data from Mexico. Of the remaining six studies that reported IQ scores, the mean FSIQ was below 95. A cutoff of 95 is significant because the measurement error for the WPPSI is ∼5 IQ points. Five of six of the studies that used normative data from a country outside their sample reported a downward shift in IQ that falls in the lower end of the average range. This has implications for how an investigator interprets their results if they are assuming that the average IQ score is supposed to be 100. Many other neurodevelopmental assessments used by studies in this review, even those with Spanish-language norms such as the MSCA, have only been officially normed within the United States. While other assessments such as the PPVT have been normed in Spanish-speaking populations of Latin America, even these tests are only generalizable to a limited number of countries in the region (e.g., Mexico). Normative samples should reflect the vast socioeconomic and cultural diversity of Spanish-speaking Latin America to best characterize the neurodevelopmental performance of children.

Most studies in our review did not report why they selected certain assessments, which editions or adaptations of assessments were used, or which normative samples were used to develop scores for their selected assessments, which limits generalizability of their results. It is possible that researchers faced barriers to accessing and utilizing certain assessments, such as high costs of obtaining a license for use. For tests in which normative versions exist in both Spain and Latin America, it was often unclear whether using the “Spanish” version of a test (a description often used in study methods) implies the use of Spain norms, or simply a translated version of the test into the Spanish language with Latin American norms. MSCA has been appraised as an outdated test given its lack of re-norming since the early 1970s (Levin, 2011; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2017). Braun et al. (2012) described the MSCA as a “valid” and “reliable” test, yet others who used the MSCA did not provide insight into why they chose the MSCA over other modern and more recently normed scales of child abilities and intelligence. Obtaining accurate norms is an expensive endeavor by publishing companies, and test developers often have limitations in what population is available to establish their normative data. As such, researchers often utilize normative data cited in research articles (Neils-Strunjaš, 1998) for updated and demographically relevant norms as a next best option. To improve international child development research moving forward, it would be beneficial for authors to be very specific in describing research methods about what normative datasets were used and the reasons for such comparisons. Future researchers should use caution when selecting tests of cognitive functioning to ensure that rigorous testing has allowed them to maintain validity and reliability in the modern age, clearly state if they made any adaptations to their chosen assessment, and discuss ways in which the assessment they chose may be biased or may not be fully applicable to their specific study sample.

Strengths and limitations

This systematic review focuses on performance-based child assessments rather than proxy questionnaires by parents, teachers, or caregivers. A systematic review by Semrud-Clikeman et al. (2017) examined the use of neurodevelopmental assessments in LMICs; however, the authors considered both parent-rating forms and direct child assessments. While proxy questionnaires are useful tools for measuring child development and may be used in conjunction with direct child assessments, the latter often provide different information regarding a child’s functioning and are sometimes preferred to proxy questionnaires (Munoz-Chereau et al., 2021). Future studies should review the proxy questionnaire measures of child development used in Latin America so that clinicians and researchers can select the type of tool or combination of tools that is best for the necessary evaluation. Furthermore, we excluded any assessment or task that was developed specifically for a single study or study sample, as these assessments are hard to replicate and typically are not scored in a standardized manner (e.g., see assessment tools utilized in Puche-Navarro and Millán [2007] or Zimerman et al. [1982]). Our focus on standardized performance-based assessments limited the number of eligible child outcome studies to review, but enhanced the implication of our findings by providing a narrower scope of child assessment tools used. Since only standardized assessments were addressed in our review, our results can inform future research on child development in Spanish-speaking Latin America with tools that are well-established and used in research across various disciplines, which will improve data harmonization for multisite studies and future meta-analyses.

While much research on child cognitive development has occurred in high-resource settings, outcomes in LMICs are particularly important to study due to the differing threats to the health of developing children, such as adverse environmental, pathogenic, and socioeconomic exposures. Like children in other regions burdened by poverty, discrimination, gender inequity, and violence, Latin American children exposed to high rates of systemic inequality are at risk of adverse early childhood developmental outcomes (Hartwig, 2020). The early preschool years represent a particularly important period of neurodevelopment, wherein the brain undergoes a high proportion of its morphologic and physiological changes relative to other periods in the life course (Brown & Jernigan, 2012). Further, diagnosis of neurodevelopmental and cognitive delays during the preschool years allows for the introduction of crucial interventions while the brain remains plastic enough to respond to them.

The ability to harmonize data between diverse, multinational cohorts using standardized assessment tools can enhance the applicability and validity of study findings. For example, the Zika Virus Individual Participant Data Consortium and meta-analysis initiative aims to harmonize data from 28 countries and territories and 52 cohort studies to understand the neurodevelopmental outcomes of children with in utero Zika-virus exposure; however, heterogeneity of methods and outcome measures across studies presents a significant challenge to harmonization (Zika Virus Individual Participant Data Consortium, 2020). The worldwide coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic had even more extensive geographic involvement than the Zika virus, amplifying the need for consistency in data reporting to mitigate the effects of COVID-19 on child development as efficiently and comprehensively as possible. Beyond infectious disease threats, relatively common conditions experienced in childhood such as congenital heart disease and epilepsy, and rare diseases such as hypothalamic hamartoma, urea cycle disorders, and white matter disease, would also benefit from data sharing and combined cohorts to strengthen research findings (e.g., Berl et al., 2017; Sanz et al., 2021).

There are several inherent limitations to our systematic review. First, this study was not developed as an appraisal of neurodevelopmental instruments, but rather as a description of the tests that are commonly utilized in LMIC populations of Spanish-speaking Latin America. Therefore, descriptions of the strengths of the individual assessments for a given study sample are outside the scope of this review; however, future research may seek to explore the utility of the most commonly used assessment tools identified in this study. Second, this study only evaluated the neurodevelopmental tools that have been utilized for research and not those used for clinical assessment; thus, this systematic review may not capture the full spectrum of neurodevelopmental evaluation tools used in Latin America for diagnostic purposes in this young child age range. We also were unable to ascertain whether there were variables such as funding limitations or inaccessibility of assessment tools which led researchers to choose one assessment over another or which potentially biased their methods. Third, although articles written in Spanish were included in the review and many were indexed in English, we recognize that some studies conducted in Latin America are written, published, and indexed in Spanish. Other internationally published research articles, even those written in English, may not be indexed on the search engines utilized by this study. Therefore, future work utilizing different databases or Spanish translations of words like “cognition” and “neurodevelopment” as search terms may yield additional results. Some articles that were indexed in our included databases were unavailable for download and thus not able be considered for inclusion in this analysis (e.g., Ayala et al., 1990, Mora et al., 1974). An additional limitation of this study is that we did not define time-based inclusion criteria and therefore did not preclude articles that may be using now-outdated neurodevelopmental assessment tools. Thus, this analysis is not necessarily a reflection of the most current tools, but rather a survey of child neurodevelopmental assessment tools that have been used across Spanish-speaking Latin America over many decades. As we were not able to include studies that were not indexed in the search engines used by this study, it is likely that studies published less recently (prior to the early 2000s) are more scarcely indexed as compared to recent studies. Thus, the results may not accurately reflect the distribution of the uses of neurodevelopmental assessments over time. While this review illustrates the range of assessments used across Spanish-speaking Latin America, researchers assessing child neurodevelopment in this region may also consider reviewing prior research conducted in Latin American regions with a primary language other than Spanish, such as Brazil. Regardless of the region in which their work is conducted, researchers committed to effectively measuring and interpreting child neurodevelopmental outcome data should consider whether an assessment tool and/or its available norms have been adapted to fit the needs and demographics of modern-day society for their samples and populations of interest.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at Journal of Pediatric Psychology (http://jpepsy.oxfordjournals.org/).

Data availability

The data underlying this article are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Author contributions

Meagan E. Williams (Conceptualization [equal], Data curation [equal], Formal analysis [equal], Investigation [equal], Methodology [equal], Project administration [equal], Supervision [equal], Writing—original draft [equal], Writing—review & editing [equal]), Elizabeth A. Corn (Conceptualization [equal], Data curation [equal], Formal analysis [equal], Investigation [equal], Methodology [equal], Project administration [equal], Writing—original draft [equal], Writing—review & editing [equal]), Santiago Martinez Ransanz (Conceptualization [equal], Data curation [equal], Investigation [equal], Methodology [equal], Writing—original draft [supporting]), Madison Berl (Conceptualization [equal], Funding acquisition [equal], Methodology [equal], Supervision [equal], Writing—review & editing [equal]), Regan Andringa-Seed (Data curation [supporting], Formal analysis [supporting], Writing—review & editing [supporting]), and Sarah Mulkey (Conceptualization [equal], Funding acquisition [equal], Methodology [equal], Supervision [lead], Writing—review & editing [equal])

Funding

This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (grant number R01HD102445) (S.B.M.). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Authors also received support from the DC Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (NICHD grant number P50HD105328-01) (M.M.B.) and Thrasher Research Fund (S.B.M.).

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to recognize and thank Children’s National research librarian Susan Keller, MLS, MS-HIT, and the Global Health Initiative program at Children’s National Hospital and George Washington University, for their support of this study.

References

Aguilar
L. C.
,
Islas
A.
,
Rosique
P.
,
Hernandez
B.
,
Portillo
E.
,
Herrera
J. M.
,
Cortes
R.
,
Cruz
S.
,
Alfaro
F.
,
Martin
R.
(
1993
).
Psychometric analysis in children with mental retardation due to perinatal hypoxia treated with fibroblast growth factor (FGF) and showing improvement in mental development
.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research: JIDR
,
37
(
6
),
507
520
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.1993.tb00321.x

Aizawa
T.
(
2021
).
Dynamic variation in receptive vocabulary acquisitions: Further evidence from the Young Lives study
.
Cognitive Development
,
58
,
101031
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101031

Álvarez González
M. Á.
, ,
Carvajal Martínez
F.
,
,
Pérez Gesén
C.
,
,
Olivares Torres
A.
,
,
Fernández Yero
J. L.
,
,
Robaina Álvarez
R.
,
,
Bencomo Gómez
F.
, &
,
Álvarez Fumero
R.
(
2004
).
Pronóstico de la cognición en el hipotiroidismo congénito tratado precozmente. Hipótesis del doble efecto
.
Revista De Neurología
, 38(
6
),
513
517
. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.3806.2003396

Alvarez-Ortega
N.
,
Caballero-Gallardo
K.
,
Olivero-Verbel
J.
(
2019
).
Toxicological effects in children exposed to lead: A cross-sectional study at the Colombian Caribbean coast
.
Environ. Int
.,
130
,
104809
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2019.05.003

Ardila
A.
,
Rosselli
M.
(
1994
).
Development of language, memory, and visuospatial abilities in 5‐ to 12‐year‐old children using a neuropsychological battery
.
Dev. Neuropsychol
.,
10
(
2
),
97
120
. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649409540571

Ardila
A.
,
Rosselli
M.
,
Matute
E.
,
Inozemtseva
O.
(
2011
).
Gender differences in cognitive development
.
Developmental Psychology
,
47
(
4
),
984
990
. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023819

Ayala
H.
,
Cabrera de Coronado
H.
,
Coronado
M. A.
(
1990
).
Estimulación temprana: Su influencia sobre el desarrollo intelectual de niños de 2 a 4 años
.
Avances en Psicologia Clinica Latinoamericana
,
8
,
115
124
.

Baker
E. K.
,
Arpone
M.
,
Aliaga Vera
S.
,
Bretherton
L.
,
Ure
A.
,
Kraan
C. M.
,
Godler
D. E.
(
2019
).
Intellectual functioning and behavioural features associated with misaicism in fragile X syndrome
.
J. Neurodev. Disord..
https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-019-9288-7

Baroni
L. V.
, ,
Sampor
C.
,
,
Gonzalez
A.
,
,
Lubieniecki
F.
,
,
Lamas
G.
,
,
Rugilo
C.
,
,
Bartels
U.
,
,
Heled
A.
,
,
Smith
K. S.
,
,
Northcott
P. A.
,
,
Bouffet
E.
,
,
Alderete
D.
, &
,
Ramaswamy
V.
(
2020
).
Bridging the treatment gap in infant medulloblastoma: molecularly informed outcomes of a globally feasible regimen
.
Neuro-Oncology
, 22(
12
),
1873
1881
. https://doi.org/10.1093/neuonc/noaa122

Bauer
C. C. C.
,
Moreno
B.
,
González-Santos
L.
,
Concha
L.
,
Barquera
S.
,
Barrios
F. A.
(
2014
).
Child overweight and obesity are associated with reduced executive cognitive performance and brain alterations: A magnetic resonance imaging study in Mexican children
.
Pediatric Obesity
,
10
(
3
),
196
204
. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.241

Bauer
C. C. C.
,
Moreno
B.
,
González-Santos
L.
,
Concha
L.
,
Barquera
S.
,
Barrios
F. A.
(
2015
).
Child overweight and obesity are associated with reduced executive cognitive performance and brain alterations: a magnetic resonance imaging study in Mexican children
.
Pediatr. Obes
., (
3
),
196
204
. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.241

Bautista
A.
,
Barker
P. A.
,
Dunn
J. T.
,
Sanchez
M.
,
Kaiser
D. L.
(
1982
).
The effects of oral iodized oil on intelligence, thyroid status, and somatic growth in school-age children from an area of endemic goiter
.
Am. J. Clin. Nutr
.,
35
(
1
),
127
134
. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/35.1.127

Bedregal
P.
,
Hernández
V.
,
Mingo
M. V.
,
Castañón
C.
,
Valenzuela
P.
,
Moore
R.
,
de la Cruz
R.
,
Castro
D.
(
2016
).
Desigualdades en desarrollo infantil temprano entre prestadores públicos y privados de salud y factores asociados en la Región Metropolitana de Chile
.
Revista Chilena de Pediatría
,
87
(
5
),
351
358
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rchipe.2016.02.008

Benito-Sánchez
I.
,
Ertl
M. M.
,
Ferrer-Cascales
R.
,
Oltra-Cucarella
J.
,
Ibáñez-Alfonso
J. A.
,
Saracostti Schwartzman
M.
,
Arango-Lasprilla
J. C.
(
2020
).
Multivariate base rates of low scores on tests of learning and memory among Spanish-speaking children
.
Dev. Neuropsychol
.,
45
(
4
),
189
199
. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2019.1706519

Berl
M. M.
,
Smith
M. L.
,
Bulteau
C.
(
2017
).
ILAE survey of neuropsychology practice in pediatric epilepsy surgery evaluation
.
Epileptic Disorders: International Epilepsy Journal with Videotape
,
19
(
2
),
166
177
. https://doi.org/10.1684/epd.2017.0908

Bernal
R.
,
Fernández
C.
(
2013
).
Subsidized childcare and child development in Colombia: Effects of Hogares Comunitarios de Bienestar as a function of timing and length of exposure
.
Social Science & Medicine (1982)
,
97
,
241
249
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.10.029

Bitta
M.
,
Kariuki
S. M.
,
Abubakar
A.
,
Newton
C.
(
2017
).
Burden of neurodevelopmental disorders in low and middle-income countries: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Wellcome Open Research
,
2
,
121
. https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.13540.3

Blouin
B.
, ,
Casapia
M.
,
,
Joseph
L.
, &
,
Gyorkos
T. W.
(
2018
).
A longitudinal cohort study of soil-transmitted helminth infections during the second year of life and associations with reduced long-term cognitive and verbal abilities
.
PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases
, 12(
7
),
e0006688
. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pntd.0006688

Bojorque
G.
, ,
Torbeyns
J.
,
,
Van Hoof
Jo.
,
,
Van Nijlen
D.
, &
,
Verschaffel
L.
(
2018
).
Effectiveness of the Building Blocks program for enhancing Ecuadorian kindergartners’ numerical competencies
.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly
, 44,
231
241
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.12.009

Braun
J. M.
,
Hoffman
E.
,
Schwartz
J.
,
Sanchez
B.
,
Schnaas
L.
,
Mercado-Garcia
A.
,
Solano-Gonzalez
M.
,
Bellinger
D. C.
,
Lanphear
B. P.
,
Hu
H.
,
Tellez-Rojo
M. M.
,
Wright
R. O.
,
Hernandez-Avila
M.
(
2012
).
Assessing windows of susceptibility to lead-induced cognitive deficits in Mexican Children
.
Neurotoxicology
,
33
(
5
),
1040
1047
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2012.04.022

Brown
T. T.
,
Jernigan
T. L.
(
2012
).
Brain development during the preschool years
.
Neuropsychology Review
,
22
(
4
),
313
333
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-9214-1

Cando
R. I.
,
DíazSilva
A.
,
Guerendiain
M.
(
2019
).
The cognitive performance of indigenous schoolchildren in relation to their feeding during infancy
.
American Journal of Human Biology: The Official Journal of the Human Biology Council
,
32
(
2
),
e23326
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23326

Caraballo
R. H.
,
Cersósimo
R. O.
,
Fejerman
N.
(
2004
).
Unilateral closed-lip schizencephaly and epilepsy: A comparison with cases of unilateral polymicrogyria
.
Brain & Development
,
26
(
3
),
151
157
. https://10.1016/S0387-7604(03)00113-X

Castro
D. C.
,
Lubker
B. B.
,
Bryant
D. M.
,
Skinner
M.
(
2002
).
Oral language and reading abilities of first-grade Peruvian children: Associations with child and family factors
.
International Journal of Behavioral Development
,
26
(
4
),
334
344
. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000229

Caulfield
L.
,
Zavaleta
N.
,
Chen
P.
,
Lazarte
F.
,
Albornoz
C.
,
Putnick
D.
,
Bornstein
M.
,
DiPietro
J.
(
2011
).
Maternal zinc supplementation during pregnancy affects autonomic function of Peruvian children assessed at 54 months of age
.
The Journal of Nutrition
,
141
(
2
),
327
332
. https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.110.128843

Chasiotis
A.
,
Kiessling
F.
,
Hofer
J.
,
Campos
D.
(
2006
).
Theory of mind and inhibitory control in three cultures: Conflict inhibition predicts false belief understanding in Germany, Costa Rica and Cameroon
.
International Journal of Behavioral Development
,
30
(
3
),
249
260
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406066759.

Chinome Torres
J. D.
,
Rodríguez Barreto
L. C.
,
Parra Pulido
J. H.
(
2017
).
Implementación y evaluación de un programa de estimulación cognitiva en preescolares rurales
.
Psicología desde el Caribe
.
34
(
3
),
184
203
. https://doi.org/10.14482/psdc.34.3.11145

Clark
K. M.
,
Castillo
M.
,
Calatroni
A.
,
Walter
T.
,
Cayazzo
M.
,
Pino
P.
,
Lozoff
B.
(
2006
).
Breast-feeding and mental and motor development at 5½ years
.
Ambulatory Pediatrics: The Official Journal of the Ambulatory Pediatric Association
,
6
(
2
),
65
71
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ambp.2005.11.003

Contini de González
E. N.
(
2000
).
El relativismo cultural y las tareas cognitivas de categorización y clasificación
.
Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación Psicológica
,
10
(
2
),
65
77
.

Counter
S. A.
,
Buchanan
L. H.
,
Ortega
F.
(
2005
).
Neurocognitive impairment in lead-exposed children of Andean lead-glazing workers
.
Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine
,
47
(
3
),
306
312
. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.jom.0000155717.45594.65

Counter
S. A.
,
Buchanan
L. H.
,
Ortega
F.
,
Jacobs
A. B.
,
Laurell
G.
(
2019
).
Neurocognitive assessment of Ecuadorian Andean children living at high altitude in the Cotopaxi mountain region
.
Journal of Soil and Water Science
,
3
(
1
),
84
90
. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.36959/624/433

Counter
S. A.
,
Buchanan
L. H.
,
Rosas
H. D.
,
Ortega
F.
(
1998
).
Neurocognitive effects of chronic lead intoxication in Andean children
.
Journal of the Neurological Sciences
,
160
(
1
),
47
53
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-510X(98)00180-4

Covidence
. (n.d.). Covidence—better systematic review management. Accessed October 26, 2022. https://www.covidence.org/

Cruz
J. S.
,
Magallanes
J. S.
(
2012
).
Memoria verbal en niños de 4 a 6 años de edad y su relación con el desarrollo de habilidades escolares
.
EduPsykhé: Revista de psicología y psicopedagogía
,
11
(
1
),
3
20
.

De los Reyes-Aragon
C. J.
,
Amar Amar
J.
,
De Castro Correa
A.
,
Lewis Harb
S.
,
Madariaga
C.
,
Abello-Llanos
R.
(
2016
).
The Care and Development of Children Living in Contexts of Poverty
.
J. Child Fam. Stud
.,
25
(
12
),
3637
3643
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016-0514-6

Diuk
B.
,
Barreyro
J. P.
,
Ferroni
M.
,
Mena
M.
,
Serrano
F.
(
2019
).
Reading difficulties in low-SES children: A study of cognitive profiles
.
Journal of Cognition and Development
,
20
(
1
),
75
95
. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2018.1545656

Dix-Cooper
L.
,
Eskenazi
B.
,
Romero
C.
,
Balmes
J.
,
Smith
K. R.
(
2012
).
Neurodevelopmental performance among school age children in rural Guatemala is associated with prenatal and postnatal exposure to carbon monoxide, a marker for exposure to woodsmoke
.
Neurotoxicology
,
33
(
2
),
246
254
.

Dórea
J. G.
(
2020
).
Exposure to environmental neurotoxic substances and neurodevelopment in children from Latin America and the Caribbean
.
Environmental Research
,
192
,
110199
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2020.110199

Dunn
D. M.
(
2018
).
Peabody picture vocabulary test
(5th ed.).
NCS Pearson, Inc
.

Dunn
L. M.
,
Dunn
M.
(
2007
).
Peabody picture vocabulary test
(4th ed.).
American Guidance Service
.

Dunn
L. M.
,
Dunn
L. M.
,
Arribas
D.
(
2010
).
PEABODY. Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes
.
TEA Ediciones
.

Dunn
M.
,
Dunn
L. M.
(
1981
).
Peabody picture vocabulary test-Revised
.
American Guidance Service
.

Dunn
M.
,
Dunn
L. M.
(
1997
).
Peabody picture vocabulary test-III
.
American Guidance Service
.

Fernandes
M.
,
Krebs
N. F.
,
Westcott
J.
,
Tshefu
A.
,
Lokangaka
A.
,
Bauseman
M.
,
Hambidge
K. M.
(
2023
).
Neurodevelopment, vision and auditory outcomes at age 2 years in offspring of participants in the ‘Women First’ maternal preconception nutrition randomized controlled trial
.
Arch. Dis. Child
.,
108
,
622
631
. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2023-325352

Flórez
B. H.
,
Castro
M. F.
,
Beltrán
G.
(
1978
). Influencia del consumo mixto de grasas y proteínas, del desarrollo corporal y del CJ., sobre el sueño R.E.M.
Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología
,
10
(
2
),
173
183
.

Flores-Mendoza
C.
,
Ardila
R.
,
Gallegos
M.
,
Reategui-Colareta
N.
(
2021
).
General intelligence and socioeconomic status as strong predictors of student performance in Latin American schools: Evidence from PISA items
.
Frontiers in Education
,
6
. https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2021.632289

Fox-Fuller
J. T.
,
Artola
A.
,
Chen
K.
,
Pulsifer
M.
,
Ramirez
D.
,
Londono
N.
,
Quiroz
Y. T.
(
2021
).
Sex differences in cognitive abilities among children with the autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease presenilin 1 E280A variant from a Colombian cohort
.
JAMA Netw. Open
,
4
(
8
),
e2121697
. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.21697

Friedman
E.
,
Hazlehurst
M. F.
,
Loftus
C.
,
Karr
C.
,
McDonald
K. N.
,
Suarez-Lopez
J. R.
(
2020
).
Residential proximity to greenhouse agriculture and neurobehavioral performance in Ecuadorian children
.
Int J Hyg Environ Health
,
223
(
1
),
220
227
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijheh.2019.08.009

Galván
M.
, ,
Uauy
R.
,
,
López-Rodríguez
G.
, &
,
Kain
J.
(
2014
).
Association between childhood obesity, cognitive development, physical fitness and social-emotional wellbeing in a transitional economy
.
Annals of Human Biology
, 41(
2
),
101
106
. https://doi.org/10.3109/03014460.2013.841288

Gatica
A.
,
Bizama
M.
(
2019
).
Inteligencia fluida y creatividad: un estudio en escolares de 6 a 8 años de edad
.
Pensamiento Psicológico
,
17
(
1
),
113
120
. https://doi.org/10.11144/javerianacali.ppsi17-1.ifce

Gerez
M.
,
Tello
A.
,
Serrano
C.
,
Ibarra
R.
,
Mallet
A.
(
1999
).
Asymmetries in brain maturation and behavioral disturbances: multivariate electroencephalogram and P300 studies
.
J. Child. Neurol
.,
14
(
2
),
88
97
. https://doi.org/10.1177/088307389901400206

Gladstone
M.
,
Abubakar
A.
,
Idro
R.
,
Langfitt
J.
,
Newton
C.
(
2017
).
Measuring neurodevelopment in low-resource settings
.
The Lancet. Child & Adolescent Health
,
1
(
4
),
258
259
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(17)30117-7

Gomez
C. J.
,
Yoshikawa
H.
(
2017
).
Earthquake effects: Estimating the relationship between exposure to the 2010 Chilean earthquake and preschool children’s early cognitive and executive function skills
.
Early Child. Res. Q
.,
38
,
127
136
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.08.004

Gomez
C. J.
,
Yoshikawa
H.
(
2017
).
Earthquake effects: Estimating the relationship between exposure to the 2010 Chilean earthquake and preschool children’s early cognitive and executive function skills
.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly
,
38
,
127
136
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.08.004

Gonzalez Casanova
I.
,
Schoen
M.
,
Tandon
S.
,
Stein
A. D.
,
Barraza Villarreal
A.
,
DiGirolamo
A. M.
,
Demmelmair
H.
,
Ramirez Silva
I.
,
Feregrino
R. G.
,
Rzehak
P.
,
Stevenson
I.
,
Standl
M.
,
Schnaas
L.
,
Romieu
I.
,
Koletzko
B.
,
Ramakrishnan
U.
(
2021
).
Maternal FADS2 single nucleotide polymorphism modified the impact of prenatal docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) supplementation on child neurodevelopment at 5 years: Follow-up of a randomized clinical trial
.
Clinical Nutrition (Edinburgh, Scotland)
,
40
(
10
),
5339
5345
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clnu.2021.08.026

González-Cortés
T.
,
Gutiérrez-Contreras
E.
,
Espino-Silva
P. K.
,
Haro-Santa Cruz
J.
,
Álvarez-Cruz
D.
,
Rosales-González
C. C.
,
Sida-Godoy
C.
,
Nava-Hernández
M. P.
,
López-Márquez
F. C.
,
Ruiz-Flores
P.
(
2019
).
Clinical profile of autism spectrum disorder in a pediatric population from Northern Mexico
.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
,
49
(
11
),
4409
4420
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04154-2

Grandjean
P.
,
Hu
H.
,
Till
C.
,
Green
R.
,
Bashash
M.
,
Flora
D.
,
Tellez-Rojo
M. M.
,
Song
P. X. K.
,
Lanphear
B.
,
Budtz-Jørgensen
E.
(
2022
).
A benchmark dose analysis for maternal pregnancy urine-fluoride and IQ in children
.
Risk Analysis: An Official Publication of the Society for Risk Analysis
,
42
(
3
),
439
449
. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13767

Grizzle
R.
,
Davis
A. S.
(
2011
). Woodcock-Johnson cognitive-achievement battery. In
Kreutzer
J. S.
,
DeLuca
J.
,
Caplan
B.
(Eds.),
Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology
.
Springer
. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_1501

Hartwig
E.
(
2020
). Guidance on the importance of quality in early childhood learning and education in Latin America and the Caribbean. United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF). https://www.unicef.org/lac/media/11066/file/Importance-Quality-ECE-LAC.pdf

Hermida
M. J.
, ,
Shalom
D. E.
,
,
Segretin
M. S.
,
,
Goldin
A. P.
,
,
Abril
M. C.
,
,
Lipina
S. J.
, &
,
Sigman
M.
(
2019
).
Risks for Child Cognitive Development in Rural Contexts
.
Frontiers in Psychology
, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02735

Herrera-Chinchay
L.
,
Silva-Ocas
I.
,
Castro-Silva
N.
,
Del Águila VIllar
C.
(
2021
).
Desarrollo social, cognitivo y psicomotor en niños peruanos con hipotiroidismo congénito
.
Andes Pediatrica: Revista Chilena de Pediatria
,
92
(
2
),
235
240
. https://doi.org/10.32641/andespediatr.v92i2.1955

Hill
C. M.
,
Dimitriou
D.
,
Baya
A.
,
Webster
R.
,
Gavlak-Dingle
J.
,
Lesperance
V.
,
Heathcote
K.
,
Bucks
R. S.
(
2014
).
Cognitive performance in high-altitude Andean residents compared with low-altitude populations: From childhood to older age
.
Neuropsychology
,
28
(
5
),
752
760
. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000065

Jofrě
J. M.
,
Jofré
M. J.
,
Arenas
M. C.
,
Azpiroz
R.
,
de Bortoli
M. Á.
(
2007
).
Importancia del desayuno en el estado nutricional y procesamiento de la información en escolares
.
Universitas Psychologica
,
6
(
2
),
371
382
.

Johnston
F. E.
,
Low
S. M.
,
de Baessa
Y.
,
MacVean
R. B.
(
1987
).
Interaction of nutritional and socioeconomic status as determinants of cognitive development in disadvantaged urban Guatemalan children
.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology
,
73
(
4
),
501
506
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330730412

Juric
L. C.
,
Richards
M. M.
,
Introzzi
I.
,
Andrés
M. L.
,
Urquijo
S.
(
2013
).
Development patterns of executive functions in children
.
The Spanish Journal of Psychology
,
16
,
E41
. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.44

Kezer
F.
,
Arik
R. S.
(
2012
).
An examination and comparison of the revisions of the Wechsler intelligence scale for children
.
Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences
,
46
,
2104
2110
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.05.436

Kihn
T.
(
2005
). The appropriateness of the Raven’s coloured progressive matrices and its existing normative data for Zulu speakers [thesis]. University of KwaZulu-Natal. Accessed July 5, 2023. https://ukzn-dspace.ukzn.ac.za/handle/10413/1890

Knauer
H. A.
,
Ozer
E. J.
,
Dow
W. H.
,
Fernald
L. C. H.
(
2018
).
Parenting quality at two developmental periods in early childhood and their association with child development
.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly
,
47
,
396
404
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.08.009

Korzeniowski
C.
,
Cupani
M.
,
Ison
M.
,
Difabio
H.
(
2017
).
School performance and poverty: The mediating role of executive functions
.
Electronic Journal of Research in Education Psychology
,
14
(
40
),
474
494
. https://doi.org/10.14204/ejrep.40.15152

Kozulin
A.
,
Lebeer
J.
,
Madella-Noja
A.
,
Gonzalez
F.
,
Jefrey
I.
,
Rosenthal
N.
,
Koslowsky
M.
(
2010
).
Cognitive modifiability of children with developmental disabilities: a multicentre study using Fuerstein’s Instrumental Enrichment–Basic program
.
Res. Dev. Disabil
,
31
(
2
),
551
559
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2009.12.001

Kupsco
A.
,
Estrada-Gutierrez
G.
,
Cantoral
A.
,
Schnaas
L.
,
Pantic
I.
,
Amarasiriwardena
C.
,
Svensson
K.
,
Bellinger
D. C.
,
Téllez-Rojo
M. M.
,
Baccarelli
A. A.
,
Wright
R. O.
(
2020
).
Modification of the effects of prenatal manganese exposure on child neurodevelopment by maternal anemia and iron deficiency
.
Pediatric Research
,
88
(
2
),
325
333
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-020-0754-4

Laborda
L.
,
Elosúa
M. R.
,
Gómez-Veiga
I.
(
2019
).
Ethnicity and intelligence in children exposed to poverty environments: An analysis using the Oaxaca-Blinder decomposition
.
Intelligence
,
72
,
49
58
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2018.11.008

Lacunza
A. B.
,
de Gonzáalez
N. C.
,
Solano
A. C.
(
2010
).
Las habilidades cognitivas en niños preescolares: Un estudio comparativo en un contexto de pobreza
.
Acta Colombiana de Psicología
,
13
(
1
),
25
34
. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2011-03384-002

LaForte
E. M.
,
Wendling
B. J.
,
Mather
N.
,
Schrank
F. A.
,
McGrew
K. S.
(
2019
). Batería IV Woodcock-Muñoz. Technical Abstract (Batería IV Assessment Service Bulletin No. 1). Itasca, IL: Riverside Insights. Accessed July 5, 2023. https://7083436.fs1.hubspotusercontent-na1.net/hubfs/7083436/BateriaIV_ASB1_021319.pdf

Lamminmäki
T.
,
Ahonen
T.
,
de Barra
H. T.
,
Tolvanen
A.
,
Michelsson
K.
,
Lyytinen
H.
(
1997
).
Comparing efficacies of neurocognitive treatment and homework assistance programs for children with learning difficulties
.
Journal of Learning Disabilities
,
30
(
3
),
333
345
. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221949703000308

Levin
E.
(
2011
). McCarthy scales of children’s abilities. In
Goldstein
S.
,
Naglieri
J. A.
(Eds.),
Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development
(pp.
928
929
).
Springer
. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79061-9_1732

Leyva
D.
,
Weiland
C.
,
Barata
M.
,
Yoshikawa
H.
,
Snow
C.
,
Treviño
E.
,
Rolla
A.
(
2015
).
Teacher–child interactions in Chile and their associations with prekindergarten outcomes
.
Child Development
,
86
(
3
),
781
799
. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12342

Lohndorf
R. T.
,
Vermeer
H. J.
,
Cárcamo
R. A.
,
De la Harpe
C.
,
Mesman
J.
(
2019
).
Preschoolers’ problem behavior, prosocial behavior, and language ability in a Latin-American context: The roles of child executive functions and socialization environments
.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly
,
48
,
36
49
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.02.005

Louison
K.
(
2016
). Assessing the cross-cultural validity of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th edition) for use in Trinidad and Tobago [Doctoral thesis]. Goldsmiths, University of London. Accessed July 5, 2023. https://research.gold.ac.uk/id/eprint/18733/

Lozoff
B.
,
Jimenez
E.
,
Smith
J. B.
(
2006
).
Double burden of iron deficiency in infancy and low socioeconomic status: A longitudinal analysis of cognitive test scores to age 19 years
.
Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine
,
160
(
11
),
1108
1113
. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.160.11.1108.

Lu
C.
,
Essig
C.
,
Root
C.
,
Rohlman
D. S.
,
McDonald
T.
,
Sulzbacher
S.
(
2009
).
Assessing the association between pesticide exposure and cognitive development in rural Costa Rican children living in organic and conventional coffee farms
.
Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health
,
21
(
4
),
609
621
. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijamh.2009.21.4.609

Lumpkin
L.
(
2023
). Implementation of care for child development in Latin America and the Caribbean. United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF). https://www.unicef.org/lac/media/41901/file/Implementation-of-ccd-in-lac.pdf

Marincovich
R. I.
,
Forno Sparosvich
H.
,
Durán Santana
M. C.
,
Hazbún Game
J.
,
Castro Gómez
C.
,
Marincovich
D. I.
(
2000
).
Estudio de la capacidad intellectual (Test de Matrices Progresivas de Raven) en escolares chilenos de 5 a 18 años I. Antecedentes generales, normas y recomendaciones
.
Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada
,
53
(
1
),
5
30
.

Manrique Millones
D.
,
Flores-Mendoza
C.
,
Millones Rivalles
R.
(
2015
).
Intelligence in Peru: Students’ results in Raven and its relationship to SES
.
Intelligence
,
51
,
71
78
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.05.004

Martínez-Cruz
C. F.
,
Poblano
A.
,
Fernández-Carrocera
L. A.
,
Jiménez-Quiróz
R.
,
Tuyú-Torres
N.
(
2006
).
Association between intelligence quotient scores and extremely low birth weight in school-age children
.
Arch. Med. Res
.,
37
(
5
),
639
645
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arcmed.2005.12.001

Matute
E.
,
Inozemtseva
O.
,
González Reyes
A. L.
,
Chamorro
Y.
(
2022
).
La Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil (ENI): Historia y fundamentos teóricos de su validación. Un acercamiento práctico a su uso y valor diagnóstico
.
Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias
,
14
(
1
),
68
95
. Accessed August 8, 2023. http://revistaneurociencias.com/index.php/RNNN/article/view/44.

Matute
E.
,
Roselli
M.
,
Ardila
A.
,
Ostrosky
F.
(
2007
).
Evaluación neuropsicológica infantil
.
Manual Moderno
.

Matute
E.
,
Roselli
M.
,
Ardila
A.
,
Ostrosky
F.
(
2013
).
Evaluación neuropsicológica infantile
(2nd ed.).
Manual Moderno
.

Matute
E.
,
Roselli
M.
,
Beltrán-Navarro
B.
,
Ardila
A.
(
2021
).
Evaluación neuropsicológica infantil para preescolares
.
Manual Moderno
.

McCarthy
D.
(
2004
). Escalas McCarthy de Aptitudes y Psicomotricidad para Niños (7th ed.). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

McInnes
L. A.
,
González
P. J.
,
Manghi
E. R.
,
Esquivel
M.
,
Monge
S.
,
Delgado
M. F.
,
Fournier
E.
,
Bondy
P.
,
Castelle
K.
(
2005
).
A genetic study of autism in Costa Rica: Multiple variables affecting IQ scores observed in a preliminary sample of autistic cases
.
BMC Psychiatry
,
5
(
15
). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-5-15

McKay
H.
,
Sinisterra
L.
,
McKay
A.
,
Gomez
H.
,
Lloreda
P.
(
1978
).
Improving cognitive ability in chronically deprived children
.
Science (New York, N.Y.)
,
200
(
4339
),
270
278
. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.635585

Moguel-Ancheita
S.
, ,
Ramírez-Sibaja
S.
,
,
Bolde
C.-P.
, &
,
Orozco-Gómez
L. P.
(
2008
).
[Study of depression and sensorial functions in children with strabismus. First phase]
.
Cirugia y Cirujanos
, 76(
2
),
101
107
. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18492430/

Mora
J. O.
,
Amezquita
A.
,
Castro
L.
,
Christiansen
N.
,
Clement-Murphy
J.
,
Cobos
L. F.
,
Cremer
H. D.
,
Dragastin
S.
,
Elias
M. F.
,
Franklin
D.
,
Herrera
M. G.
,
Ortiz
N.
,
Pardo
F.
,
de Paredes
B.
,
Ramos
C.
,
Riley
R.
,
Rodriguez
H.
,
Vuori-Christiansen
L.
,
Wagner
M.
,
Stare
F. J.
(
1974
).
Nutrition, health and social factors related to intellectual performance
.
World Review of Nutrition and Dietetics
,
19
,
205
236
. https://doi.org/10.1159/000394769

Morales
G.
,
Matute
E.
,
Murray
J.
,
Hardy
D. J.
,
O'Callaghan
E. T.
,
Tlacuilo-Parra
A.
(
2013
).
Is executive function intact after pediatric intracranial hemorrhage? A sample of Mexican children with hemophilia
.
Clinical Pediatrics
,
52
(
10
),
950
959
. https://doi.org/10.1177/0009922813495311

Morales-Buenrostro
L. E.
, ,
Alberu
J.
,
,
Mancilla-Urrea
E.
,
,
Vélez-García
A.
,
,
Espinoza-Pérez
R.
,
,
Cruz-Santiago
J.
,
,
Parra-Michel
R.
,
,
Parra-Avila
I.
,
,
Flores-Nava
G.
,
,
Caballero-Andrade
G.
,
,
Niebla-Cardenas
A.
,
,
Pérez-Avendaño
R.
,
,
Angulo-Dominguez
A.
,
,
Lascarez
S.
, &
,
Sánchez-Román
S.
(
2019
).
Intellectual performance of kidney transplant recipients’ offspring: a cross-sectional, multicenter study
.
The Journal of Maternal-Fetal & Neonatal Medicine
, 32(
4
),
542
549
. https://doi.org/10.1080/14767058.2017.1384805

Moreno-Villagómez
J.
,
Yáñez-Téllez
G.
,
Prieto-Corona
B.
,
Subert-Ravelo
A. N.
,
García
A.
(
2021
).
Cognitive performance of preschool children with different types of non-syndromic craniosynostosis
.
Brain Impair
.,
22
(
2
),
125
134
. https://doi.org/10.1017/BrImp.2020.7

Munoz-Chereau
B.
,
Ang
L.
,
Dockrell
J.
,
Outhwaite
L.
,
Heffernan
C.
(
2021
).
Measuring early child development across low and middle-income countries: A systematic review
.
Journal of Early Childhood Research
,
19
(
4
),
443
470
.

Muñoz-Sandoval
A. F.
,
Woodcock
R. W.
,
McGrew
K. S.
,
Mather
N.
(
2015
).
Batería III Woodcock-Muñoz
.
Ciencias Psicológicas
,
3
(
2
),
245
246
. https://doi.org/10.22235/cp.v3i2.155

Neils-Strunjaš
J.
(
1998
). Chapter 3—Clinical assessment strategies: Evaluation of language comprehension and production by formal test batteries. In
Stemmer
B.
,
Whitaker
H.
(Eds.),
A handbook of neurolinguistics
(pp.
71
82
).
Academic Press
. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012666055-5/50007-1

Nogueira
G. J.
,
Castro
A.
,
Naveira
L.
,
Nogueira-Antuñano
F.
,
Natinzon
A.
,
Gigli
S. L.
,
Marchesi
M.
(
2005
).
Evaluation of the higher brain functions in 1st and 7th grade schoolchildren belonging to two different socioeconomic groups
.
Rev. Neurol
.,
40
(
7
),
397
406
.

Ortiz-Andrellucchi
A.
,
Peña-Quintana
L.
,
Saavedra-Santana
P.
,
Albino-Beñacar
A.
,
Monckeberg-Barros
F.
,
Serra-Majem
L.
(
2009
).
Facing malnutrition and poverty: Evaluating the CONIN experience
.
Nutrition Reviews
,
67 Suppl 1
,
S47
55
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.2009.00159.x

Otero
G. A.
,
Aguirre
D. M.
,
Porcayo
R.
,
Fernández
T.
(
1999
).
Psychological and electroencephalographic study in school children with iron deficiency
.
Int. J. Neurosci
,
99
(
1-4
),
113
121
. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459908994318

Osorio-Valencia
E.
,
Torres-Sánchez
L.
,
López-Carrillo
L.
,
Rothenberg
S. J.
,
Schnaas
L.
(
2018
).
Early motor development and cognitive abilities among Mexican preschoolers
.
Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence
,
24
(
8
),
1015
1025
. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2017.1354979

Page
M. J.
,
Moher
D.
,
Bossuyt
P. M.
,
Boutron
I.
,
Hoffmann
T. C.
,
Mulrow
C. D.
,
Shamseer
L.
,
Tetzlaff
J. M.
,
Akl
E. A.
,
Brennan
S. E.
,
Chou
R.
,
Glanville
J.
,
Grimshaw
J. M.
,
Hróbjartsson
A.
,
Lalu
M. M.
,
Li
T.
,
Loder
E. W.
,
Mayo-Wilson
E.
,
McDonald
S.
,
McKenzie
J. E.
(
2021
).
PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews
.
BMJ (Clinical Research ed.)
,
372
,
n160
. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n160

Pineda
D. A.
,
Puerta
I. C.
,
Aguirre
D. C.
,
García-Barrera
M. A.
,
Kamphaus
R. W.
(
2007
).
The role of neuropsychologic tests in the diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
.
Pediatr. Neurol
.,
36
(
6
),
373
381
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2007.02.002

Pineda
D. A.
,
Lopera
F.
,
Puerta
I. C.
,
Trujillo-Orrego
N.
,
Aguirre-Acevedo
D. C.
,
Hincapié-Henao
L.
,
Arcos-Burgos
Mauricio
(
2011
).
Potential cognitive endophenotypes in multigenerational families: segregating ADHD from a genetic isolate
.
Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord
.,
3
(
3
),
291
299
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-011-0061-3

Pinto-Dussán
M. C.
,
Aguilar-Mejía
O. M.
,
Gómez-Rojas
J. D.
(
2010
).
Estrés psicológico materno como posible factor de riesgo prenatal para el desarrollo de dificultades cognoscitivas: Caracterización neuropsicologica de una muestra colombiana
.
Universitas Psychologica
,
9
(
3
),
749
759
.

Porras-Kattz
E.
,
Harmony
T.
,
Ricardo-Garcell
J.
,
Galán
L.
,
Fernández
T.
,
Prado-Alcalá
R.
,
Avecilla-Ramírez
G.
,
Sánchez-Moreno
L.
,
Barrera-Reséndiz
J.
,
Corsi-Cabrera
M.
,
Valencia-Solís
E.
(
2011
).
Magnesium valproate in learning disabled children with interictal paroxysmal EEG patterns: Preliminary report
.
Neuroscience Letters
,
492
(
2
),
99
104
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.065

Puche-Navarro
R.
,
Millán
R.
(
2007
).
Inferential functioning in visually impaired children
.
Research in Developmental Disabilities
,
28
(
3
),
249
265
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2006.01.003

Raven
J.
(
1936
). Raven Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM). APA PsycTests. https://doi.org/10.1037/t07027-000

Reynolds
S. A.
(
2022
).
Center-based child care and differential improvements in the child development outcomes of disadvantaged children
.
Child & Youth Care Forum
,
51
(
2
),
395
420
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-021-09634-0

Rico
J. A.
,
Kordas
K.
,
López
P.
,
Rosado
J. L.
,
Vargas
G. G.
,
Ronquillo
D.
,
Stoltzfus
R. J.
(
2006
).
Efficacy of iron and/or zinc supplementation on cognitive performance of lead-exposed Mexican schoolchildren: A randomized, placebo-controlled trial
.
Pediatrics
,
117
(
3
),
e518
e527
. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1172

Rindermann
H.
,
Stiegmaier
E.-M.
,
Meisenberg
G.
(
2015
).
Cognitive ability of preschool, primary and secondary school children in Costa Rica
.
Journal of Biosocial Science
,
47
(
3
),
281
310
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021932014000066

Romero
J. B.
,
Palacios
G. C.
,
Gómez
N.
,
Silva
A.
,
Fabela
J. H.
(
2011
).
Intelligence quotient related with congenital hypothyroidism etiology
.
Rev. Med. Inst. Mex. Seguro Soc
.,
49
(
2
),
179
183
.

Rosales-Rueda
M.
(
2018
).
The impact of early life shocks on human capital formation: Evidence from El Niño floods in Ecuador
.
Journal of Health Economics
,
62
,
13
44
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2018.07.003

Rosselli
M.
,
Ardila
A.
,
Bateman
J. R.
,
Guzmán
M.
(
2001
).
Neuropsychological test scores, academic performance, and developmental disorders in Spanish-speaking children
.
Developmental Neuropsychology
,
20
(
1
),
355
373
. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN2001_3

Rubio-Codina
M.
,
Grantham-McGregor
S.
(
2019
)
Evolution of the wealth gap in child development and mediating pathways: Evidence from a longitudinal study in Bogota, Colombia
.
Developmental Science
,
22
(
5
),
e12810
. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12810

Salinas-Piélago
J. E.
, ,
Vega-Dienstmaier
J. M.
, &
,
Rojas-Oblitas
M.
(
1998
).
[Effect of biscuits fortified with haem iron on the intellectual status of pre-school children]
.
Revista De Neurologia
, 27(
157
),
400
404
. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9774808/

Salinas-Piélago
J. E.
,
Vega-Dienstmaier
J. M.
,
Rojas-Oblitas
M.
(
1998
).
Effect of biscuits fortified with haem iron on the intellectual status of pre-school children
.
Revista De Neurologia
,
27
(
157
),
400
404
.

Sánchez
E. Y.
,
Velarde
S.
,
Britton
G. B.
(
2011
).
Estimated prevalence of attention-deficient/hyperactivity disorder in a sample of Panamanian school-aged children
.
Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev
.,
42
(
2
),
243
255
. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-010-0213-2

Sánchez-Escobedo
P.
,
Esquivel-Ancona
F.
,
Hollingworth
L.
(
2016
).
Intellectual assessment of children and youth in Mexico: Past, present, and future
.
International Journal of School & Educational Psychology
,
4
(
4
),
247
253
. https://doi.org/10.1080/21683603.2016.1163745

Sánchez-Escudero
J.
,
Medina-Gómez
C.
,
Gómez-Toro
Y.
(
2019
).
Academic skills and processing speed. Predictive models of school achievement in primary school
.
Psychologia
,
13
(
1
),
25
39
. https://doi.org/10.21500/19002386.3754

Sands
R.
,
D’Amato
R. C.
(
2017
). McCarthy scales of children’s abilities. In: Kreutzer, J., DeLuca, J., & Caplan, B. (eds) Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_1462-3

Sanz
J. H.
,
Anixt
J.
,
Bear
L.
,
Basken
A.
,
Beca
J.
,
Marino
B. S.
,
Mussatto
K. A.
,
Nembhard
W. N.
,
Sadhwani
A.
,
Sananes
R.
,
Shekerdemian
L. S.
,
Sood
E.
,
Uzark
K.
,
Willen
E.
,
Ilardi
D.
(
2021
).
Characterisation of neurodevelopmental and psychological outcomes in CHD: A research agenda and recommendations from the cardiac neurodevelopmental outcome collaborative
.
Cardiology in the Young
,
31
(
6
),
876
887
. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1047951121002146

Segretin
M S.
,
Lipina
S. J.
,
Hermida
M. J.
,
Sheffield
T. D.
,
Nelson
J. M.
,
Espy
K. A.
,
Colombo
J. A.
(
2014
).
Predictors of cognitive enhancement after training in preschoolers from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds
.
Front. Psychol
.,
5
. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00205

Schady
N.
(
2006
). Early childhood development in Latin America and the Caribbean. World Bank. Policy Research Working Paper. https://documents.worldbank.org/pt/publication/documents-reports/documentdetail/589551468016812642/early-childhood-development-in-latin-america-and-the-caribbean

Schnaas
L.
,
Rothenberg
S. J.
,
Perroni
E.
,
Martínez
S.
,
Hernández
C.
,
Hernández
R. M.
(
2000
).
Temporal pattern in the effect of postnatal blood lead level on intellectual development of young children
.
Neurotoxicology and Teratology
,
22
(
6
),
805
810
. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-0362(00)00101-x.

Semrud-Clikeman
M.
,
Romero
R. A. A.
,
Prado
E. L.
,
Shapiro
E. G.
,
Bangirana
P.
,
John
C. C.
(
2017
).
Selecting measures for neurodevelopmental assessment in children in low and middle-income countries
.
Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence
,
23
(
7
),
761
802
. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2016.1216536

Shuttleworth-Edwards
A. B.
(
2016
).
Generally representative is representative of none: Commentary on the pitfalls of IQ test standardization in multicultural settings
.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist
,
30
(
7
),
975
998
. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2016.1204011

Statucka
M.
,
Cherian
K.
,
Fasano
A.
,
Munhoz
R. P.
,
Cohn
M.
(
2021
).
Multiculturalism: A challenge for cognitive screeners in Parkinson's disease
.
Movement Disorders Clinical Practice
,
8
(
5
),
733
742
. https://doi.org/10.1002/mdc3.13240

Talero-Guttierez
C.
,
Van Meerbeke
A. V.
,
Reyes
R. G.
(
2012
).
A clinical study of ADHD symptoms with relation to symptoms of learning disorders in schoolchildren in Bogota, Colombia
.
J. Atten. Disord
.,
16
(
2
),
157
63
. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054710383116

Torrado
M.
,
Araoz
V.
,
Baialardo
E.
,
Abraldes
K.
,
Mazza
C.
,
Krochik
G.
,
Chertkoff
L.
(
2007
).
Clinical-etiologic correlation in children with Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS): an interdisciplinary study
.
Am. J. Med. Genet. A
.,
143A
(
5
),
460
468
. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.31520

Treviño
M.
,
Beltrán-Navarro
B.
,
León
R. M.-C. Y.
,
Matute
E.
(
2021
).
Clustering of neuropsychological traits of preschoolers
.
Scientific Reports
,
11
(
1
),
6533
. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-85891-2

UNESCO
. (
2010
). Early childhood care and education, regional report: Latin America and the Caribbean. Report Prepared for the World Conference on Early Childhood Care and Education. United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization. https://unesdoc.unesco.org/ark:/48223/pf0000189212

Ugarte
L. F.
,
Mondaca
C.
,
Ortiz
C.
,
Quiroz
J.
,
Vásquez
K.
,
Morales
C.
(
2019
).
Use of TEVI-R to describe passive vocabulary in children belonging to a Mapuche community. Communication Disorders
,
Audiology and Swallowing
,
31
(
5
),
1
7
. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20182018130

Urazán-Torres
G. R.
,
Puche-Cabrera
M. J.
,
Caballero-Forero
M.
,
Rey-Anacona
C. A.
(
2013
).
Cognitive and executive functions in Colombian school children with conduct disorder: Sex differences
.
Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatria
,
42
(
4
),
324
332
. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0034-7450(13)70029-5

van Wendel de Joode
B.
,
Mora
A. M.
,
Lindh
C. H.
,
Hernández-Bonilla
D.
,
Córdoba
L.
,
Wesseling
C.
,
Hoppin
J. A.
,
Mergler
D.
(
2016
).
Pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in children aged 6-9 years from Talamanca, Costa Rica
.
Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior
,
85
,
137
150
. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.09.003

Varela
V.
,
Torres
F.
,
Rosselli
M.
,
Quezada
C.
(
2020
).
Neuropsychological assessment of Chilean children with a history of extreme prematurity: An exploratory study
.
Applied Neuropsychology. Child
,
9
(
1
),
56
67
. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2018.1510328

Vega-Dienstmaier
J. M.
,
Salinas Piélago
J. E.
,
Gutiérrez-Campos
M.
,
Mandamiento-Ayquipa
R. D.
,
Yara-Hokama
M.
,
Ponce-Canchihuamán
J.
,
Castro-Morales
J.
(
2006
).
Lead levels and cognitive abilities in Peruvian children
.
Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria
,
28
(
1
),
33
39
. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462006000100008

Velez van Meerbeke
A.
,
Talero-Gutierrez
C.
,
Gonzalez-Reyes
R.
(
2007
).
Prevalence of delayed neurodevelopment in children from Bogotá, Colombia, South America
.
Neuroepidemiology
,
29
(
1
2
),
74
77
. https://doi.org/10.1159/000109499

Vera Noriega
J. A.
,
Peña Ramos
M. O.
(
2005
).
Desarrollo, estimulación y estrés de la crianza en infantes rurales de México
.
Apuntes de Psicología
,
23
(
3
),
305
319
. https://www.apuntesdepsicologia.es/index.php/revista/article/view/100

Virués-Ortega
J.
,
Bucks
R.
,
Kirkham
F. J.
,
Baldeweb
T.
,
Baya-Botti
A.
,
Hogan
. A. M.
(
2011
).
Changing patterns of neuropsychological functioning in children living at high altitude above and below 4000 m: A report from the Bolivian Children Living at Altitude (BoCLA) study
.
Dev. Sci
.,
5
,
1185
1193
. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01064.x

Warthon-Medina
M.
,
QUalter
P.
,
Zavaleta
N.
,
Dillon
S.
,
Lazarte
F.
,
Lowe
N. M.
(
2015
).
The long term impact of micronutrient supplementation during infancy on cognition and executive function performance in pre-school children
.
Nutrients
,
7
(
8
),
6606
6627
. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7085302

Wechsler
D
. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Fourth Edition,
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation
,
2012
.

Wechsler
D
. The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Third Edition (WPPSI-III),
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation
,
2002
.

Wechsler
D
. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Revised,
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
,
1989.

World Education Network
. (n.d.). K-12 education system of Colombia. Retrieved January 26, 2023. https://www.colombiaeducation.info/k12/colombian-k-12-education-system.html

Zambrano-Sánchez
E.
,
del Consuelo Martínez-Wbaldo
M.
,
Poblano
A.
(
2010
).
Risk factor frequency for learning disabilities in low socioeconomic level preschool children in Mexico city
.
Revista Latino-Americana de Enfermagem
,
18
(
5
),
998
1004
. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-11692010000500022

Zika Virus Individual Participant Data Consortium
. (
2020
).
The Zika Virus Individual Participant Data Consortium: A global initiative to estimate the effects of exposure to zika virus during pregnancy on adverse fetal, infant, and child health outcomes
.
Tropical Medicine and Infectious Disease
,
5
(
4
),
152
.

Zimerman
R.
, ,
Figueroa
J.
,
,
Gutiérrez
R.
, &
,
Cortés
T.
(
1982
).
Logros en el desarrollo cognoscitivo de niños mexicanos de 0 a 6 años: Reporte preliminar
.
Salud Mental
, 5(
2
),
82
89
.

Author notes

M.E.W. and E.A.C. contributed equally.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/pages/standard-publication-reuse-rights)

Supplementary data