Abstract

Stereotypies that develop spontaneously in nonhuman primates can provide an effective model for repetitive stereotyped behavior in people with neurodevelopmental or obsessive-compulsive disorders. The behaviors are similar in form, are similarly affected by environmental conditions, and are improved with similar treatment methods such as enrichment, training, and drug therapy. However, because of a greater number of commonalities in these factors, nonhuman primates may serve as a better model for stereotyped behavior in individuals with autism or intellectual disability than for compulsions in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Because animal models may not be exact in all features of the disorder being studied, it is important to investigate the strengths and weaknesses of using a nonhuman primate model for stereotyped behavior in people with psychological disorders.

Introduction

Stereotypies

Stereotypies can be defined as repetitive, unvarying, and seemingly functionless behavior patterns that are reported to occur in a variety of species (Mason 1991). They are a heterogeneous group of behaviors that vary in form, frequency, and situation and include behaviors such as pacing, rearing, and head twirling in mink (Mason 1993a); stereotyped walking and swimming bouts in polar bears (Wechsler 1991); repetitive jumping in bank voles (Cooper and Nicol 1991); crib-biting and weaving in horses (Dallaire 1993); chain chewing in pigs (Dantzer 1986; Wiepkema and Schouten 1992); self-clasping, rocking, eye poking, and pacing in nonhuman primates (Capitanio 1986; Erwin and Deni 1979); and rocking, eye poking, and hair twirling in humans (Berkson and Davenport 1962; Forehand and Baumeister 1971a; Foster 1998). These behaviors often lack variability in response to environmental change, and in humans, they tend to be inappropriate relative to general developmental level (Berkson 1983). Stereotypies also vary in causal factors, whether induced by environmental conditions, drug administration, or psychiatric disorders (Mason 1993b). In some cases, stereotypies may have resulted from the associated pain or discomfort of clinical disorders. In other cases, they may have resulted from psychopathological disorders or poor rearing conditions (Novak et al. 2012). Determining whether or not a behavior is a stereotypy can be difficult. Often there is a continuum between stereotypies and normal behavior patterns; stereotypies may be incorporated into normal behavior patterns, and many normal behavior patterns are stereotyped. Therefore, in some cases drawing a line between normal and abnormal behavior can be arbitrary (Mason 1993b).

Nonhuman Primate Model for the Human Condition

Stereotypies that develop spontaneously in animal populations can serve as valuable models for behavioral disorders in humans; they provide additional tools for investigating the physiological, developmental, and environmental impact on the behavior. Nonhuman primates are an especially valuable model for behavioral disorders in the human population, because, unlike rodent models, nonhuman primates have a prolonged developmental period, higher cognitive processing, complex social interactions, a more diversified behavioral repertoire, and they share a closer evolutionary history. In this regard, captive nonhuman primates with abnormal stereotyped behaviors may serve as a potential model for humans with psychiatric or neurodevelopmental disorders who display similar behaviors. Although the form of the behavior may not necessarily be equivalent across species, it is reasonable to consider that similar mechanisms may be involved (Ridley and Baker 1982). However, animal models are useful only to the extent that they generalize to the human condition being studied (Suomi 1986). To be effective, an animal model should include, among other criteria, face validity (e.g., similarity in behavioral output), construct validity (e.g., similarity in cause or origin), and predictive validity (e.g., similar treatment responses) (Albelda and Joel 2012; Suomi 1986; Willner 1986). Given the experiential and cognitive differences between nonhuman and human primates and the complexity of human psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders, nonhuman primates will likely not serve as a complete model. Therefore, it is important to understand both the strengths and weaknesses of using nonhuman primates as a model for the human condition.

Assessing Behavior in Human and Nonhuman Primates

Behavior can be assessed in a number of ways in both human and nonhuman primates, ranging from direct report to behavioral assessment and physiological measures. However, the method of choice depends both on the question being asked and on the capabilities of the individual. One method of assessing behavior in human research is by self-report of individuals or groups, which allows the person to give a direct account of internal states (Diener et al. 2003; Miguel et al. 1995). However, results from self-report may be potentially problematic because the subject might not be able to correctly identify internal states or may intentionally give an incorrect response (Brune M. et al. 2006). In cases where individuals are not capable of providing a self-report, information can also be obtained from family, friends, or caregivers (Diener et al. 2003; Fisher et al. 1997; Wieseler et al. 1985) or by direct observation of behavior (Berkson and Davenport 1962; Forehand and Baumeister 1970). Additionally, physiological measures can be used to study internal states associated with stereotyped behavior. For example, heart rate was measured in school children during stereotypical leg-swinging (Soussignan and Koch 1985) and in severely mentally handicapped children during complex repetitive hand movements (Young and Clements 1979). Measures of cortisol were also collected from plasma in a study of patients at a developmental center who exhibited stereotyped behavior (Sandman et al. 1990). Because direct self-report measures are not possible in many human subjects, such alternative methods can be informative when assessing behavior.

Using animal models for studying behavior in humans can have some challenges, in part because mental states are difficult to measure or verify. Although self-report cannot be used with nonhuman primates, cognitive bias testing, a relatively novel form of assessing the internal state of an animal, is a potential measure (Mendl et al. 2009). In a study of 16 capuchin monkeys, those displaying high levels of stereotypic head twirls exhibited negative cognitive bias, whereas pacing was not correlated with cognitive bias, suggesting that some behaviors may be more indicative of the animal's affective state than others (Pomerantz et al. 2012). Additionally, investigators often rely on ratings obtained from individuals familiar with the animals (Stevenson-Hinde and Zunz 1978) or by direct observation. Behavioral assessments, which involve observation and data collection by trained human observers, are routinely conducted at primate facilities. These assessments often help to identify individuals exhibiting stereotypies (Baker et al. 2007; Lutz et al. 2003; Vandeleest et al. 2011) or to assess the impact of interventions on abnormal behavior (Baker et al. 2009; Novak et al. 1998). As with humans, physiological measures can also be used in nonhuman primates to study internal states associated with stereotyped behavior. For example, heart rate was measured in rhesus macaques to assess self-biting behavior (Marinus et al. 1999), and fecal cortisol was measured in capuchin monkeys to test the association between stereotypies and stress (Pomerantz et al. 2012). Because self-report measures are not possible in nonhuman primates, alternative methods are necessary when assessing behavior.

Stereotypies in Nonhuman Primates

Stereotypies have been observed in a variety of nonhuman primate species, including prosimians (Tarou et al. 2005), New World monkeys (Berkson et al. 1966), Old World monkeys (Goosen 1981; Lutz et al. 2003; Mitchell 1968; Vandeleest et al. 2011), and apes (Berkson and Mason 1964a; Davenport and Menzel 1963; Nash et al. 1999; Pazol and Bloomsmith, 1993). These stereotypies can take a variety of forms, including both whole-body and self-directed movements. Whole-body stereotypies may include behaviors such as pacing, bouncing, rocking, flipping, and swinging (Davenport and Menzel 1963; Lutz et al. 2003; Pazol and Bloomsmith 1993; Vandeleest et al. 2011), whereas self-directed or fine-motor stereotypies may include behaviors such as eye poking, digit sucking, hair pulling, and self-grasping (Davenport and Menzel 1963; Lutz et al. 2003). In some studies, stereotypies have been experimentally induced through procedures such as drug administration (Levin et al. 1990; Scraggs and Ridley 1978), lesioning (Bauman et al. 2008; Malkova et al. 1997), or restrictive rearing practices (Harlow and Harlow 1962a, 1962b). However, these behaviors are also reported to occur spontaneously in animals socially housed in zoos (Bollen and Novak 2000; Marriner and Drickamer 1994), as well as in mother-reared laboratory animals (Lutz et al. 2003). In one survey of 108 zoos, 14% of the primates were reported to exhibit some form of abnormal behavior, with apes exhibiting the highest percentage (40%), followed by Old World monkeys (14%), New World monkeys (6%), and prosimians (7%) (Bollen and Novak 2000). In a similar survey conducted at the St. Louis Zoological Park, eight species of zoo primates exhibited stereotyped behaviors such as pacing, rocking, twirling, saluting, coprophagy, urophagy, and regurgitation (Marriner and Drickamer 1994). Studies of abnormal behavior in laboratory primates have reported that as high as 89–100% of singly housed macaques exhibited some form of abnormal behavior (Bayne, Dexter, Suomi 1992; Camus et al. 2013; Lutz et al. 2003), and more than 25% were reported to exhibit motor stereotypy such as pacing (Lutz et al. 2003; Vandeleest et al. 2011). Abnormal stereotypic behavior can be considered pathological in nonhuman primates if it occupies a substantial portion of the animal's time budget, interferes with biological functions, or induces tissue damage (Bayne and Novak 1998).

Alternatively, displacement behaviors are relatively benign repetitive behaviors that occur normally in nonhuman primate populations and consist mostly of self-directed activities such as scratching, self-grooming, yawning, and body-shaking (Maestripieri et al. 1992). These behavior patterns often occur in situations where they would not be expected, and they are often associated with uncertainty and anxiety (Troisi 2002). For example, low-ranking Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) were more likely to scratch than their high-ranking counterparts (Troisi et al. 1991), and socially housed chimpanzees responded to neighbor vocalizations with an increase in scratching and yawning (Baker and Aureli 1997). Similarly, the rate of self-grooming in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) increased when female macaques were within one meter of the dominant male macaque. This was interpreted as a conflict situation; the female macaques were attracted to the male, but at the same time, they risked attack. Therefore, the self-grooming may have been used as a displacement activity (Troisi and Schino 1987).

Stereotypies in Human Primates

Many human stereotypies are similar to those exhibited by nonhuman primates in terms of motor output such as rocking (Beckett et al. 2002; Mitchell and Etches 1977), pacing (Walsh 1994), and digit sucking (Foster 1998; Ozturk and Ozturk 1977; Troster et al. 1991). However, there are also clear species differences in the form and expression of some stereotyped activities. Although both human and nonhuman primates can show idiosyncratic abnormal activities, these activities are typically more complex in people (Goosen 1981). Such complex stereotypies may include rocking while flicking the hand in front of the face or rubbing a finger against the thumb of the same hand (Hutt et al. 1965), hair twirling (Foster 1998; Rago and Case 1978), repetitive manipulation of objects (Troster et al. 1991), and complex hand movements (Young and Clements 1979).

Stereotypies in the General Human Population

Stereotypies can and do occur in the general human population. In some cases, the sequence of the development of stereotypies parallels the sequence in motor development (Wolff 1967). For example, infants exhibit rhythmical stereotypies such as rhythmical kicking, rocking, bouncing, and arm waving during their first year of development, and this tends to occur on a standard developmental trajectory (Thelen 1979). In a sample of more than 470 children, 19.1% were reported to rock and 6.3% were reported to head roll, often when alone in the crib or playpen (Sallustro and Atwell 1978), and 5–15% of infants were reported to exhibit head banging behavior (de Lissovoy 1961; Sallustro and Atwell 1978). Digit sucking is another example of a developmental stereotypy, which is reported to occur in normal children who were weaned from the breast early, schedule-fed, and/or left alone to fall asleep (Ozturk and Ozturk 1977). Although stereotypies such as head banging, digit sucking, and rocking tend to decline as the infant gets older (Werry et al. 1983), some behaviors such as digit sucking can continue into early childhood (Foster 1998).

As with nonhuman primates, humans also exhibit displacement behaviors that can occur in potentially stressful situations where they would otherwise not be expected. Such behaviors can consist of self-directed activities such as self-touching, scratching, and self-grooming (Troisi, 2002). For example, hair and face manipulation, as well as foot and leg rocking, were reported in college students participating in a psychology study and in the classroom (Rago and Case 1978; Woods and Miltenberger 1996), and stereotypic leg swinging was observed in school children during passive and active learning situations (Soussignan and Koch 1985). Motor stereotypies such as finger, foot, or forehead tapping were also reported in dental patients sitting in a waiting room (Barash 1974), a situation that may also be considered stressful. Because such stereotypical patterns of behavior in the normal population tend to occur on a normal developmental trajectory, do not interfere with normal interactions or behaviors, and do not cause the individual distress, they will not be the focus of this article.

Stereotypies in People with Neurodevelopmental or Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders

Although stereotypies do occur in the general population, these abnormal repetitive behaviors are more pronounced and disruptive in individuals with neurodevelopmental or psychiatric disorders such as autism, intellectual disabilities, and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Repetitive behaviors associated with these disorders can be organized into two classifications that may consist of separate and distinct symptoms: lower-order motor movements, such as stereotypies or repetitive manipulation of objects, and higher-order behaviors, such as rituals, preoccupations with aspects of the environment, or compulsions that result from irresistible impulses to perform a seemingly irrational act with the aim of preventing or reducing anxiety or distress (Lewis et al. 2007; Turner 1997; Turner 1999).

Autism

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder whose central features include impaired social interaction and communication as well as stereotyped patterns of behavior (American Psychiatric Association 2013). In one study of 32 adults with autism, all of the subjects exhibited at least one form of stereotypy (Bodfish et al. 2000). Motor stereotypies such as head banging, body rocking, and hand flicking are common stereotyped patterns of behavior in autistic individuals (Prior and Macmillan 1973). The stereotypic behavior is often different across autistic individuals, and each individual may have a preferred or characteristic pattern, such as rocking while flicking the hand in front of the face, rubbing a finger against the thumb of the same hand, rapidly rotating the hands, repetitively shrugging the shoulder, or bouncing on the feet (Hutt et al. 1965). Compulsive behaviors have also been reported in this population. In a study of 32 adults with autism, 94% displayed at least one compulsive behavior (Bodfish et al. 2000) such as checking; counting; hoarding; the need to touch, tap, or rub; and self-injurious behavior (McDougle et al. 1995).

Intellectual Disability

Diagnostic criteria for intellectual disability include below-average intellectual functioning along with deficits in the ability to be independent and handle common demands in life when compared with others of a similar age and background (i.e., adaptive functioning) (American Psychiatric Association 2013). Although stereotypic behaviors are not diagnostic criteria, they are commonly associated with this disorder; stereotypies tend to be negatively associated with IQ (Ando and Yoshimura 1978). In a residential institution for adults with learning disability, 57% of residents with a mild learning disability and 63% of residents with a severe disability exhibited some form of stereotypy (Walsh 1994). Similarly, in a study of 210 residents of a facility for individuals with intellectual disability, 60.9% were reported to exhibit stereotypies (Bodfish et al. 1995). Examples of stereotyped behaviors in this population include swaying, rocking, twirling, eye poking, shaking, nodding, head rolling, head banging, and pill rolling (Berkson and Davenport 1962; Mitchell and Etches 1977). The most common stereotypies exhibited by adults with learning disabilities include body rocking, hand motions, pacing, and rubbing/scratching (Walsh 1994). Compulsions have also been reported to occur in this population. In one study, 40% of intellectually disabled residents exhibited compulsive behaviors such as ordering (e.g., arranging objects), closing doors, and hand washing (Bodfish et al. 1995).

Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

OCD is a disorder in which the individual has obsessions or persistent thoughts, images, or urges that cause anxiety and compulsions or repetitive acts that aim to reduce the anxiety. The obsessions or compulsions cause marked distress (American Psychiatric Association 2013), and the individuals generally recognize their symptoms as senseless and often struggle to stop performing the compulsive rituals (Zohar and Insel 1987). Such compulsive acts in OCD may include cleaning/washing, checking, and repetitive rituals (Miguel et al. 1995).

Cause or Origin of Repetitive Behavior

Inadequate Rearing

An inadequate rearing environment has consistently been shown to adversely affect stereotypical behavior in nonhuman primates. This association has been studied extensively in macaques and chimpanzees. Early research by Harlow and Harlow (1962a, 1962b) demonstrated the intense, negative impact of isolation rearing on infant rhesus macaques. Macaques and chimpanzees reared in isolation exhibited high levels of stereotyped behaviors such as rocking, swaying, and digit sucking (Davenport and Menzel 1963; Mitchell 1968), often within the first month of life (Baysinger et al. 1972). Stereotypies in isolation-reared rhesus macaques were significantly greater than those exhibited by control animals that had been reared with their mother and peers (Lewis et al. 1990). Even infant rhesus macaques reared in partial isolation, where they could see and hear, but not touch, other monkeys, displayed stereotypic behaviors such as self-sucking, rocking, self-clutching, and pacing (Cross and Harlow 1965; Suomi et al. 1971). In contrast, macaques reared in the wild and later individually housed did not exhibit rocking behavior (Suomi et al. 1971). Stereotypies often continue in the behavioral repertoire as the animal grows older. For example, after leaving the nursery and being housed in a social group, nursery- and peer-reared rhesus macaques showed higher levels of stereotypy than did mother-reared animals (Feng et al. 2011). Stereotypic behaviors associated with nursery rearing in macaques include motor stereotypies (e.g., pacing, swinging, flipping, rocking, and bouncing) (Gottlieb, Capitanio, McCowan 2013), digit sucking (Lutz et al. 2003), and self-directed abnormal behavior (e.g., self-clasping, eye poking, self-sucking, repetitive cage licking, and feces smearing) (Bellanca and Crockett 2002). Similarly, hand-reared chimpanzees showed significantly more rocking and self-sucking behaviors than did mother-reared animals (Nash et al. 1999), and in two surveys of zoo primates, past rearing history (i.e., hand-rearing) had a greater impact on stereotypies than present environmental conditions (Bollen and Novak 2000; Marriner and Drickamer 1994).

Studies regarding restrictive rearing or housing in human infants and children are typically retrospective, based on situations where the individuals were placed in an orphanage or in a residential institution. As with nonhuman primates, human infants restrictively reared in impoverished facilities also developed stereotypies. For example, before adoption, up to 84% of children who spent time in a Romanian orphanage exhibited stereotyped behaviors such as rocking or repetitive hand movements (Beckett et al. 2002; Fisher et al. 1997; Hoksbergen et al. 2005). Those placed in foster care showed significantly lower levels of stereotypies, and the reduction was greatest in those with earlier or longer placement in foster care (Bos et al. 2010).

Environmental Restriction Later in Life

Low stimulus input later in life can also foster the development or expression of stereotypies. For individuals experiencing an inadequate environment lacking in stimulation, stereotypies may impart some benefit and function as a form of “do-it-yourself enrichment,” helping to improve well-being in a suboptimal environment (Mason and Latham 2004). In rhesus macaques, stereotypies have been associated with environmental restriction such as single housing (Lutz et al. 2003; Gottlieb, Capitanio, McCowan 2013); those housed singly exhibited more repetitive locomotion, stereotypy, and self-directed behavior than did monkeys housed in social groups (Bayne, Dexter, Suomi 1992). Similarly, chimpanzees removed from their social group and placed in individual cages also showed an increase in stereotyped behaviors (Brent et al. 1989). Single housing at an early age or for an extended period of time were shown to be risk factors for stereotyped behaviors such as pacing, flipping, eye poking, hair pulling, and self-grasping (Lutz et al. 2003; Vandeleest et al. 2011), and singly housed rhesus macaques expressed 1.34 times as much motor stereotypy as did pair-housed animals (Gottlieb, Capitanio, McCowan 2013). Simply being housed in an indoor environment is also associated with stereotypies (Gottlieb, Capitanio, McCowan 2013). For example, six chimpanzees showed higher levels of repetitive stereotypy (e.g., rocking or swaying) when placed in a small enclosed cage than when housed in their outdoor home cage. When manipulable objects were provided, the occurrence of stereotyped behaviors (e.g., abnormal limb postures, eye poking, and thumb sucking) was reduced. In this example, both general arousal and the ability to perform alternative activities had an impact on the performance of stereotyped behaviors (Berkson et al. 1963).

As with nonhuman primates, an environment lacking sensory input can also result in increased stereotyped behavior in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. For example, stereotypies were positively correlated with age and time in an institution (Berkson and Davenport 1962), and in a study of 60 adult residents of a state hospital who exhibited stereotypical behavior, 92% of the staff ranked self-stimulation as the most common consequence of their behavior (Wieseler et al. 1985). In two studies of individuals with intellectual disabilities, one with 14 adult men and one with 55 children, stereotypies increased when the subjects were placed in a restricted environment such as an empty room or a crib and decreased when they were placed in a less-restrictive environment with opportunities for locomotion and manipulation (Berkson and Mason 1963; Warren and Burns 1970). Similarly, the behavior of 12 intellectually disabled subjects was recorded in two situations: when they were located in a 6 × 6 foot chamber and when they were housed in a residential ward. The six subjects with a higher IQ (mean = 26) exhibited significantly more stereotyped rocking while in the chamber setting. However, in the six subjects with a lower IQ (untestable range), there was no effect of location on rocking. These results suggest that subjects with a higher IQ were more responsive to their environment (Forehand and Baumeister 1971a). The impact of environment on behavior was further demonstrated in a study of 46 children with intellectual disabilities. Those that lived in an institution (n = 23) were compared with children living at home but on a waiting list for institutionalization (n = 23) to reduce the variability between the two populations. Stereotyped and self-stimulating behaviors (e.g., head rolling, hand waving, and hand mouthing) were greater in the institutionalized population than the population living at home. In contrast, the home population exhibited greater social behavior (Kaufman 1967). Environmental restrictions also occur intrinsically in autistic and intellectually disabled children because they are less interactive with the environment and/or receive less environmental stimulation (Prior and Macmillan,1973; Walsh 1994). This intrinsic form of restriction may also have led to stereotypies in these populations.

Environmental Overstimulation

Stereotypies may also be used as a mechanism to maintain homeostasis in in an overstimulating environment. This theory is centered on the idea that there is an optimal level of stimulation. If there are deviations in either direction from the optimal level, the individual will compensate behaviorally to regulate the stimulation (Zentall and Zentall 1983). Therefore, stereotypic rhythmic patterns may be, in part, a homeostatic response to environmental stimuli (Guess and Carr 1991). Because stereotypies are highly predictable, such repetitive activities may help to reduce anxiety (Baron-Cohen 1989) or to reduce awareness of environmental stimuli. For example, autistic children who were taught to respond to a signal that produced a reward were slower to respond when they were exhibiting stereotypical behavior (Lovaas et al. 1971), and human infants were less responsive to tickling when they were repetitively sucking on a pacifier than when they had no pacifier in their mouth (Wolff and Simmons 1967).

Stereotypies may therefore serve as an arousal-reducing response to situations of frustration or aversive stimuli. For example, when chimpanzees were exposed to white noise, a buzzer, or a stimulus object that rocked and produced a grinding noise, they responded with an increase in repetitive stereotyped movement such as rocking or swaying (Berkson et al. 1963; Berkson and Mason 1964a). The rocking or swaying increased as the decibels of the white noise increased, with the highest rates occurring with the buzzer sound (Berkson and Mason 1964a). Common husbandry events such as feeding, sedations, and location changes may also be stressful and have an impact on stereotypies. For example, macaques exhibited motor stereotypies such as pacing, swinging, and bouncing before feeding. When the feeding time was made predictable, the subjects were less likely to perform stereotypies (Gottlieb, Coleman, McCowan 2013). When feeding time was delayed, the stereotyped behavioral patterns were prolonged (Waitt and Buchanan-Smith 2001). Similarly, the risk of displaying motor stereotypies in rhesus macaques significantly increased with the number of sedations and blood draws (Lutz et al. 2003; Vandeleest et al. 2011), and motor stereotypy in rhesus macaques increased by 3% for each relocation to a new room (Gottlieb, Capitanio, McCowan 2013). When events such as routine husbandry activities were predictable, levels of stereotypies were decreased (Gottlieb, Coleman, McCowan 2013).

This relationship between stereotypies and environmentally induced anxiety, frustration, or stimulation has also been demonstrated in people with neurodevelopmental and obsessive-compulsive disorders. For example, in a study of six autistic children, levels of stereotypy (e.g., rocking, head shaking) increased as the environment increased in complexity, from an empty room, to a box of wooden blocks placed in the room, to the addition of an adult sitting passively in the room. In this example, stereotypies may have helped reduce levels of arousal as the environment increased in complexity (Hutt and Hutt 1965; Hutt et al. 1965). In intellectually disabled individuals, frustration of goal-directed behavior (e.g., lever pressing) (Forehand and Baumeister 1971b) and noise (Forehand and Baumeister 1970; Levitt and Kaufman 1965) resulted in increased stereotyped behaviors, with the behavior increasing as the noise level increased (Levitt and Kaufman 1965). Anxiety is also commonly associated with OCD (Abramowitz and Arch 2014; O’Connor et al. 2006); almost 90% of subjects with OCD reported anxiety before their compulsive behavior or when the behavior was prevented (Miguel et al. 1995).

Treatment

Environmental Enrichment

Improving environmental conditions is one way in which stereotypies can be reduced in both human and nonhuman primates. For example, infant nursery-reared rhesus monkeys exhibited lower levels of stereotypy when they had stimulation from a mobile surrogate than when they were provided with a stationary surrogate (Mason and Berkson 1975). Providing even more stimulation in the form of a canine companion also resulted in a reduction of stereotyped rocking in a group of 17 chimpanzee infants (Pazol and Bloomsmith 1993). In older animals, additional environmental enhancements such as foraging opportunities can also promote an improvement in behavior. For example, the provisioning of straw, food puzzles, and forage materials reduced abnormal behavior in chimpanzees and rhesus macaques (Baker 1997; Bayne, Dexter, Mainzer, et al. 1992; Bloomsmith et al. 1988); as foraging increased, abnormal behaviors, including stereotypies, decreased (Bayne, Dexter, Mainzer, et al. 1992). Even simple chew toys placed in the cage resulted in reduced levels of abnormal behavior such as feces painting, hair pulling, and pacing (Brent and Belik 1997; Kessel and Brent 1998).

In addition to the contents of the cage, cage location and cage size can also have an impact on stereotyped behavior. For example, a greater proportion of life lived outdoors significantly decreased the risk of motor stereotypies in rhesus macaques (Vandeleest et al. 2011), and motor stereotypy decreased by 20% for every year that an animal spent outdoors (Gottlieb, Capitanio, McCowan 2013). Increased cage size also reduced stereotyped locomotion in rhesus macaques (Draper and Bernstein 1963; Paulk et al. 1977); however, this did not occur in all situations (Bayne and McCully 1989; Kaufman et al. 2004; Line et al. 1990), suggesting that a single environmental change may not always be sufficient.

Social housing may be considered to be the best form of environmental enrichment for nonhuman primates; social interactions routinely vary, and they are less likely to produce habituation than are inanimate forms of enrichment. For example, group-housed squirrel monkeys exhibited less stereotyped behavior than did their singly housed counterparts (Spring et al. 1997), and rhesus macaques group-housed in corn cribs exhibited significantly less repetitive locomotion, stereotypy, and self-directed behavior than did singly housed animals (Bayne, Dexter, Suomi 1992). Similarly, when singly housed baboons were moved to outdoor social groups, levels of abnormal behavior, including eye poking, flipping, and pacing, were reduced (Kessel and Brent 2001), and reductions in abnormal behavior were also observed when rhesus macaques were placed into compatible social pairs (Baker et al. 2012; Eaton et al. 1994).

As with nonhuman primates, environmental enrichment also reduced stereotyped behavior in both children and adults with autism and intellectual disabilities. For example, complex hand movements in intellectually disabled children decreased when the environment was enriched with play materials such as puzzles, dolls, and animals (Young and Clements 1979), and when institutionalized intellectually disabled adults were presented with pictures to look at or objects to manipulate, allowing for an alternate activity, they showed reduced levels of stereotypies (Berkson and Mason 1964b; Forehand and Baumeister 1970). Outdoor access also resulted in reduced levels of stereotypies in intellectually disabled adults; in the outdoor environment, levels of stereotypy decreased while locomotion increased (Berkson and Mason 1963). Similarly, when 13 autistic children were provided with multiple sensorimotor stimuli, including olfactory enrichment, music enrichment, and exposure to different textures and toys, they showed a significant reduction in autism severity scores in comparison with the control group that did not receive the enrichment, and there was also a significant increase in the number of parents reporting an improvement in autism symptoms (Woo and Leon 2013). In addition, when two autistic children were provided free access to toys, they showed a reduction in stereotyped scratching (Sidener et al. 2005). These results demonstrate that environmental changes or improvements can have a positive impact on resulting behavior across species.

Behavioral Treatment and Training

Behavioral treatment and training can also be effective in reducing stereotyped and repetitive behaviors. In the human population, functional assessment methods have been used to identify environmental variables that reinforce or maintain stereotypies in order to devise procedures to reduce the behavior (Bloomsmith et al. 2007). Sensory extinction is a behavioral method that involves eliminating the relationship between a behavior and its sensory outcome (Rapp and Vollmer 2005). Although this technique can be problematic because an increase in the unwanted behavior can initially occur (Bloomsmith et al. 2007), this procedure has been effective in a number of cases. For example, functional analyses suggested that repetitive hair pulling and/or hair manipulation in two subjects with neurodevelopmental disorders was maintained by automatic reinforcement and sensory stimulation. Providing gloves for them to wear reduced the sensory stimulation as well as the hair pulling and manipulation (Rapp et al. 1999; Rapp et al. 2000). Sensory extinction was also used to reduce stereotypic vocalizations produced by two autistic children. In comparison with baseline (earphones alone), earphones plus white noise to mask the sounds of stereotypic vocalizations resulted in a decrease in the vocalizations (Aiken and Salzberg 1984). An alternative training procedure, differential reinforcement of other behavior, is a strategy in which reinforcement is provided when the target behavior has not occurred during a specified period of time. This method was used both in the classroom setting and in individual sessions to reduce repetitive behavior exhibited by intellectually disabled children. In this paradigm, the children were rewarded with praise, candy, or tokens if they did not exhibit a given stereotyped behavior during a predetermined interval. If the behavior was exhibited, the timer was reset for that interval. Results showed a decrease in repetitive behavior, suggesting that training with differential reinforcement of other behavior is an effective means of reducing stereotyped behavior (Repp et al. 1976). Another method of reducing abnormal behavior is with noncontingent reinforcement, the delivery of a reinforcer on a schedule that is independent of the behavior. This procedure was used with three individuals who exhibited abnormal behavior, two diagnosed with intellectual disability and one diagnosed with autism. The reinforcers were provided on a fixed-time schedule and consisted of access to a bucket of toys and attention from the staff. In all three cases, the aberrant behavior was reduced to near-zero levels (Britton et al. 2000). Cognitive behavioral therapy is a form of training that is often used for individuals with OCD. With this training, a person is repeatedly exposed to the stimulus that makes him or her anxious or uncomfortable and then is trained to resist the urge to carry out the responding compulsion and to wait for the resulting anxiety to subside. In a study of children aged 7 to 17 years with OCD, the use of cognitive behavioral therapy in conjunction with medication resulted in a significant improvement in their obsessive-compulsive scale score when compared with medication alone (Franklin et al. 2011), and in a similar study of adult participants with OCD, those receiving cognitive behavioral therapy showed a clinically significant treatment effect in contrast with those receiving medication alone (O’Connor et al. 2006). However, even though this treatment method is effective, some patients may experience a relapse (Abramowitz and Arch 2014).

As with human subjects, training has also been effective in reducing stereotyped behaviors in nonhuman primates. Although a functional analysis approach may be effective in treating stereotyped behaviors in individual primates, it can be time-consuming and not cost-effective for larger numbers of animals. Instead, nonhuman primates are often trained through positive reinforcement to cooperate with husbandry procedures or to perform behaviors that are incompatible with the stereotyped behavior. Training involving behaviors such as holding a target, stationing, or presenting body parts has resulted in decreased levels of abnormal and stereotyped behavior in rhesus macaques, baboons, and chimpanzees (Baker et al. 2009; Bourgeois and Brent 2005; Coleman and Maier 2010; Pomerantz and Terkel 2009). In a study of 12 chimpanzees trained to hold targets and present body parts, this decrease was observed throughout the day, even outside of the training sessions (Pomerantz and Terkel 2009). In a larger study of 63 rhesus macaques, positive reinforcement training resulted in an overall reduction in stereotyped abnormal behavior, but the reduction occurred only in the animals that exhibited the highest levels of abnormal behavior (Baker et al. 2009). Therefore, positive reinforcement training focusing on husbandry-related or incompatible behaviors can help to reduce stereotyped behaviors in nonhuman primates, but the training may be more effective in some individuals than in others.

Drug Therapy

Altered brain functioning has been associated with the development and expression of stereotypies in both human and nonhuman primates. Pharmacological studies have linked dopaminergic, serotoninergic, and opioid systems to stereotypic behavior, leading to treatment options for nonhuman primates and for individuals with psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders.

Dopamine

Dopamine is a neurochemical that has been associated with stereotypical behavior in both human and nonhuman primates. Dopamine and dopamine agonists have been demonstrated to induce stereotypies, whereas blocking dopamine receptors with antagonists decreased stereotypies (Lanovaz 2011). In a study of 24 adult residents of a state developmental center who had severe or profound mental retardation, the 12 subjects who exhibited stereotyped behavior had significantly lower levels of homovanillic acid, a dopamine metabolite, than those who did not stereotype (Lewis et al. 1996). In addition, the administration of amphetamine or the dopamine agonist apomorphine resulted in increased levels of stereotyped behavior (Koizumi 1985; Kramer et al. 1967), and the administration of cocaine, a serotonin-norepinephrine-dopamine reuptake inhibitor caused repetitive involuntary movements (Daras et al. 1994). Cocaine addiction, as well as dopaminergic treatment, has also resulted in repetitive behavior known as “punding,” a stereotyped behavior characterized by complex, non-goal-oriented, repetitive activities (Fasano et al. 2008; Spencer et al. 2011). Dopamine antagonists have been used as a treatment to reduce stereotypical behavior in individuals with neurodevelopmental disabilities. For example, chlorpromazine reduced rocking in subjects with severe mental retardation (Aman et al. 1984), haloperidol was reported to reduce stereotypies in autistic children in two double-blind studies (Campbell et al. 1978; Cohen et al. 1980), and thioridazine clinically (but not statistically) reduced stereotypies in a group of 20 severely mentally retarded patients (Singh and Aman 1981).

As with humans, in nonhuman primates, the administration of amphetamine or the dopamine agonist apomorphine resulted in increased levels of stereotyped behavior. This has been reported to occur in monkeys (Levin et al. 1990; Lewis et al. 1990; Sams-Dodd and Newman 1997; Schlemmer et al. 1980; Scraggs and Ridley 1978) and chimpanzees (Berkson and Mason 1964a), and the behavioral response can persist, even when it seems unlikely that any drug remains in the system (Castner and Goldman-Rakic 1999). Similarly, cocaine caused repetitive stereotypies such as head swinging and whole body rocking and swinging in squirrel monkeys (Saka et al. 2004). Dopamine antagonists have also been utilized as a treatment to reduce stereotypical behavior in nonhuman primates. For example, chlorpromazine reduced abnormal behavior in four rhesus monkeys reared in partial isolation. When treated, three of the four monkeys showed a significant reduction in self-disturbance behaviors (e.g., self-mouth, self-bite, self-clasp, huddle, rock, and stereotypy) (McKinney et al. 1973). These results demonstrate an association between dopamine and stereotyped behavior in nonhuman primates and in humans with autism or intellectual disability.

Serotonin

Serotonin is a neurotransmitter that is also implicated in repetitive behaviors. For example, in child and adolescent subjects with autism, those with the long allele (L group) of the serotonin transporter gene promotor polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) showed increased severity in stereotyped and repetitive mannerisms when compared with those with the short allele (S group) (Brune CW et al. 2006). Serotonin has also been implicated in repetitive behaviors associated with OCD. When subjects were given metachlorophenylpoperazine, a serotonergic agonist, the OCD symptoms were exacerbated (Hollander et al. 1992; Zohar and Insel 1987; Zohar et al. 1987). In addition, in a double-blind study of 24 hospitalized patients with OCD, there was a significant correlation between 5-HIAA (a serotonin metabolite) level in cerebrospinal fluid and improvement of obsessional symptoms scores (Thoren et al. 1980). Serotonin has been further implicated in in studies using drug therapy to reduce stereotypies. For example, fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, was tested on a group of 39 children with autism spectrum disorder in a double-blind placebo crossover study. Fluoxetine administration resulted in a significant reduction in repetitive behaviors and a clinically meaningful, although not statistically significant, effect on global autism symptoms (Hollander et al. 2005). Fluvoxamine, a selective serotonin uptake inhibitor, was also tested on thirty adults with autistic disorder in a 12-week double-blind placebo-controlled study; by week 8, a significant reduction in repetitive behaviors was reported (McDougle et al. 1996). Similarly, clomipramine, a 5-HT uptake inhibitor, reduced body and object stereotypy in adults with intellectual disabilities (Lewis et al. 1995) and decreased obsessive-compulsive ratings in children and adolescents with OCD (Leonard et al. 1989). In these studies, however, the ratings used to determine improvement in OCD symptoms largely measure distress and the ability to control the symptoms (Hollander et al. 1992) or obsessions with thoughts or ideas (Leonard et al. 1989; Zohar et al. 1987), conditions that cannot easily be modeled in an animal population.

In nonhuman primates, fluoxetine was tested on a group of captive vervet monkeys exhibiting stereotypic behavior such as head-tossing, weaving, somersaulting, and saluting. By week 6, the group receiving fluoxetine exhibited significantly fewer stereotypies than did the “no treatment” group (Hugo et al. 2003). However, this effect has not been reported in all studies (Fontenot et al. 2009).

Opiates

One theory of the opiate hypothesis is that repetitive abnormal behavior is associated with a release of β-endorphin that reinforces the behavior (Sandman et al. 1990). In adult human patients with intellectual disabilities, the plasma concentration of β-endorphin was significantly higher in the groups with self-injurious and stereotypic behaviors when compared with the control group that exhibited neither types of behavior (Sandman et al. 1990). As a treatment, naltrexone, a long-acting opioid antagonist, was demonstrated to improve behavior and reduce stereotypies in subjects with autism or intellectual disabilities. In two open-trial studies of naltrexone on children with autism, the autistic score, which includes stereotypical behavior, significantly improved (Campbell et al. 1988; Panksepp and Lensing 1991). Similarly, in a placebo-controlled study of six children with autism, the administration of naltrexone resulted in an improvement of the social interaction and stereotyped behavior score (Williams et al. 2001). Naltrexone was also used to treat two subjects diagnosed with profound mental retardation and autistic disorder in an ABAB-designed study; in this case, treatment significantly reduced stereotyped body rocking (Smith et al. 1995). These results support the theory that stereotyped behavior stimulates the release of endorphins, which may function as positive reinforcement (Smith et al. 1995). However, not all studies support the opiate hypothesis regarding stereotyped behavior (Bodfish et al. 1997; Kolmen et al. 1995; Willemsen-Swinkels et al. 1995a; Willemsen-Swinkels et al. 1995b), suggesting that there may be individual differences in responsiveness to naltrexone.

In nonhuman primates, a positive relationship between intact plasma beta endorphin and severity of abnormal behavior has been reported in pig-tailed and long-tailed macaques (Crockett et al. 2007). However, opioid antagonists such as naloxone have not yet been examined as treatments for stereotypies.

Conclusions

Nonhuman primates exhibiting stereotyped behaviors may serve as excellent models for humans with neurodevelopmental or obsessive-compulsive disorders. Although stereotypic behavior includes a diverse range of repetitive behaviors, there are important similarities between stereotypies displayed by nonhuman primates and by their human counterparts. For example, the form of the behavior is similar; both human and nonhuman primates exhibit repetitive behaviors such as motor stereotypies and self-directed behaviors. Additional similarities are noted with respect to the cause or origin of the behavior (e.g., environmental restriction or overstimulation) and the types of interventions, whether enrichment, training, or drug therapy (see Table 1 for a summary). However, because of a greater number of commonalities in these factors, nonhuman primates may serve as a better model for the stereotyped behaviors in autism and intellectual disability than for the compulsions in OCD. Because no animal model is exact in all aspects of the condition being studied, it is important to further investigate the use of nonhuman primates in the study of neuropsychiatric and developmental disorders in the human population.

Table 1

A comparison of the nonhuman primate model and the human disorders

Face validity: similarity in behavioral output
Construct validity:
similarity in cause or origin of stereotypies
Predictive validity:
similar treatment responses
DisorderStereotypyCompulsionInadequate rearingEnvironmental restrictionEnvironmental overstimulation/ stress reductionEnvironmental enrichmentTrainingDopamineSerotoninOpiates
NHP stereotypies*********
Autism*********
Intellectual disability*********
OCD****
Face validity: similarity in behavioral output
Construct validity:
similarity in cause or origin of stereotypies
Predictive validity:
similar treatment responses
DisorderStereotypyCompulsionInadequate rearingEnvironmental restrictionEnvironmental overstimulation/ stress reductionEnvironmental enrichmentTrainingDopamineSerotoninOpiates
NHP stereotypies*********
Autism*********
Intellectual disability*********
OCD****

An asterisk indicates an association between the disorder and the listed category. NHP, nonhuman primate; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Table 1

A comparison of the nonhuman primate model and the human disorders

Face validity: similarity in behavioral output
Construct validity:
similarity in cause or origin of stereotypies
Predictive validity:
similar treatment responses
DisorderStereotypyCompulsionInadequate rearingEnvironmental restrictionEnvironmental overstimulation/ stress reductionEnvironmental enrichmentTrainingDopamineSerotoninOpiates
NHP stereotypies*********
Autism*********
Intellectual disability*********
OCD****
Face validity: similarity in behavioral output
Construct validity:
similarity in cause or origin of stereotypies
Predictive validity:
similar treatment responses
DisorderStereotypyCompulsionInadequate rearingEnvironmental restrictionEnvironmental overstimulation/ stress reductionEnvironmental enrichmentTrainingDopamineSerotoninOpiates
NHP stereotypies*********
Autism*********
Intellectual disability*********
OCD****

An asterisk indicates an association between the disorder and the listed category. NHP, nonhuman primate; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Grant R24OD01180–15 to Melinda Novak at Harvard Medical School and P51OD011133 to Texas Biomedical Research Institute (SNPRC).

References

Abramowitz
JS
Arch
JJ
,
Strategies for improving long-term outcomes in cognitive behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: Insights from learning theory
Cogn Behav Pract
,
2014
, vol.
21
(pg.
20
-
31
)
Aiken
JM
Salzberg
CL
,
The effects of a sensory extinction procedure on stereotypic sounds of two autistic children
J Autism Dev Disord
,
1984
, vol.
14
(pg.
291
-
299
)
Albelda
N
Joel
D
,
Current animal models of obsessive compulsive disorder: An update
Neuroscience
,
2012
, vol.
211
(pg.
83
-
106
)
Aman
MG
White
AJ
Field
C
,
Chlorpromazine effects on stereotypic and conditioned behaviour of severely retarded patients—A pilot study
J Ment Defic Res
,
1984
, vol.
28
(pg.
253
-
260
)
American Psychiatric Association
,
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
,
2013
Arlington, VA
American Psychiatric Association
 
Fifth Edition
Ando
H
Yoshimura
I
,
Prevalence of maladaptive behavior in retarded children as a function of IQ and age
J Abnorm Child Psychol
,
1978
, vol.
6
(pg.
345
-
349
)
Baker
KC
,
Straw and forage material ameliorate abnormal behaviors in adult chimpanzees
Zoo Biol
,
1997
, vol.
16
(pg.
225
-
236
)
Baker
KC
Aureli
F
,
Behavioural indicators of anxiety: an empirical test in chimpanzees
Behaviour
,
1997
, vol.
134
(pg.
1031
-
1050
)
Baker
KC
Bloomsmith
M
Neu
K
Griffis
C
Maloney
M
Oettinger
B
Schoof
VAM
Martinez
M
,
Positive reinforcement training moderates only high levels of abnormal behavior in singly housed rhesus macaques
J Appl Anim Welf Sci
,
2009
, vol.
12
(pg.
236
-
252
)
Baker
KC
Bloomsmith
MA
Oettinger
B
Neu
K
Griffis
C
Schoof
V
Maloney
M
,
Benefits of pair housing are consistent across a diverse population of rhesus macaques
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2012
, vol.
137
(pg.
148
-
156
)
Baker
KC
Weed
JL
Crockett
CM
Bloomsmith
MA
,
Survey of environmental enhancement programs for laboratory primates
Am J Primatol
,
2007
, vol.
69
(pg.
377
-
394
)
Barash
DP
,
Human ethology: Displacement activities in a dental office
Psychol Rep
,
1974
, vol.
34
(pg.
947
-
949
)
Baron-Cohen
S
,
Do autistic children have obsessions and compulsions?
Br J Clin Psychol
,
1989
, vol.
28
(pg.
193
-
200
)
Bauman
MD
Toscano
JE
Babineau
BA
Mason
WA
Amaral
DG
,
Emergence of stereotypies in juvenile monkeys with neonatal amygdala or hippocampus lesions
Behav Neurosci
,
2008
, vol.
122
(pg.
1005
-
1015
)
Bayne
K
Dexter
S
Mainzer
H
McCully
C
Campbell
G
Yamada
F
,
The use of artificial turf as a foraging substrate for individually housed rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)
Anim Welf
,
1992
, vol.
1
(pg.
39
-
53
)
Bayne
K
Dexter
S
Suomi
S
,
A preliminary survey of the incidence of abnormal behavior in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) relative to housing condition
Lab Anim
,
1992
, vol.
21
(pg.
38
-
46
)
Bayne
KAL
McCully
C
,
The effect of cage size on the behavior of individually housed rhesus monkeys
Lab Anim
,
1989
, vol.
18
(pg.
25
-
28
)
Bayne
K
Novak
M
Bennett
BT
Abee
CR
Henrickson
R
,
Behavioral disorders
Nonhuman Primates in Biomedical Research Diseases
,
1998
New York
Academic Press
(pg.
485
-
500
)
Baysinger
CM
Brandt
EM
Mitchell
G
,
Development of infant social isolate monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in their isolation environments
Primates
,
1972
, vol.
13
(pg.
257
-
270
)
Beckett
C
Bredenkamp
D
Castle
J
Groothues
C
O'Connor
TG
Rutter
M
the English and Romanian Adoptees (E.R.A.) Study Team
,
Behavior patterns associated with institutional depreivation: A study of children adopted from Romania
J Dev Behav Pediatr
,
2002
, vol.
23
(pg.
297
-
303
)
Bellanca
RU
Crockett
CM
,
Factors predicting increased incidence of abnormal behavior in male pigtailed macaques
Am J Primatol
,
2002
, vol.
58
(pg.
57
-
69
)
Berkson
G
,
Repetitive stereotyped behaviors
Am J Ment Defic
,
1983
, vol.
88
(pg.
239
-
246
)
Berkson
G
Davenport
RK
Jr
,
Stereotyped movements of mental defectives I. initial survey
. Am J Ment Defic
,
1962
, vol.
66
(pg.
849
-
852
)
Berkson
G
Goodrich
J
Kraft
I
,
Abnormal stereotyped movements of marmosets
Percept Mot Skills
,
1966
, vol.
23
(pg.
491
-
498
)
Berkson
G
Mason
WA
,
Stereotyped movements of mental defectives III
Situation effects. Am J Ment Defic
,
1963
, vol.
68
(pg.
409
-
412
)
Berkson
G
Mason
WA
,
Stereotyped behaviors of chimpanzees: relation to general arousal and alternative activities
Percept Mot Skills
,
1964a
, vol.
19
(pg.
635
-
652
)
Berkson
G
Mason
WA
,
Stereotyped movements of mental defectives
IV. The effects of toys and the character of the acts. Am J Ment Defic
,
1964b
, vol.
68
(pg.
511
-
524
)
Berkson
G
Mason
WA
Saxon
SV
,
Situation and stimulus effects on stereotyped behaviors of chimpanzees
J Comp Physiol Psychol
,
1963
, vol.
56
(pg.
786
-
792
)
Bloomsmith
MA
Alford
PL
Maple
TL
,
Successful feeding enrichment for captive chimpanzees
Am J Primatol
,
1988
, vol.
16
(pg.
155
-
164
)
Bloomsmith
MA
Marr
MJ
Maple
TL
,
Addressing nonhuman primate behavioral problems through the application of operant conditioning: Is the human treatment approach a useful model?
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2007
, vol.
102
(pg.
205
-
222
)
Bodfish
JW
Crawford
TW
Powell
SB
Parker
DE
Golden
RN
Lewis
MH
,
Compulsions in adults with mental retardation: Prevalence, phenomenology, and comorbidity with stereotypy and self-injury
Am J Ment Retard
,
1995
, vol.
100
(pg.
183
-
192
)
Bodfish
JW
McCuller
WR
Madison
JM
Register
M
Mailman
RB
Lewis
MH
,
Placebo, double-blind evaluation of long-term naltrexone treatment effects for adults with mental retardation and self-injury
J Dev Phys Disabil
,
1997
, vol.
9
(pg.
135
-
152
)
Bodfish
JW
Symons
FJ
Parker
DE
Lewis
MH
,
Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: Comparisons to mental retardation
J Autism Dev Disord
,
2000
, vol.
30
(pg.
237
-
243
)
Bollen
KS
Novak
MA
,
A survey of abnormal behavior in captive zoo primates
Am J Primatol
,
2000
, vol.
51
pg.
47
Bos
KJ
Zeanah
CH
Jr.
Smyke
AT
Fox
NA
Nelson
CA
III
,
Stereotypies in children with a history of early institutional care
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
,
2010
, vol.
164
(pg.
406
-
411
)
Bourgeois
SR
Brent
L
,
Modifying the behaviour of singly caged baboons: Evaluating the effectiveness of four enrichment techniques
Anim Welf
,
2005
, vol.
14
(pg.
71
-
81
)
Brent
L
Belik
M
,
The response of group-housed baboons to three enrichment toys
Lab Anim
,
1997
, vol.
31
(pg.
81
-
85
)
Brent
L
Lee
DR
Eichberg
JW
,
The effects of single caging on chimpanzee behavior
Lab Anim Sci
,
1989
, vol.
39
(pg.
345
-
346
)
Britton
LN
Carr
JE
Kellum
KK
Dozier
CL
Weil
TM
,
A variation of noncontingent reinforcement in the treatment of aberrant behavior
Res Dev Disabil
,
2000
, vol.
21
(pg.
425
-
435
)
Brune
CW
Kim
SJ
Salt
J
Leventhal
BL
Lord
C
Cook
EH
Jr
,
5-HTTLPR genotype-specific phenotype in children and adolescents with autism
Am J Psychiatry
,
2006
, vol.
163
(pg.
2148
-
2156
)
Brune
M
Brune-Cohrs
U
McGrew
WC
Preuschoft
S
,
Psychopathology in great apes: Concepts, treatment options and possible homologies to human psychiatric disorders
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
,
2006
, vol.
30
(pg.
1246
-
1259
)
Campbell
M
Adams
P
Small
AM
Tesch
LM
Curren
EL
,
Naltrexone in infantile autism
Psychopharmacol Bull
,
1988
, vol.
24
(pg.
135
-
139
)
Campbell
M
Anderson
LT
Meier
M
Cohen
IL
Small
AM
Samit
C
Sachar
EJ
,
A comparison of haloperidol and behavior therapy and their interaction in autistic children
J Am Acad Child Psychiatry
,
1978
, vol.
17
(pg.
640
-
655
)
Camus
SMJ
Blois-Heulin
C
Li
Q
Hausberger
M
Bezard
E
,
Behavioural profiles in captive-bred cynomolgus macaques: towards monkey models of mental disorders?
PLoS One
,
2013
, vol.
8
(pg.
1
-
14
)
Capitanio
JP
Mitchell
G
Erwin
J
,
Behavioral pathology
Comparative Primate Biology, Volume 2A: Behavior, Conservation, and Ecology
,
1986
New York
Alan R. Liss
(pg.
411
-
454
)
Castner
SA
Goldman-Rakic
PS
,
Long-lasting psychotomimetic consequences of repeated low-dose amphetamine exposure in rhesus monkeys
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
1999
, vol.
20
(pg.
10
-
28
)
Cohen
IL
Campbell
M
Posner
D
Small
AM
Triebel
D
Anderson
LT
,
Behavioral effects of haloperidol in young autistic children
J Am Acad Child Psychiatry
,
1980
, vol.
19
(pg.
665
-
677
)
Coleman
K
Maier
A
,
The use of positive reinforcement training to reduce stereotypic behavior in rhesus macaques
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2010
, vol.
124
(pg.
142
-
148
)
Cooper
JJ
Nicol
CJ
,
Stereotypic behaviour affects environmental preference in bank voles, Clethrionomys glareolus
Anim Behav
,
1991
, vol.
41
(pg.
971
-
977
)
Crockett
CM
Sackett
GP
Sandman
CA
Chicz-DeMet
A
Bentson
KL
,
Beta-endorphin levels in longtailed and pigtailed macaques vary by abnormal behavior rating and sex
Peptides
,
2007
, vol.
28
(pg.
1987
-
1997
)
Cross
HA
Harlow
HF
,
Prolonged and progressive effects of partial isolation on the behavior of macaque monkeys
J Exp Res Pers
,
1965
, vol.
1
(pg.
39
-
49
)
Dallaire
A
,
Stress and behavior in domestic animals
Ann N Y Acad Sci
,
1993
, vol.
697
(pg.
269
-
274
)
Dantzer
R
,
Behavioral, physiological and functional aspects of stereotyped behavior: A review and a re-interpretation
J Anim Sci
,
1986
, vol.
62
(pg.
1776
-
1786
)
Daras
M
Koppel
BS
Atos-Radzion
E
,
Cocaine-induced choreoathetoid movements (“crack dancing”)
Neurology
,
1994
, vol.
44
(pg.
751
-
752
)
Davenport
RK
Menzel
EW
,
Stereotyped behavior of the infant chimpanzee
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1963
, vol.
8
(pg.
99
-
104
)
de Lissovoy
V
,
Head banging in early childhood
J Pediatr
,
1961
, vol.
58
(pg.
803
-
805
)
Diener
E
Oishi
S
Lucas
RE
,
Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life
Annu Rev Psychol
,
2003
, vol.
54
(pg.
403
-
425
)
Draper
WA
Bernstein
IS
,
Stereotyped behavior and cage size
Percept Mot Skills
,
1963
, vol.
16
(pg.
231
-
234
)
Eaton
GG
Kelley
ST
Axthelm
MK
Iliff-Sizemore
SA
Shiigi
SM
,
Psychological well-being in paired adult female rhesus (Macaca mulatta)
Am J Primatol
,
1994
, vol.
33
(pg.
89
-
99
)
Erwin
J
Deni
R
Erwin
J
Maple
TL
Mitchell
G
,
Strangers in a strange land: Abnormal behaviors or abnormal environments?
Captivity and Behavior: Primates in Breeding Colonies, Laboratories, and Zoos
,
1979
New York
Van Nostrand Reinhold
(pg.
1
-
28
)
Fasano
A
Barra
A
Nicosia
P
Rinaldi
F
Bria
P
Bentivoglio
AR
Tonioni
F
,
Cocaine addiction: From habits to stereotypical-repetitive behaviors and punding
,
2008
, vol.
96
(pg.
178
-
182
)
Feng
X
Wang
L
Yang
S
Qin
D
Wang
J
Li
C
Lv
L
Ma
Y
Hu
X
,
Maternal separation produces lasting changes in cortisol and behavior in rhesus monkeys
PNAS
,
2011
, vol.
108
(pg.
14312
-
14317
)
Fisher
L
Ames
EW
Chisholm
K
Savoie
L
,
Problems reported by parents of Romanian orphans adopted to British Columbia
Int J Behav Dev
,
1997
, vol.
20
(pg.
67
-
82
)
Fontenot
MB
Musso
MW
McFatter
RM
Anderson
GM
,
Dose-finding study of fluoxetine and venlafaxine for the treatment of self-injurious and stereotypic behavior in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)
JAALAS
,
2009
, vol.
48
(pg.
176
-
184
)
Forehand
R
Baumeister
AA
,
The effect of auditory and visual stimulation on stereotyped rocking behavior and general activity of severe retardates
J Clin Psychol
,
1970
, vol.
26
(pg.
426
-
429
)
Forehand
R
Baumeister
AA
,
Stereotyped body rocking as a function of situation, IQ and time
J Clin Psychol
,
1971a
, vol.
27
(pg.
324
-
326
)
Forehand
R
Baumeister
AA
,
Rate of stereotyped body rocking of severe retardates as a function of frustration of goal-directed behavior
J Abnorm Psychol
,
1971b
, vol.
78
(pg.
35
-
42
)
Foster
LG
,
Nervous habits and stereotyped behaviors in preschool children
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
,
1998
, vol.
37
(pg.
711
-
717
)
Franklin
ME
Sapyta
J
Freeman
JB
Khanna
M
Compton
S
et al.
,
Cognitive behavior therapy augmentation of pharmacotherapy in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder
JAMA
,
2011
, vol.
306
(pg.
1224
-
1232
)
Goosen
C
,
Abnormal behavior patterns in rhesus monkeys: Symptoms of mental disease?
Biol Psychiatry
,
1981
, vol.
16
(pg.
697
-
716
)
Gottlieb
DH
Capitanio
JP
McCowan
B
,
Risk factors for stereotypic behavior and self-biting in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): Animal's history, current environment, and personality
Am J Primatol
,
2013
, vol.
75
(pg.
995
-
1008
)
Gottlieb
DH
Coleman
K
McCowan
B
,
The effects of predictability in daily husbandry routines on captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2013
, vol.
143
(pg.
117
-
127
)
Guess
D
Carr
E
,
Emergence and maintenance of stereotypy and self-injury
Am J Ment Retard
,
1991
, vol.
96
(pg.
299
-
319
)
Harlow
HF
Harlow
MK
,
The effect of rearing conditions on behavior
Bull Menninger Clin
,
1962a
, vol.
26
(pg.
213
-
224
)
Harlow
HF
Harlow
MK
,
Social deprivation in monkeys
Sci Am
,
1962b
, vol.
207
(pg.
136
-
146
)
Hoksbergen
R
ter Laak
J
Rijk
K
van Dijkum
C
Stoutjesdijk
F
,
Post-institutional autistic syndrome in Romanian adoptees
J Autism Dev Disord
,
2005
, vol.
35
(pg.
615
-
623
)
Hollander
E
DeCaria
CM
Nitescu
A
Gully
R
Suckow
RF
Cooper
TB
Gorman
JM
Klein
DF
Liebowitz
MR
,
Serotonergic function in obsessive-compulsive disorder
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1992
, vol.
49
(pg.
21
-
28
)
Hollander
E
Phillips
A
Chaplin
W
Zagursky
K
Novotny
S
Wasserman
S
Iyengar
R
,
A placebo controlled crossover trial of liquid fluoxetine on repetitive behaviors in childhood and adolescent autism
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2005
, vol.
30
(pg.
582
-
589
)
Hugo
C
Seier
J
Mdhluli
C
Daniels
W
Harvey
BH
Du Toit
D
Wolfe-Coote
S
Nel
D
Stein
DJ
,
Fluoxetine decreases stereotypic behavior in primates
Prog Neuropsychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry
,
2003
, vol.
27
(pg.
639
-
643
)
Hutt
C
Hutt
SJ
,
Effects of environmental complexity on stereotyped behaviours of children
Anim Behav
,
1965
, vol.
13
(pg.
1
-
4
)
Hutt
SJ
Hutt
C
Lee
D
Ounsted
C
,
A behavioural and electroencephalographic study of autistic children
J Psychiatr Res
,
1965
, vol.
3
(pg.
181
-
197
)
Kaufman
BM
Pouliot
AL
Tiefenbacher
S
Novak
MA
,
Short and long-term effects of a substantial change in cage size on individually housed, adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2004
, vol.
88
(pg.
319
-
330
)
Kaufman
ME
,
The effects of institutionalization on development of stereotyped and social behaviors in mental defectives
Am J Ment Defic
,
1967
, vol.
71
(pg.
581
-
585
)
Kessel
AL
Brent
L
,
Cage toys reduce abnormal behavior in individually housed pigtail macaques
J Appl Anim Welf Sci
,
1998
, vol.
1
(pg.
227
-
234
)
Kessel
A
Brent
L
,
The rehabilitation of captive baboons
J Med Primatol
,
2001
, vol.
30
(pg.
71
-
80
)
Koizumi
HM
,
Obsessive-compulsive symptoms following stimulants
Biol Psychiatry
,
1985
, vol.
20
(pg.
1332
-
1337
)
Kolmen
BK
Feldman
HM
Handen
BL
Janosky
JE
,
Naltrexone in young autistic children: A double blind, placebo-controlled crossover study
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
,
1995
, vol.
34
(pg.
223
-
231
)
Kramer
JC
Fischman
VS
Littlefield
DC
,
Amphetamine abuse
JAMA
,
1967
, vol.
201
(pg.
305
-
309
)
Lanovaz
MJ
,
Towards a comprehensive model of stereotypy: Integrating operant and neurobiological interpretations
Res Dev Disabil
,
2011
, vol.
32
(pg.
447
-
455
)
Leonard
HL
Swedo
SE
Rapoport
JL
Koby
EV
Lenane
MC
Cheslow
DL
Hamburger
SD
,
Treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder with clomipramine and desipramine in children and adolescents
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1989
, vol.
46
(pg.
1088
-
1092
)
Levin
ED
Bushnell
PJ
Baysinger
CM
,
d-Amphetamine-induced “floating limb” syndrome in young rhesus monkeys
Psychopharmacology
,
1990
, vol.
101
(pg.
112
-
117
)
Levitt
H
Kaufman
ME
,
Sound induced drive and stereotyped behavior in mental defectives
Am J Ment Defic
,
1965
, vol.
69
(pg.
729
-
734
)
Lewis
MH
Bodfish
JW
Powell
SB
Golden
RN
,
Clomipramine treatment for stereotypy and related repetitive movement disorders associated with mental retardation
Am J Ment Retard
,
1995
, vol.
100
(pg.
299
-
312
)
Lewis
MH
Bodfish
JW
Powell
SB
Wiest
K
Darling
M
Golden
RN
,
Plasma HVA in adults with mental retardation and stereotyped behavior: Biochemical evidence for a dopamine deficiency model
Am J Ment Retard
,
1996
, vol.
100
(pg.
413
-
427
)
Lewis
MH
Gluck
JP
Beauchamp
AJ
Keresztury
MF
Mailman
RB
,
Long-term effects of early social isolation in Macaca mulatta: Changes in dopamine receptor function following apomorphine challenge
Brain Res
,
1990
, vol.
513
(pg.
67
-
73
)
Lewis
MH
Tanimura
Y
Lee
LW
Bodfish
JW
,
Animal models of restricted repetitive behavior in autism
Behav Brain Res
,
2007
, vol.
176
(pg.
66
-
74
)
Line
SW
Morgan
KN
Markowitz
H
Strong
S
,
Increased cage size does not alter heart rate or behavior in female rhesus monkeys
Am J Primatol
,
1990
, vol.
20
(pg.
107
-
113
)
Lovaas
OI
Litrownik
A
Mann
R
,
Response latencies to auditory stimuli in autistic children engaged in self-stimulatory behavior
Behav Res and Therapy
,
1971
, vol.
9
(pg.
39
-
49
)
Lutz
C
Well
A
Novak
M
,
Stereotypic and self-injurious behavior in rhesus macaques: A survey and retrospective analysis of environment and early experience
Am J Primatol
,
2003
, vol.
60
(pg.
1
-
15
)
Maestripieri
D
Schino
G
Aureli
F
Troisi
A
,
A modest proposal: Displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates
Anim Behav
,
1992
, vol.
44
(pg.
967
-
979
)
Malkova
L
Mishkin
M
Suomi
SJ
Bachevalier
J
,
Socioemotional behavior in adult rhesus monkeys after early versus late lesions of the medial temporal lobe
Ann N Y Acad Sci
,
1997
, vol.
807
(pg.
538
-
540
)
Marinus
LM
Chase
WK
Rasmussen
KL
Jorgensen
MJ
Novak
MA
,
Reaction of rhesus monkeys with self-injurious behavior to heart rate testing: Is biting a coping strategy?
Am J Primatol
,
1999
, vol.
49
pg.
79
Marriner
LM
Drickamer
LC
,
Factors influencing stereotyped behavior of primates in a zoo
Zoo Biol
,
1994
, vol.
13
(pg.
267
-
275
)
Mason
GJ
,
Stereotypies and suffering
Behav Processes
,
1991
, vol.
25
(pg.
103
-
115
)
Mason
GJ
,
Age and context affect the stereotypies of caged mink
Behaviour
,
1993a
, vol.
127
(pg.
191
-
229
)
Mason
GJ
Lawrence
AB
Rushen
J
,
Forms of stereotypic behaviour
Stereotypic Animal Behavior: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare
,
1993b
Wallingford UK
CAB International
(pg.
7
-
40
)
Mason
GJ
Latham
NR
,
Can't stop, won't stop: Is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator?
Anim Welf
,
2004
, vol.
13
(pg.
S57
-
S69
)
Mason
WA
Berkson
G
,
Effects of maternal mobility on the development of rocking and other behaviors in rhesus monkeys: A study with artificial mothers
Dev Psychobiol
,
1975
, vol.
8
(pg.
197
-
211
)
McDougle
CJ
Kresch
LE
Goodman
WK
Naylor
ST
Volkmar
FR
Cohen
DJ
Price
LH
,
A case-controlled study of repetitive thoughts and behavior in adults with autistic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder
Am J Psychiatry
,
1995
, vol.
152
(pg.
772
-
777
)
McDougle
CJ
Naylor
ST
Cohen
DJ
Volkmar
FR
Heninger
GR
Price
LH
,
A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of fluvoxamine in adults with autistic disorder
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1996
, vol.
53
(pg.
1001
-
1008
)
McKinney
WT
Young
LD
Suomi
SJ
Davis
JM
,
Chlorpromazine treatment of disturbed monkeys
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1973
, vol.
29
(pg.
490
-
494
)
Mendl
M
Burman
OHP
Parker
RMA
Paul
ES
,
Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2009
, vol.
118
(pg.
161
-
181
)
Miguel
EC
Coffey
BJ
Baer
L
Savage
CR
Rauch
SL
Jenike
MA
,
Phenomenology of intentional repetitive behaviors in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette's disorder
J Clin Psychiatry
,
1995
, vol.
56
(pg.
246
-
255
)
Mitchell
GD
,
Persistent behavior pathology in rhesus monkeys following early social isolation
Folia Primatol
,
1968
, vol.
8
(pg.
132
-
147
)
Mitchell
R
Etches
P
,
Rhythmic habit patterns (stereotypies)
Develop Med Child Neurol
,
1977
, vol.
19
(pg.
545
-
550
)
Nash
LT
Fritz
J
Alford
PA
Brent
L
,
Variables influencing the origins of diverse abnormal behaviors in a large sample of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
Am J Primatol
,
1999
, vol.
48
(pg.
15
-
29
)
Novak
MA
Kelly
BJ
Bayne
K
Meyer
JS
Abee
C
Mansfield
K
Tardif
S
Morris
T
,
Behavioral disorders of nonhuman primates
Nonhuman Primates in Biomedical Research Biology and Management
,
2012
, vol.
vol. 1
Waltham MA
Elsevier
(pg.
177
-
196
)
Novak
MA
Kinsey
JH
Jorgensen
MJ
Hazen
TJ
,
Effects of puzzle feeders on pathological behavior in individually housed rhesus monkeys
Am J Primatol
,
1998
, vol.
46
(pg.
213
-
227
)
O'Connor
KP
Aardema
F
Robillard
S
Guay
S
Pelissier
M-C
Todorov
C
Borgeat
F
Leblanc
V
Grenier
S
Doucet
P
,
Cognitive behaviour therapy and medication in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder
Acta Psychiatr Scand
,
2006
, vol.
113
(pg.
408
-
419
)
Ozturk
M
Ozturk
OM
,
Thumbsucking and falling asleep
Br J Med Psychol
,
1977
, vol.
50
(pg.
95
-
103
)
Panksepp
J
Lensing
P
,
Brief report: A synopsis of an open-trial of naltrexone treatment of autism with four children
J Autism Dev Disord
,
1991
, vol.
21
(pg.
243
-
249
)
Paulk
HH
Dienske
H
Ribbens
LG
,
Abnormal behavior in relation to cage size in rhesus monkeys
J Abnorm Psychol
,
1977
, vol.
86
(pg.
87
-
92
)
Pazol
KA
Bloomsmith
MA
,
The development of stereotyped body rocking in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) reared in a variety of nursery settings
Anim Welf
,
1993
, vol.
2
(pg.
113
-
129
)
Pomerantz
O
Terkel
J
,
Effects of positive reinforcement training techniques on the psychological welfare of zoo-housed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
Am J Primatol
,
2009
, vol.
71
(pg.
687
-
695
)
Pomerantz
O
Terkel
J
Suomi
SJ
Paukner
A
,
Stereotypic head twirls, but not pacing, are related to a “pessimistic”-like judgment bias among captive tufted capuchins (Cebus apella)
Anim Cogn
,
2012
, vol.
15
(pg.
689
-
698
)
Prior
M
Macmillan
MB
,
Maintenance of sameness in children with Kanner's syndrome
J Autism and Child Schizophr
,
1973
, vol.
3
(pg.
154
-
167
)
Rago
WV
Jr
Case
JC
,
Stereotyped behavior in special education teachers
Except Child
,
1978
, vol.
44
(pg.
342
-
344
)
Rapp
JT
Dozier
CL
Carr
JE
Patel
MR
Enloe
KA
,
Functional analysis of hair manipulation: A replication and extension
Behav Intervent
,
2000
, vol.
15
(pg.
121
-
133
)
Rapp
JT
Miltenberger
RG
Galensky
TL
Ellingson
SA
Long
ES
,
A functional analysis of hair pulling
J Appl Behav Anal
,
1999
, vol.
32
(pg.
329
-
337
)
Rapp
JT
Vollmer
TR
,
Stereotypy I: A review of behavioral assessment and treatment
Res Dev Disabil
,
2005
, vol.
26
(pg.
527
-
547
)
Repp
AC
Deitz
SM
Deitz
DED
,
Reducing inappropriate behaviors in classrooms and in individual sessions through DRO schedules of reinforcement
Ment Retard
,
1976
, vol.
14
(pg.
11
-
15
)
Ridley
RM
Baker
HF
,
Stereotypy in monkeys and humans
Psychol Med
,
1982
, vol.
12
(pg.
61
-
72
)
Saka
E
Goodrich
C
Harlan
P
Madras
BK
Graybiel
AM
,
Repetitive behaviors in monkeys are linked to specific striatal activation patterns
J Neurosci
,
2004
, vol.
24
(pg.
7557
-
7565
)
Sallustro
F
Atwell
CW
,
Body rocking, head banging, and head rolling in normal children
J Pediatr
,
1978
, vol.
93
(pg.
704
-
708
)
Sams-Dodd
F
Newman
JD
,
Effects of administration regime on the psychotomimetic properties of d-amphetamine in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus)
Pharmacol Biochem Behav
,
1997
, vol.
56
(pg.
471
-
480
)
Sandman
CA
Barron
JL
Chicz-DeMet
A
DeMet
EM
,
Plasma B-endorphin levels in patients with self-injurious behavior and stereotypy
Am J Ment Retard
,
1990
, vol.
95
(pg.
84
-
92
)
Schlemmer
RF
Jr.
Narasimhachari
N
Davis
JM
,
Dose-dependent behavioural changes induced by apomorphine in selected members of a primate social colony
J Pharm Pharmacol
,
1980
, vol.
32
(pg.
285
-
289
)
Scraggs
PR
Ridley
RM
,
Behavioural effects of amphetamine in a small primate: Relative potencies of the d- and l-isomers
Psychopharmacology
,
1978
, vol.
59
(pg.
243
-
245
)
Sidener
TM
Carr
JE
Firth
AM
,
Superimposition and withholding of edible consequences as treatment for automatically reinforced stereotypy
J Appl Behav Anal
,
2005
, vol.
38
(pg.
121
-
124
)
Singh
NN
Aman
MG
,
Effects of thioridazine dosage on the behavior of severely mentally retarded persons
Am J Ment Defic
,
1981
, vol.
85
(pg.
580
-
587
)
Smith
SG
Gupta
KK
Smith
SH
,
Effects of naltrexone on self-injury, stereotypy, and social behavior of adults with developmental disabilities
J Dev Phys Disabil
,
1995
, vol.
7
(pg.
137
-
146
)
Soussignan
R
Koch
P
,
Rhythmical stereotypies (leg-swinging) associated with reductions in heart-rate in normal school children
Biol Psychol
,
1985
, vol.
21
(pg.
161
-
167
)
Spencer
AH
Rickards
H
Fasano
A
Cavanna
AE
,
The prevalence and clinical characteristics of punding in Parkinson's disease
Mov Disord
,
2011
, vol.
26
(pg.
578
-
586
)
Spring
SE
Clifford
JO
Tomko
DL
,
Effect of environmental enrichment devices on behaviors of single- and group-housed squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus)
Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci
,
1997
, vol.
36
(pg.
72
-
75
)
Stevenson-Hinde
J
Zunz
M
,
Subjective assessment of individual rhesus monkeys
Primates
,
1978
, vol.
19
(pg.
473
-
482
)
Suomi
SJ
Gittelman
R
,
Anxiety-like disorders in young nonhuman primates
Anxiety Disorders of Childhood
,
1986
New York
Guilford Press
(pg.
1
-
23
)
Suomi
SJ
Harlow
HF
Kimball
SD
,
Behavioral effects of prolonged partial social isolation in the rhesus monkey
Psychol Rep
,
1971
, vol.
29
(pg.
1171
-
1177
)
Tarou
LR
Bloomsmith
MA
Maple
TL
,
Survey of stereotypic behavior in prosimians
Am J Primatol
,
2005
, vol.
65
(pg.
181
-
196
)
Thelen
E
,
Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants
Anim Behav
,
1979
, vol.
27
(pg.
699
-
715
)
Thoren
P
Asberg
M
Bertilsson
L
Mellstrom
B
Sjoqvist
F
Traskman
L
,
Clomipramine treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: II. Biochemical aspects
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1980
, vol.
37
(pg.
1289
-
1294
)
Troisi
A
,
Displacement activities as a behavioral measure of stress in nonhuman primates and human subjects
Stress
,
2002
, vol.
5
(pg.
47
-
54
)
Troisi
A
Schino
G
,
Environmental and social influences on autogrooming behaviour in a captive group of Java monkeys
Behaviour
,
1987
, vol.
100
(pg.
292
-
302
)
Troisi
A
Schino
G
D'Antoni
M
Pandolfi
N
Aureli
F
D'Amato
FR
,
Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers
Behav Neur Biol
,
1991
, vol.
56
(pg.
307
-
313
)
Troster
H
Brambring
M
Beelmann
A
,
The age dependence of stereotyped behaviours in blind infants and preschoolers
Child Care Health Dev
,
1991
, vol.
17
(pg.
137
-
157
)
Turner
M
Russell
J
,
Towards an executive dysfunction account of repetitive behaviour in autism
Autism as an Executive Disorder
,
1997
New York
Oxford University Press
(pg.
57
-
100
)
Turner
M
,
Annotation: Repetitive behaviour in autism: A review of psychological research
J Child Psychol Psychiat
,
1999
, vol.
40
(pg.
839
-
849
)
Vandeleest
JJ
McCowan
B
Capitanio
JP
,
Early rearing interacts with temperament and housing to influence the risk for motor stereotypy in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2011
, vol.
132
(pg.
81
-
89
)
Waitt
C
Buchanan-Smith
HM
,
What time is feeding? How delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump-tailed macaque behavior
Appl Anim Behav Sci
,
2001
, vol.
75
(pg.
75
-
85
)
Walsh
PG
,
A survey of stereotyped behaviours exhibited by adults with learning disability living in a residential centre in the West of Ireland
Brit J Learn Disabil
,
1994
, vol.
22
(pg.
90
-
93
)
Warren
SA
Burns
NR
,
Crib confinement as a factor in repetitive and stereotyped behavior in retardates
Ment Retard
,
1970
, vol.
8
(pg.
25
-
28
)
Wechsler
B
,
Stereotypies in polar bears
Zoo Biol
,
1991
, vol.
10
(pg.
177
-
188
)
Werry
JS
Carlielle
J
Fitzpatrick
J
,
Rhythmic motor activities (stereotypies) in children under five: etiology and prevalence
J Am Acad Child Psychiatry
,
1983
, vol.
22
(pg.
329
-
336
)
Wiepkema
PR
Schouten
WGP
,
Stereotypies in sows during chronic stress
Psychother Psychosom
,
1992
, vol.
57
(pg.
194
-
199
)
Wieseler
NA
Hanson
RH
Chamberlain
TP
Thompson
T
,
Functional taxonomy of stereotypic and self-injurious behavior
Ment Retard
,
1985
, vol.
23
(pg.
230
-
234
)
Willemsen-Swinkels
SHN
Buitelaar
JK
Nijhof
GJ
van Engeland
H
,
Failure of naltrexone hydrochloride to reduce self-injurious and autistic behavior in mentally retarded adults
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1995a
, vol.
52
(pg.
766
-
773
)
Willemsen-Swinkels
SHN
Buitelaar
JK
Weijnen
FG
van Engeland
H
,
Placebo-controlled acute dosage naltrexone study in young autistic children
Psychiatry Res
,
1995b
, vol.
58
(pg.
203
-
215
)
Williams
PG
Allard
A
Sears
L
Dalrymple
N
Bloom
AS
,
Brief report: Case reports on naltrexone use in children with autism: Controlled observations regarding benefits and practical issues of medication management
J Autism Dev Disord
,
2001
, vol.
31
(pg.
103
-
108
)
Willner
P
,
Validation criteria for animal models of human mental disorders: Learned helplessness as a paradigm case
Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiat
,
1986
, vol.
10
(pg.
677
-
690
)
Wolff
PH
,
The role of biological rhythms in early psychological development
Bull Menninger Clin
,
1967
, vol.
31
(pg.
197
-
218
)
Wolff
PH
Simmons
MA
,
Nonnutritive sucking and response thresholds in young infants
Child Dev
,
1967
, vol.
38
(pg.
631
-
638
)
Woo
CC
Leon
M
,
Environmental enrichment as an effective treatment for autism: A randomized controlled trial
Behav Neurosci
,
2013
, vol.
127
(pg.
487
-
497
)
Woods
DW
Miltenberger
RG
,
Are persons with nervous habits nervous? A preliminary examination of habit function in a nonreferred population
J Appl Behav Anal
,
1996
, vol.
29
(pg.
259
-
261
)
Young
R
Clements
J
,
The functional significance of complex hand movement stereotypies in the severely retarded
Brit J Devel Disabil
,
1979
, vol.
25
(pg.
79
-
87
)
Zentall
SS
Zentall
TR
,
Optimal stimulation: A model of disordered activity and performance in normal and deviant children
Psychol Bull
,
1983
, vol.
94
(pg.
446
-
471
)
Zohar
J
Insel
TR
,
Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Psychobiological approaches to diagnosis, treatment, and pathophysiology. Biol
Psychiatry
,
1987
, vol.
22
(pg.
667
-
687
)
Zohar
J
Mueller
EA
Insel
TR
Zohar-Kadouch
RC
Murphy
DL
,
Serotonergic responsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder
Arch Gen Psychiatry
,
1987
, vol.
44
(pg.
946
-
951
)

Author notes

Corrine Lutz, PhD, is the Director of Behavioral Services at the Southwest National Primate Research Center at the Texas Biomedical Research Institute in San Antonio, Texas.