Abstract

Background

Approach system considered a motivational system that activates reward-seeking behavior is associated with exploration/impulsivity, whereas avoidance system considered an attentional system that promotes inhibition of appetitive responses is associated with active overt withdrawal. Approach and avoidance dispositions are modulated by distinct neurochemical profiles and synaptic patterns. However, the precise working of neurons and trafficking of molecules in the brain activity predisposing to approach and avoidance are yet unclear.

Methods

In 3 phenotypes of inbred mice, avoiding, balancing, and approaching mice, selected by using the Approach/Avoidance Y-maze, we analyzed endogenous brain levels of brain derived neurotrophic factor, one of the main secretory proteins with pleiotropic action. To verify the effects of the acute increase of brain derived neurotrophic factor, balancing and avoiding mice were bilaterally brain derived neurotrophic factor-infused in the cortical cerebellar regions.

Results

Approaching animals showed high levels of explorative behavior and response to novelty and exhibited higher brain derived neurotrophic factor levels in the cerebellar structures in comparison to the other 2 phenotypes of mice. Interestingly, brain derived neurotrophic factor-infused balancing and avoiding mice significantly increased their explorative behavior and response to novelty.

Conclusions

Cerebellar brain derived neurotrophic factor may play a role in explorative and novelty-seeking responses that sustain the approach predisposition.

Significance Statement

The present research deals with the neurobiological basis of individual differences, in particular the predispositions in approach and avoidance. The findings indicated that the individual differences in the inherent predisposition to approach were associated with the basal cerebellar brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels, and that the cerebellar BDNF levels were causally related with the acute response of approach in its double component of exploration and search for novelty. Here, we are proposing that the basal endogenous or acutely increased cerebellar BDNF levels promote explorative response and searching for novelty, components that sustain the approach pattern.

Introduction

Novelty- and reward-seeking behavior associated with exploration is typical of the motivational approach system, whereas appetitive response inhibition and active withdrawal are representative of the attentional avoidance system (McNaughton and Gray, 2000; Pickering and Gray, 2001; Carver and Miller, 2006). Within the same species, some individuals have an overt tendency toward positive (e.g., rewarding) or away from negative (e.g., dangerous) stimuli, neophilic or neophobic responses, exploratory or withdrawal behaviors (Greenberg, 2003; Laricchiuta and Petrosini, 2014; Laricchiuta, 2015). Thus, individual differences in approach and avoidance may be considered constitutionally ingrained stable traits (Elliot, 2005, 2008; Sullivan et al., 2008; Helfinstein et al., 2011).

Approach and avoidance predispositions emerge from mechanisms operative in the whole brain, from spinal cord (Schutter et al., 2011) to brainstem (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998; Challis et al., 2013), to cortical regions (Nasser and McNally, 2012). Even if it is known that the intensity of the appetitive or defensive behavior is modulated by the levels of specific neurotransmitters and neuromodulators (Berridge, 2000; Linfoot et al., 2009; Groppe et al., 2013; Mogi et al., 2014), the precise working of neurons and trafficking of molecules in determining approach and avoidance patterns are yet unclear.

In an attempt to clarify this issue, we selected 3 phenotypes of inbred mice that spontaneously react to conflicting (simultaneously rewarding and threating) stimuli with withdrawing (avoiding [AV] mice), balanced (balancing [BA] mice), or advancing (approaching [AP] mice) responses (Laricchiuta et al., 2012a, 2012b, 2014a, 2016). The presynaptic control of cannabinoid type-1 (CB1) receptors on GABAergic transmission in the dorsostriatal medium spiny neurons is nearly absent in the AV mice and conversely markedly increased in the AP mice in comparison with BA mice (Laricchiuta et al., 2012b). Further, when compared with BA animals, both AP and AV animals have greater CB1 receptor density in the amygdaloid nuclei and ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus, and only AP animals have also higher CB1 receptor functionality in the amygdaloid nuclei and dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus (Laricchiuta et al., 2012a). Evidence suggests that density and functionality of CB1 receptors in the corticolimbic, striatal, and cerebellar areas correlate with the levels of brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), an activity-regulated secretory protein with pleiotropic action widely expressed within the previously quoted areas. In fact, BDNF brain levels are lower in mice lacking CB1 receptors (Aso et al., 2008), whereas CB1 activation increases BDNF levels in rodents (Butovsky et al., 2005) and humans (D’Souza et al., 2009). The increased BDNF release triggered by CB1 stimulation mediates in turn the neuroprotective effects of endocannabinoids (Khaspekov et al., 2004). The mutant DISC1 mice, model of schizophrenia-like endophenotype, display impaired preference for social novelty, reduced BDNF receptor levels in prefrontal cortex, and diminished CB1 expression in hippocampus (Kaminitz et al., 2014). BDNF inhibits CB1 response in the visual cortex (Huang et al., 2008) and increases the expression of CB1 receptor transcripts in cultured cerebellar granule neurons (Maison et al., 2009). However, striatal BDNF inhibits CB1 functionality, and this interplay crucially controls the emotional consequences of stressful or rewarding experiences (Berton et al., 2006; De Chiara et al., 2010). Furthermore, prolonged exposure to palatable food suppresses CB1 receptor gene expression and reduces BDNF levels (Martire et al., 2014). Interestingly, adult offspring of dams treated with corticosterone and fed a tryptophan-deficient diet show increased avoidance behaviors and anhedonia toward highly palatable reward, reduced striatal and increased hypothalamic BDNF levels, and reduced dopamine and serotonin levels in prefrontal cortex (Zoratto et al., 2013).

Starting from BDNF’s role as CB1 mediator and from observation that endocannabinoid activity is linked to the individual differences in approach and avoidance stable predispositions (Laricchiuta et al., 2012a, 2012b), it is possible to hypothesize that the difference in basal BDNF levels can represent a neurobiological marker of individual differences in approach and avoidance enduring tendencies. Thus, the present research firstly analyzed the endogenous BDNF levels in the 3 AV, BA, and AP phenotypes of mice in frontal cortex, hippocampus, striatum, and cerebellum, regions involved in approach and avoidance stable traits (Laricchiuta et al., 2012a, 2012b; Picerni et al., 2013; Laricchiuta et al., 2014a, 2014b, 2014c, 2016) and expressing high BDNF levels (Angelucci et al., 2009; De Chiara et al., 2010; Caporali et al., 2014; Cutuli et al., 2015). The present results indicate that in comparison with AV and BA mice, AP mice showed higher BDNF levels only in the cerebellum. Subsequently, we thus performed bilateral BDNF infusions in the cerebellar cortical regions to investigate whether even acutely increased cerebellar BDNF levels promoted explorative and novelty-seeking responses, typical components of the approach predisposition.

Methods

Subjects and Experimental Procedure

Male C57BL/6JOlaHsd mice (n=198; 40 d old at study onset) (Envigo) were housed 4 per cage, with food (Mucedola) and water ad libitum. The mice were kept under a 12-h-light/-dark cycle, controlled temperature (22°C–23°C), and constant humidity (60%±5%). All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering and reduce the number of animals used, per the European Directive (2010/63/EU).

The timeline of the experimental procedure is reported in Figure 1. Based on the distribution of responses in the Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-maze, we selected AV (n=5), BA (n=5), and AP (n=5) animals. After 2 weeks, the animals were tested in the open field with novel object (OFo) task. To analyze the BDNF role in approach and avoidance stable dispositions, 2 weeks later the animals were killed to determine endogenous BDNF brain levels.

Timeline of the experimental procedure. (A) Avoiding (AV, n=5), balancing (BA, n=5), and approaching (AP, n=5) mice were selected based on the distribution of responses in the session 1 (S1) and session 2 (S2) of the Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-maze. Two weeks later, the animals were tested in the Open Field with novel object (OFo) task. Two weeks later the animals were killed to determine endogenous BDNF brain levels through enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay procedure. (B) Other BA (n=36) mice were bilaterally implanted with guide cannulas into the cerebellar cortical regions. Twenty-four hours later, 21 of 36 BA animals were injected with PBS (BA-PBS, n=7), 0.25 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.25, n=7), or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.75, n=7). Two hours later, the animals performed the OFo task. Seventy-two hours after cannula implantations, the remaining 15 BA mice were injected with PBS (n=5) or 0.25 (n=5) or 0.75 (n=5) μg/μL PBS/side BDNF. Two hours later, the animals were retested in session 3 (S3) of A/A Y-Maze. All BA (n=36) infused animals were injected with 1 μL/side of PBS containing methylene blue to perform histological control of BDNF injection sites and diffusion. Two hours later, they were killed by decapitation. (C) Other AV (n=8) mice were bilaterally implanted with guide cannulas into the cerebellar cortical regions. Twenty-four hours later, they were injected with PBS (AV-PBS, n=4), or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (AV-BDNF 0.75, n=4). Two hours later, the animals performed the OFo task. Forty-eight hours later, the AV mice were re-injected with PBS or BDNF. Two hours later, the animals were retested in A/A Y-Maze S3. The AV infused-animals were injected with 1 μL/side of PBS containing methylene blue and 2 h later they were killed by decapitation.
Figure 1.

Timeline of the experimental procedure. (A) Avoiding (AV, n=5), balancing (BA, n=5), and approaching (AP, n=5) mice were selected based on the distribution of responses in the session 1 (S1) and session 2 (S2) of the Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-maze. Two weeks later, the animals were tested in the Open Field with novel object (OFo) task. Two weeks later the animals were killed to determine endogenous BDNF brain levels through enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay procedure. (B) Other BA (n=36) mice were bilaterally implanted with guide cannulas into the cerebellar cortical regions. Twenty-four hours later, 21 of 36 BA animals were injected with PBS (BA-PBS, n=7), 0.25 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.25, n=7), or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.75, n=7). Two hours later, the animals performed the OFo task. Seventy-two hours after cannula implantations, the remaining 15 BA mice were injected with PBS (n=5) or 0.25 (n=5) or 0.75 (n=5) μg/μL PBS/side BDNF. Two hours later, the animals were retested in session 3 (S3) of A/A Y-Maze. All BA (n=36) infused animals were injected with 1 μL/side of PBS containing methylene blue to perform histological control of BDNF injection sites and diffusion. Two hours later, they were killed by decapitation. (C) Other AV (n=8) mice were bilaterally implanted with guide cannulas into the cerebellar cortical regions. Twenty-four hours later, they were injected with PBS (AV-PBS, n=4), or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (AV-BDNF 0.75, n=4). Two hours later, the animals performed the OFo task. Forty-eight hours later, the AV mice were re-injected with PBS or BDNF. Two hours later, the animals were retested in A/A Y-Maze S3. The AV infused-animals were injected with 1 μL/side of PBS containing methylene blue and 2 h later they were killed by decapitation.

Two weeks after the A/A Y-Maze, other AV (n=8) and BA (n=36) mice were bilaterally implanted with guide cannulas into the cerebellar cortical regions. Twenty-four hours later, the AV and BA (n=21 of 36) animals were injected with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) (group names: BA-PBS, n=7; AV-PBS, n=4) or 0.25 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (group name: BA-BDNF 0.25, n=7) or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (group names: BA-BDNF 0.75, n=7; AV-BDNF 0.75, n=4). Two hours later, all animals performed the OFo task.

Seventy-two hours after cannula implantations, the remaining BA (n=15) mice were injected with PBS (n=5) or 0.25 (n=5) or 0.75 (n=5) μg/μL PBS/side BDNF. In parallel, the AV mice were re-injected with PBS or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF. Two hours later, all animals were re-tested in the A/A Y-Maze.

To perform histological control of BDNF injection sites and diffusion, the mice infused with PBS or BDNF were injected with 1 μL/side of PBS containing methylene blue, and 2 h later they were killed by decapitation (supplementary Methods; Figure 2).

Histological control of bilateral brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) injection sites and diffusion in the cerebellar cortex. A representative photomicrograph (2x magnification) showing the left methylene blue injection site and the cerebellar cortical diffusion is superimposed to a drawing adapted from Franklin and Paxinos (1997). Abbreviations: Med, medial (fastigial) cerebellar nucleus; Int, interpositus cerebellar nucleus; IntA, interpositus cerebellar nucleus, anterior; Lat, lateral (dentate) cerebellar nucleus; LatPC, lateral cerebellar nucleus, parvicellular; PFl, paraflocculus; Fl, flocculus; MVePC, medial vestibular nucleus, parvicellular; MVeMC, medial vestibular nucleus, magnocellular; LVe, lateral vestibular nucleus; SpVe, spinal vestibular nucleus; DC, dorsal cochlear nucleus; 7N, facial nucleus.
Figure 2.

Histological control of bilateral brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) injection sites and diffusion in the cerebellar cortex. A representative photomicrograph (2x magnification) showing the left methylene blue injection site and the cerebellar cortical diffusion is superimposed to a drawing adapted from Franklin and Paxinos (1997). Abbreviations: Med, medial (fastigial) cerebellar nucleus; Int, interpositus cerebellar nucleus; IntA, interpositus cerebellar nucleus, anterior; Lat, lateral (dentate) cerebellar nucleus; LatPC, lateral cerebellar nucleus, parvicellular; PFl, paraflocculus; Fl, flocculus; MVePC, medial vestibular nucleus, parvicellular; MVeMC, medial vestibular nucleus, magnocellular; LVe, lateral vestibular nucleus; SpVe, spinal vestibular nucleus; DC, dorsal cochlear nucleus; 7N, facial nucleus.

Behavioral Testing

A/A Y-Maze

The test implemented to select approach/avoidance phenotypes has been previously described (Laricchiuta et al., 2012a, 2014a, 2014b, 2016) and is accurately detailed in the supplementary Methods. The apparatus consisted of a Y-maze with a starting gray arm and 2 choice arms: 1 black and dark, the other one white and lit.

In Session 1 (S1), the slightly food-deprived animal could choose to enter one of the 2 arms, both containing the same standard food reward. During Session 2 (S2), which started 24 h after S1, the white arm was rewarded with a new palatable food (Fonzies, KP Snack Foods) (Bassareo et al., 2002), while the black arm remained rewarded with the standard food. Notably, the S2 of A/A test required to choose between 2 conflicting drives: reaching the new palatable reward placed in an aversive (white and lighted) environment or reaching the familiar standard food placed in a reassuring (black and dark) environment. The slightly food-deprived animals to be re-tested were submitted to a new session (S3) applying the S2 protocol (Laricchiuta et al., 2012b, 2014a).

The parameters considered were: white choices, frequency of entry into the white arm in S1, S2, and S3; A/A conflict index, the difference in the number of white choices between S2 and S1. Given that this index was normally distributed, it allowed us to identify the 3 AV, BA, and AP phenotypes. In particular, BA animals (22% of mice) showed values in the A/A conflict index corresponding to the mean of the distribution. The 2 tails of the distribution curve represented the few subjects that exhibited responses unbalanced toward one of the conflicting inputs: AV animals (7% of mice) had A/A conflict index values corresponding to -2 SDs of the mean, while AP animals (6% of mice) had values corresponding to + 2 SDs of the mean.

OFo Task

To eliminate the “food” and “palatability” dimensions and maintain the conflicting drives given by an appealing new object placed in an anxiogenic central location of a wide arena, the OFo task (detailed in the supplementary Methods) was used (Laricchiuta et al., 2012b, 2014a). In S1 the animal was allowed to explore an empty 60-cm circular arena, while in S2 an object (a gray plastic cone: 10×6 cm; base diameter :  9.5 cm) was put in the arena center. Notably, the approach to the object requires the subject to overcome its innate fear toward open spaces and indicates thus that the animal is reacting to the mismatch between the initial (empty arena) and new (presence of the object) situations.

The parameters considered were: total distance (in cm) traveled in the arena in each session; peripheral distance, the percentage of total distance traveled in a 6-cm peripheral annulus in each session; central distance, the percentage of total distance traveled in a central circular area (diameter 21.5 cm) in each session; mean velocity in each session; contact time with the object.

BDNF Determination

Tissue Dissection

The AV, BA, and AP mice (n=5/group) were decapitated, and the brains were removed and dissected on ice using a binocular dissection microscope. Frontal cortex, hippocampus, striatum, and cerebellum were bilaterally collected according to Glowinski and Iversen’s method (1966). All regions were extracted in 1 mL extraction buffer/100 mg tissue. Brain tissue samples were homogenized in an ice-cold lysis buffer containing 137 mM NaCl, 20 mM Tris-HCl (pH 8.0), 1% NP40, 10% glycerol, 1 mM phenylmethanesulfonylfluoride, 10 mg/mL aprotinin, 1 mg/mL leupetin, and 0.5 mM sodium vanadate. The tissue homogenate solutions were centrifuged at 14000g for 25 minutes at 4°C. The supernatants were collected and stored at -80°C until analysis for quantification of BDNF.

Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay

BDNF concentrations were assessed using a 2-site enzyme immunoassay kit (G7610 Promega). In brief, 96-well immunoplates (NUNC) were coated with 50 μL/well with the corresponding capture antibody and stored overnight at 4°C. The next day, serial dilutions of known amounts of BDNF ranging from 0 to 500 pg/mL were performed in duplicate to generate a standard curve. The plates were washed 3 times with wash buffer, and the standard curves and supernatants of brain tissue homogenates were incubated in the coated wells (100 μL each) for 2 h at room temperature (RT) with shaking. After additional washes, the antigen was incubated with second specific antibody for 2 h at RT. The plates were washed again with wash buffer and then incubated with an anti-IgY HRP for 1 h at RT. After another wash, the plates were incubated with a TMB/peroxidase substrate solution for 15 min and phosphoric acid 1 M (100 μL/well) was added to the wells. The colorimetric reaction product was measured at 450 nm using a microplate reader (Dynatech MR 5000). BDNF concentrations were determined from the regression line for the BDNF standard (ranging from 7.8 to 500 pg/mL-purified mice BDNF) incubated under similar conditions in each assay. As declared by the company (Promega), the cross-reactivity with other related neurotrophic factors (NGF, NT-3, and NT-4) was <3%. BDNF concentration was expressed as pg/g wet weight. All assays were performed in triplicate.

Cerebellar BDNF Infusion

Mice were anesthetized by using Zoletil 100 (tiletamine HCl 50 mg/mL + zolazepam HCl 50 mg/mL; Virbac) and Rompun 20 (xylazine 20 mg/mL; Bayer S.p.A) dissolved in a volume of 4.1 mg/mL and 1.6 mg/mL, respectively, in saline and i.p. injected in a volume of 7.3 mL/kg. Mice were mounted onto a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments) equipped with a mouse adapter and bilaterally implanted with guide cannulas (stainless steel, shaft outer diameter 0.38 mm, Metalant AB) lowered 0.3 mm from the scalp in cerebellar cortical regions. The coordinates from bregma, measured according to the atlas of Franklin and Paxinos (1997) and Mouse Brain Atlases (The Mouse Brain Library, www.nervenet.org), were: AP -6.1; L ±2.2. The guide cannulas were fixed with epoxy glue and dental cement. The length of the guide cannulas was 4.5 mm. Twenty-four hours after the guide cannula implantations, the infusion cannulas (diameter 0.25 mm; Unimed) were bilaterally inserted into the guide cannulas, so that 0.6 mm of the infusion cannula extended past the end of the guide cannula. According to Saylor and McGinty (2010), human recombinant BDNF (Tocris Bioscience, R & D Systems) at 2 concentrations (0.25 or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side) or sterile PBS was infused into cerebellar regions by using 10-μL Hamilton syringes and an infusion pump (Harvard Apparatus). Volumes of 1 μL/side of BDNF or PBS were infused over 5 min, and the infusion cannulas remained in the guide cannulas for 5 min before and after the infusion. Two hours later, mice were submitted to behavioral testing.

Statistical Analysis

Data presented as mean ± SEM were tested for normality (Will-Shapiro’s test) and homoscedasticity (Levene’s test). Behavioral and neurochemical data were compared by using ANOVAs, followed by Tukey’s HSD test when appropriate. When data did not fully meet parametric assumptions, nonparametric analyses (Friedman ANOVA, Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test, and Mann–Whitney U) were used. Linear regression analyses were run to determine the associations between cerebellar BDNF levels and A/A conflict index and white choices in the S2 of A/A Y-Maze, or distances, velocity, and contact time with the novel object in the OFo task sessions. The differences were considered significant at the P<.05 level.

Results

A/A Y-Maze Performances of AV, BA, and AP Mice

The A/A conflict index was normally distributed (Figure 3A), and its bell-shaped curve indicated that in S2 the new palatable food, even if placed in the aversive white environment, was salient enough to increase white choices number.

Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze performances of avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice. (A) The curve of distribution of the A/A conflict index (the difference [Δ] in the number of white choices between session 2 [S2] and 1 [S1]) indicates that the white choice frequency increased (mean = Δ+1). (B) The white choices were similar among phenotypes in S1, while AV mice showed the lowest number of white choices (***P=.0002), and BA mice showed a number of white choices lower than AP mice (***P=.0002) in S2. Between S1 and S2, the number of white choices within groups was different (***P=.0002), given that it decreased in the AV mice and increased in the BA and AP mice. In B, data are presented as means ± SEM.
Figure 3.

Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze performances of avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice. (A) The curve of distribution of the A/A conflict index (the difference [Δ] in the number of white choices between session 2 [S2] and 1 [S1]) indicates that the white choice frequency increased (mean = Δ+1). (B) The white choices were similar among phenotypes in S1, while AV mice showed the lowest number of white choices (***P=.0002), and BA mice showed a number of white choices lower than AP mice (***P=.0002) in S2. Between S1 and S2, the number of white choices within groups was different (***P=.0002), given that it decreased in the AV mice and increased in the BA and AP mice. In B, data are presented as means ± SEM.

When white choices in S1 and S2 were analyzed in relation to the phenotype of the animals (Figure 3B), a 2-way ANOVA (phenotype x session) revealed significant phenotype (F2,12=12.93, P=.001) and session (F1,12=14.3, P=.001) effects. The interaction was significant (F2,12=42.0, P=.0001). Posthoc comparisons on interaction revealed no significant differences in S1 among AV, BA, and AP mice. In S2, while AV mice showed the lowest number of white choices (always P=.0002), BA mice showed a number of white choices lower than AP mice (P=.0002). Between S1 and S2, the number of white choices was significantly different (always P=.0002) in the 3 phenotypes, given that it decreased in the AV mice and increased in the BA and AP mice.

OFo Performances of AV, BA, and AP Mice

AP mice were significantly more active and explorative than AV and BA animals. Two-way ANOVA (phenotype×session) on total distances (Figure 4A) revealed a significant phenotype effect (F2,12=8.82, P=.004), while session effect (F1,12=2.67, P=.13) and interaction (F2,12=3.19, P=.08) were not significant. Posthoc comparisons on phenotype effect revealed that AP animals explored the environment more actively than BA (P=.003) and AV (P=.05) animals. Two-way ANOVA (phenotype×session) on peripheral distances (Figure 4B) revealed no significant phenotype effect (F2,12=1.30, P=.31), while session effect (F1,12=1 87.66, P<.00001) was significant. Interaction (F2,12=1.43, P=.28) was not significant. Two-way ANOVA (phenotype×session) on central distances (mean±SE: AV: S1 12.5 ± 1.9, S2 41.0 ± 5.0; BA: S1 10.5 ± 2.5, S2 49.8 ± 2.9; AP: S1 13.7 ± 3.7, S2 47.6 ± 2.3) showed no significant phenotype effect (F2,12=1.39, P=.29), while session effect (F1,12=1 94.62, P<.00001) was significant. Interaction (F2,12=1.66, P=.23) was not significant. Two-way ANOVA (phenotype×session) on velocity (Figure 4C) revealed a significant phenotype effect (F2,12=8.88, P=.004), while session effect (F1,12=2.70, P=.13) and interaction (F2,12=3.31, P=.07) were not significant. Posthoc comparisons on phenotype effect revealed that AP animals were more rapid than BA (P=.003) and AV (P=.05) animals. One-way ANOVA on contact time with the novel object (Figure 4D) revealed a significant phenotype effect (F2,12=15.46, P=.0005). In fact, the AP animals contacted the novel object longer than BA (P=.003) and AV (P=.007) animals.

Open Field with object (OFo) performances of avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice. Total distances (A), peripheral distances (B), and velocity (C) in session 1 (S1) and 2 (S2) are reported. Peripheral distances (B) decreased between S1 and S2 (***P=.001). AP animals were more active and rapid in exploring the environment (A, C), and contacted the novel object (D) longer than BA (**P=.003) and AV (*P=.007) animals. Data are presented as means ± SEM.
Figure 4.

Open Field with object (OFo) performances of avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice. Total distances (A), peripheral distances (B), and velocity (C) in session 1 (S1) and 2 (S2) are reported. Peripheral distances (B) decreased between S1 and S2 (***P=.001). AP animals were more active and rapid in exploring the environment (A, C), and contacted the novel object (D) longer than BA (**P=.003) and AV (*P=.007) animals. Data are presented as means ± SEM.

Brain BDNF Determination in AV, BA, and AP Mice

One-way ANOVA on BDNF levels in the cerebellum revealed a significant phenotype effect (F2,12=6.64, P=.01). Posthoc comparisons revealed that AP animals exhibited the highest BDNF cerebellar levels (AP vs BA: P=.05; AP vs AV: P=.01; BA vs AV: P=.73) (Figure 5A). Conversely, 1-way ANOVAs on BDNF levels in the frontal cortex (F2,12=1.63, P=.24), hippocampus (F2,12=1.13, P=.35), and striatum (F2,12=0.38, P=.69) failed to reveal any significant difference among the 3 phenotypes (Figure 5B-D).

Brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) determination in avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice in the cerebellum (A), frontal cortex (B), hippocampus (C), and striatum (D). AP animals showed the highest cerebellar BDNF levels (*P=.05; **P=.01). Data are presented as means ± SEM and values are expressed in pg/g of wet weight (WW).
Figure 5.

Brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) determination in avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice in the cerebellum (A), frontal cortex (B), hippocampus (C), and striatum (D). AP animals showed the highest cerebellar BDNF levels (*P=.05; **P=.01). Data are presented as means ± SEM and values are expressed in pg/g of wet weight (WW).

Linear Regressions between Cerebellar BDNF Levels and Behavioral Data in AV, BA, and AP Mice

Positive significant associations were found between cerebellar BDNF levels and A/A conflict index (ß=0.69, F1,14=12.03, P=.0041), number of white choices in the S2 of the A/A Y-Maze (ß=0.59, F1,14=6.89, P=.02), and contact times with the novel object (ß=0.61, F1,14=7.83, P=.01) in the OFo task (Figure 6A-C). No significant associations were found between cerebellar BDNF levels and total (S1: ß=0.47, F1,14=3.65, P=.08; S2: ß=0.21, F1,14=0.63, P=.44), peripheral (S1: ß=0.33, F1,14=1.55, P=.23; S2: ß=0.09, F1,14=0.11, P=.75), and central (S1: ß=0.38, F1,14=1.75, P=.28; S2: ß=0.17, F1,14=0.18, P=.74) distances, as well as mean velocity (S1: ß=0.47, F1,14=3.72, P=.07; S2: ß=0.21, F1,14=0.62, P=.45).

Linear regressions between cerebellar brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels and behavioral data in avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice. A positive significant association was found between cerebellar BDNF levels and conflict index of the Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze (A). A positive association was found between cerebellar BDNF levels and number of white choices in the session 2 (S2) of the A/A Y-Maze (B). Positive association was also found between cerebellar BDNF levels and contact times with the novel object in the Open Field (OFo) task (C). In the scatterplots are reported the linear fits (solid black lines), equations and R2.
Figure 6.

Linear regressions between cerebellar brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels and behavioral data in avoiding (AV), balancing (BA), and approaching (AP) mice. A positive significant association was found between cerebellar BDNF levels and conflict index of the Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze (A). A positive association was found between cerebellar BDNF levels and number of white choices in the session 2 (S2) of the A/A Y-Maze (B). Positive association was also found between cerebellar BDNF levels and contact times with the novel object in the Open Field (OFo) task (C). In the scatterplots are reported the linear fits (solid black lines), equations and R2.

OFo Performances of BA Mice Infused with BDNF into the Cerebellar Cortices

BA animals infused with 0.25 or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF were significantly more active than BA animals bilaterally infused with PBS, and BA animals infused with 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF contacted the object significantly longest. Two-way ANOVA (group×session) on total distances (Figure 7A) revealed significant group (F2,18=5.89, P=.01) and session (F1,18=5.47, P=.03) effects. The interaction (F2,18=1.61, P=.22) was not significant. Posthoc comparisons on group effect revealed that the BA-PBS group was less active in moving into the arena than the BA-BDNF 0.25 (P=.02) and BA-BDNF 0.75 (P=.01) groups. Two-way ANOVA (group×session) on peripheral distances (Figure 7B) showed no significant group effect (F2,18=1.41, P=.27), while session effect (F1,18=104.85, P<.00001) was significant. Interaction (F2,18=2.15, P=.15) was not significant. Two-way ANOVA (group×session) on central distances (mean±SE: BA-PBS: - S1 9.4±1.4, - S2 41.9±0.7; BA-BDNF 0.25: - S1 11.5±0.8, - S2 30.7±2.6; BA-BDNF 0.75: - S1 12.0±1.5, - S2 40.9±6.0) showed no significant phenotype effect (F2,12=2.07, P=.16), while session effect (F1,12=6522.00, P<.00001) was significant. Interaction (F2,12=3.02, P=.08) was not significant.

Open Field with novel object (OFo) performances of balancing (BA) brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-infused mice into the cerebellar cortices. Total distances (A), peripheral distances (B), and velocity (C) are reported. Animals infused with 0.25 (BA-BDNF 0.25) or 0.75 (BA-BDNF 0.75) μg/μL PBS/side BDNF were more active and rapid in exploring the environment in comparison to BA-PBS animals. The BA-BDNF 0.75 group contacted the novel object longer (D) than the BA-BDNF 0.25 and BA-PBS (*P=.02) groups. Data are presented as means ± SEM.
Figure 7.

Open Field with novel object (OFo) performances of balancing (BA) brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-infused mice into the cerebellar cortices. Total distances (A), peripheral distances (B), and velocity (C) are reported. Animals infused with 0.25 (BA-BDNF 0.25) or 0.75 (BA-BDNF 0.75) μg/μL PBS/side BDNF were more active and rapid in exploring the environment in comparison to BA-PBS animals. The BA-BDNF 0.75 group contacted the novel object longer (D) than the BA-BDNF 0.25 and BA-PBS (*P=.02) groups. Data are presented as means ± SEM.

Two-way ANOVA (group×session) on velocity (Figure 7C) showed significant group (F2,18=6.03, P=.01) and session (F1,18=5.78, P=.03) effects. The interaction (F2,18=1.39, P=.27) was not significant. Posthoc comparisons on group effect revealed that the BA-PBS group was less rapid in moving into the environment than the BA-BDNF 0.25 (P=.02) and BA-BDNF 0.75 (P=.01) groups. One-way ANOVA on contact time with the novel object (Figure 7D) was significant (F2,18=6.16, P=.01), with the BA-BDNF 0.75 group contacting the novel object more than the BA-BDNF 0.25 and BA-PBS (always P=.02) groups.

A/A Y-Maze Performances of BA Mice Infused with BDNF into the Cerebellar Cortices

BA animals were infused with PBS or 0.25 or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF before S3. When white choices were analyzed (Figure 8), a 2-way ANOVA (group x session) revealed no significant group effect (F2,12=2.90, P=.09), while the session effect (F1,12=12.26, P=.0002) was significant. The interaction (F4,24=2.81, P=.05) was significant. Posthoc comparisons on significant interaction revealed no significant difference among groups in S1 and S2, while in S3, BA-BDNF 0.75 mice showed a number of white choices similar to the BA-BDNF 0.25 group (P=.72) and increased compared with the BA-PBS group (P=.05). The number of white choices was similar between BA-BDNF 0.25 and BA-PBS groups (P=.72).

Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze performances of balancing (BA) brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-infused mice into the cerebellar cortices. The white choices were similar among groups in session 1 (S1) and session 2 (S2), while the session 3 (S3) animals infused with 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.75) showed a number of white choices similar to animals infused with 0.25 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.25), but higher (*P=.05) than BA-PBS animals. Data are presented as means ± SEM.
Figure 8.

Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze performances of balancing (BA) brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-infused mice into the cerebellar cortices. The white choices were similar among groups in session 1 (S1) and session 2 (S2), while the session 3 (S3) animals infused with 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.75) showed a number of white choices similar to animals infused with 0.25 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (BA-BDNF 0.25), but higher (*P=.05) than BA-PBS animals. Data are presented as means ± SEM.

OFo Performances of AV Mice Infused with BDNF into the Cerebellar Cortices

AV animals infused with 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF were significantly more active and contacted the object significantly longer than AV animals bilaterally infused with PBS. Nonparametric analyses (Mann–Whitney U) on total distances (Figure 9A) revealed a significant difference between groups in S1 (U=0, P=.02) but not in S2 (U=8, P=1). Mann–Whitney U test on peripheral distances (Figure 9B) revealed no significant difference between groups in S1 (U=4, P=.25) and a significant difference in S2 (U=0, P=.02). Mann–Whitney U test on central distances (mean±SE: AV-PBS: - S1 4.0±0.7, - S2 37.9±5.2; AV-BDNF 0.75: - S1 6.9±2.5, - S2 38.6±1.8) revealed no significant differences between groups in S1 (U=6, P=.56) and S2 (U=8, P=1). Mann–Whitney U test on mean velocity (Figure 9C) revealed a significant difference between groups in S1 (U=0, P=.02) but not in S2 (U=8, P=1). Mann–Whitney U test on contact time with the novel object (Figure 9D) revealed a significant difference between groups (U=1, P=.05).

Open Field with novel object (OFo) performances of avoiding (AV) brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-infused mice into the cerebellar cortices. Total distances (A), peripheral distances (B), velocity (C), and contact time with the novel object (D) in session 1 (S1) and session 2 (S2) are reported. AV animals infused with 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (AV-BDNF 0.75) were more active (#P=.02) and rapid (#P=.02) in exploring the environment and contacted the object longer (*P=.05) than AV-PBS animals. In the AV-PBS group, total and peripheral distances and mean velocity were significantly different between S1 and S2 (*P=.05). In the AV-BDNF 0.75 group, peripheral distances were significantly different between S1 and S2 (*P=.05). Data are presented as means ± SEM.
Figure 9.

Open Field with novel object (OFo) performances of avoiding (AV) brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-infused mice into the cerebellar cortices. Total distances (A), peripheral distances (B), velocity (C), and contact time with the novel object (D) in session 1 (S1) and session 2 (S2) are reported. AV animals infused with 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF (AV-BDNF 0.75) were more active (#P=.02) and rapid (#P=.02) in exploring the environment and contacted the object longer (*P=.05) than AV-PBS animals. In the AV-PBS group, total and peripheral distances and mean velocity were significantly different between S1 and S2 (*P=.05). In the AV-BDNF 0.75 group, peripheral distances were significantly different between S1 and S2 (*P=.05). Data are presented as means ± SEM.

As regards the AV-PBS group, Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test on total, peripheral, and central distances as well as mean velocity revealed significant differences between S1 and S2 (always P=.05) (Figure 9A-C). As regards the AV-BDNF 0.75 groups, Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test showed that peripheral and central distances were significantly different between S1 and S2 (always P=.05) (Figure 9B).

A/A Y-Maze Performances of AV Mice Infused with BDNF into the Cerebellar Cortices

AV animals were re-infused with PBS or 0.75 μg/μL PBS/side BDNF before S3. When white choices were analyzed (Figure 10), Mann–Whitney U test revealed no significant differences in S1 (U=2, P=.08) and S2 (U=2, P=.08), while a significant difference between groups was found in S3 (U=1, P=.04).

Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze performances of avoiding (AV) mice brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) re-infused into the cerebellar cortices before session 3 (S3). The AV-BDNF 0.75 group showed a higher number of white choices than the AV-PBS group (*P=.04). Data are presented as means ± SEM.
Figure 10.

Approach/Avoidance (A/A) Y-Maze performances of avoiding (AV) mice brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) re-infused into the cerebellar cortices before session 3 (S3). The AV-BDNF 0.75 group showed a higher number of white choices than the AV-PBS group (*P=.04). Data are presented as means ± SEM.

Friedman ANOVAs on white choices revealed significant differences among S1, S2, and S3 in AV-PBS (P=.02) and AV-BDNF 0.75 groups (P=.02).

Discussion

According to Elliot (2008), a subject characterized by a stable predisposition of approach is explorative, curious, and seeks novelty. In fact, the AP mice were more active, rapid, and prone to explore the OFo arena by travelling longer distances in comparison with the BA and AV mice. Notably, the longest distances moved by the AP animals were not related to anxious behaviors, given that in all animals the percentages of peripheral and central distances were similar and decreased between S1 and S2. Even more importantly, the AP animals made contact with the novel object longer than BA and AV animals. To verify whether the basal BDNF levels were associated with a specific and stable predisposition to approach or avoidance, the BDNF levels in frontal cortex, hippocampus, striatum, and cerebellum were evaluated in the 3 phenotypes. These analyses were made at a time-point distant from any behavioral testing to rule out any acute effect of the behavioral performance on BDNF levels. Interestingly, the AP mice showed the highest cerebellar BDNF levels. Regression analyses indicated that the basal cerebellar BDNF levels were positively associated with the conflict index and white choices in the S2 of the A/A Y-Maze (when the aversive white environment was rewarded with the new palatable food, thus making it worth risking to enter the “threatening and anxiogenic” white and lighted arm to obtain the reward). Furthermore, the cerebellar BDNF levels were positively associated with the contact times with the novel object placed in the “threatening and anxiogenic” central location of OFo arena. Our findings indicate that high cerebellar BDNF levels may represent a biomarker associated with stable predisposition to approach in its 2 components of exploration and search for novelty. These data are consistent with the observations that the endogenous concentrations and signaling of BDNF are associated with individual differences in specific temperamental traits and dispositions (Okuno et al., 2011; Duclot and Kabbaj, 2013; Yasui-Furukori et al., 2013). It was recently reported in healthy individuals a coupling between anxiety temperamental traits and basal resting blood flow in fronto-limbic circuitry, determined in part by genetically mediated BDNF signaling (Wei et al., 2017). Furthermore, the individuals with at least 1 copy of the methionine allele in the BDNF gene show increased predisposition to anxious and depressive behaviors (Gatt et al., 2009; Terracciano et al., 2010; Verhagen et al., 2010; Minelli et al., 2011). They also exhibit increased positive mood, lower perceived exertion, and increased motivation during exercise (Bryan et al., 2007, 2013; Caldwell Hooper et al., 2014). By using low and high exploratory mice differing in their OF exploratory behavior (Kazlauckas, 2005), Kazlauckas et al. (2011) demonstrated that low exploratory mice show less retention in the inhibitory avoidance and lower BDNF levels in the hippocampus (unfortunately the only brain area taken into account).

Nevertheless, it must be taken into account that BDNF is associated not only with stable temperamental predispositions but also with the ongoing behavioral performance. Namely, exercise acutely increases BDNF levels (Adlard et al., 2005; Ferris et al., 2007) and improves BDNF transcription (Oliff et al., 1998), thereby improving cognitive function (Berchtold et al., 2005, 2010). In parallel, single intraventricular injections of BDNF elicit anticataleptic effects (Naumenko et al., 2012; Tikhonova and Kulikov, 2012; Kulikov et al., 2014), and BDNF administration attenuates behavioral responses to stress (Schmidt and Duman, 2010; Ye et al., 2011).

Returning to the functions of BDNF in the cerebellar system, it has been demonstrated that BDNF transgene improves motor behavior in mutant mice characterized by severe cerebellar ataxia (Meng et al., 2007), and that BDNF is implicated in the cerebellar long-term plasticity induced by the environmental enrichment (Angelucci et al., 2009; Vazquez-Sanroman et al., 2013). It is noteworthy that the offspring of pre-reproductively enriched female rats show early maturation of complex motor abilities and increased cerebellar (and striatal) BDNF levels (Caporali et al., 2014).

Accordingly, after providing the indication that the individual differences in the inherent predisposition to approach were associated with the cerebellar BDNF levels, in the present study it was needed to verify whether the cerebellar BDNF levels were causally related with the acute response of approach, in its double component of exploration and novelty-seeking. BDNF-infused BA and AV animals were more explorative, rapid, and approaching toward novelty and reward than PBS-infused animals.

Overall, these data demonstrate that the approach behavior, in its 2 components of search for novelty and exploration, is a BDNF-mediated cerebellar process. The involvement of cerebellar system in the approach is an intriguing, but not unforeseeable, outcome. Besides motor coordination and learning, cerebellar system has been functionally implied in cognitive, emotional, and motivational processes (Ito, 2006; Zhu et al., 2006; De Smet et al., 2013; Laricchiuta et al., 2015), and more importantly (with regard to the present issue) in neural substrates of temperamental individual differences (Wei et al., 2011; Picerni et al., 2013; Laricchiuta et al., 2014b; Petrosini et al., 2015, 2016). It was asserted that the cerebellum is the site where new and familiar stimuli are compared to detect discordances, and where the novelty-related information is processed more and more efficiently and adaptively (Restuccia et al., 2007; O’Reilly et al., 2008). In accordance with the cerebellar functions of error/novelty detection and internal model formation, Ito (2008, 2013) proposed that the cerebellum may alert the prefrontal cortex about the absence of internal models matching the novel information, maintain the newly generated internal models, and incorporate them into routine schemes of thought. Also, its reciprocal connections with basal ganglia (Hoshi et al., 2005; Centonze et al., 2008; Rossi et al., 2008; Bostan et al., 2010) allow the cerebellum to influence reward-driven behavior and to process information related to motivational valence linked in turn to novelty detection and seeking. Not by chance, cerebellar BDNF infusions in AV and BA mice increased their approaching behavior toward the new palatable food. Crucially, in healthy individuals we found cerebellar volumes associated positively with Novelty Seeking scores and negatively with Harm Avoidance scores of the Temperament and Character Inventory by Cloninger (1986, 1987) (Picerni et al., 2013; Laricchiuta et al., 2014b).

Beyond seeking novelty, the cerebellar system has even been linked to the other component of approach, the explorative behavior that, by requiring close integration between environmental (sensory) information and searching (motor) acts, involves the sensory-motor role classically attributed to cerebellar networks. Several studies reported explorative deficits and spatial difficulties following cerebellar damage (Petrosini et al., 1996; 1998; Noblett and Swain, 2003; Molinari et al., 2004). In particular, hemicerebellectomized rats exhibit reduced exploration in the OF (Mandolesi et al., 2003), and cerebellar mutant mice (Rora(sg), Nna1(pcd-1J), nervous, Lurcher) exhibiting degeneration of cerebellar cortex or dentate nucleus or selective Purkinje cell loss display reduced exploration (Lalonde et al., 1988a, 1988b; Caston et al., 1998; Lalonde and Strazielle, 2003). Even in humans, the link between cerebellar function and exploration has been reported (Pierce and Courchesne, 2001; Kawa and Pisula, 2010). High scores in the exploratory excitability subscale of the novelty seeking scale were related with micro-structural variations in cerebellar lobules IV, V, and VI (Picerni et al., 2013). However, despite the interest of these human findings, it is still to be clarified whether specific functions associated with individual differences determine the cerebellar regional structure or conversely, the different structure determines the specific functions. Through the present experimental approach, we provide evidence for the existence of a link between BDNF-mediated cerebellar processing and behaviors of exploration and novelty seeking. Even if an analysis of the possible BDNF action mechanisms on cerebellar processing is beyond the scope of the present paper, it is important to note that the BDNF has been recognized as a crucial modulator of synaptic plasticity in the adult brain (Alkadhi, 2017). By binding the extracellular domain of tyrosine kinase B receptor, BDNF may enhance the quantal release of glutamate and the functionality of NMDA receptors, acting pre- and postsynaptically, respectively (Lista and Sorrentino, 2010). These mechanisms influence various downstream signaling molecules involved in calcium entry and actin polymerization, improving structural plasticity and brain functionality (Vasuta et al., 2007).

In addition to the various events related to BDNF levels, including movement, physical activity, and exploration of a novel environment (Vaynman et al., 2004; Berchtold et al., 2010; Sleiman et al., 2016), here we propose that the endogenous basal or acutely increased cerebellar BDNF levels promote the responses of exploration and novelty seeking, components that sustain the approach pattern.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary data are available at International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology online.

Statement of Interest

None.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research (Ateneo 2016 to L.P.).

References

Adlard
PA
,
Perreau
VM
,
Cotman
CW
(
2005
)
The exercise-induced expression of BDNF within the hippocampus varies across life-span
.
Neurobiol Aging
26
:
511
520
.

Alkadhi
KA
(
2017
)
Exercise as a positive modulator of brain function
.
Mol Neurobiol
doi:
101007/s12035-017-0516-4
.

Angelucci
F
,
De Bartolo
P
,
Gelfo
F
,
Foti
F
,
Cutuli
D
,
Bossù
P
,
Caltagirone
C
,
Petrosini
L
(
2009
)
Increased concentrations of nerve growth factor and brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the rat cerebellum after exposure to environmental enrichment
.
Cerebellum
8
:
499
506
.

Aso
E
,
Ozaita
A
,
Valdizán
EM
,
Ledent
C
,
Pazos
A
,
Maldonado
R
,
Valverde
O
(
2008
)
BDNF impairment in the hippocampus is related to enhanced despair behavior in CB1 knockout mice
.
J Neurochem
105
:
565
572
.

Bassareo
V
,
De Luca
MA
,
Di Chiara
G
(
2002
)
Differential expression of motivational stimulus properties by dopamine in nucleus accumbens shell versus core and prefrontal cortex
.
J Neurosci
22
:
4709
4719
.

Berchtold
NC
,
Chinn
G
,
Chou
M
,
Kesslak
JP
,
Cotman
CW
(
2005
)
Exercise primes a molecular memory for brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein induction in the rat hippocampus
.
Neuroscience
133
:
853
861
.

Berchtold
NC
,
Castello
N
,
Cotman
CW
(
2010
)
Exercise and time-dependent benefits to learning and memory
.
Neuroscience
167
:
588
597
.

Berridge
KC
(
2000
)
Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
24
:
173
198
.

Berton
O
,
McClung
CA
,
Dileone
RJ
,
Krishnan
V
,
Renthal
W
,
Russo
SJ
,
Graham
D
,
Tsankova
NM
,
Bolanos
CA
,
Rios
M
,
Monteggia
LM
,
Self
DW
,
Nestler
EJ
(
2006
)
Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress
.
Science
311
:
864
868
.

Bostan
AC
,
Dum
RP
,
Strick
PL
(
2010
)
The basal ganglia communicate with the cerebellum
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
107
:
8452
8456
.

Bryan
A
,
Hutchison
KE
,
Seals
DR
,
Allen
DL
(
2007
)
A transdisciplinary model integrating genetic, physiological, and psychological correlates of voluntary exercise
.
Health Psychol
26
:
30
39
.

Bryan
AD
,
Magnan
RE
,
Hooper
AE
,
Ciccolo
JT
,
Marcus
B
,
Hutchison
KE
(
2013
)
Colorado stride (COSTRIDE): testing genetic and physiological moderators of response to an intervention to increase physical activity
.
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
10
:
139
.

Butovsky
E
,
Juknat
A
,
Goncharov
I
,
Elbaz
J
,
Eilam
R
,
Zangen
A
,
Vogel
Z
(
2005
)
In vivo up-regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in specific brain areas by chronic exposure to delta-tetrahydrocannabinol
.
J Neurochem
93
:
802
811
.

Caldwell Hooper
AE
,
Bryan
AD
,
Hagger
MS
(
2014
)
What keeps a body moving? The brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and intrinsic motivation to exercise in humans
.
J Behav Med
37
:
1180
1192
.

Caporali
P
,
Cutuli
D
,
Gelfo
F
,
Laricchiuta
D
,
Foti
F
,
De Bartolo
P
,
Mancini
L
,
Angelucci
F
,
Petrosini
L
(
2014
)
Pre-reproductive maternal enrichment influences offspring developmental trajectories: motor behavior and neurotrophin expression
.
Front Behav Neurosci
8
:
195
.

Carver
CS
,
Miller
CJ
(
2006
)
Relations of serotonin function to personality: current views and a key methodological issue
.
Psychiatry Res
144
:
1
15
.

Caston
J
,
Chianale
C
,
Delhaye-Bouchaud
N
,
Mariani
J
(
1998
)
Role of the cerebellum in exploration behavior
.
Brain Res
808
:
232
237
.

Centonze
D
,
Rossi
S
,
De Bartolo
P
,
De Chiara
V
,
Foti
F
,
Musella
A
,
Mataluni
G
,
Rossi
S
,
Bernardi
G
,
Koch
G
,
Petrosini
L
(
2008
)
Adaptations of glutamatergic synapses in the striatum contribute to recovery from cerebellar damage
.
Eur J Neurosci
27
:
2188
2196
.

Challis
C
,
Boulden
J
,
Veerakumar
A
,
Espallergues
J
,
Vassoler
FM
,
Pierce
RC
,
Beck
SG
,
Berton
O
(
2013
)
Raphe GABAergic neurons mediate the acquisition of avoidance after social defeat
.
J Neurosci
33
:
13978
88, 13988a
.

Cloninger
CR
(
1986
)
A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states
.
Psychiatr Dev
4
:
167
226
.

Cloninger
CR
(
1987
)
A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. A proposal
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
44
:
573
588
.

Cutuli
D
,
Caporali
P
,
Gelfo
F
,
Angelucci
F
,
Laricchiuta
D
,
Foti
F
,
De Bartolo
P
,
Bisicchia
E
,
Molinari
M
,
Farioli Vecchioli
S
,
Petrosini
L
(
2015
)
Pre-reproductive maternal enrichment influences rat maternal care and offspring developmental trajectories: behavioral performances and neuroplasticity correlates
.
Front Behav Neurosci
9
:
66
.

D’Angelo
E
,
Casali
S
(
2012
)
Seeking a unified framework for cerebellar function and dysfunction: from circuit operations to cognition
.
Front Neural Circuits
6
:
116
.

De Chiara
V
,
Angelucci
F
,
Rossi
S
,
Musella
A
,
Cavasinni
F
,
Cantarella
C
,
Mataluni
G
,
Sacchetti
L
,
Napolitano
F
,
Castelli
M
,
Caltagirone
C
,
Bernardi
G
,
Maccarrone
M
,
Usiello
A
,
Centonze
D
(
2010
)
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor controls cannabinoid CB1 receptor function in the striatum
.
J Neurosci
30
:
8127
8137
.

De Smet
HJ
,
Paquier
P
,
Verhoeven
J
,
Mariën
P
(
2013
)
The cerebellum: its role in language and related cognitive and affective functions
.
Brain Lang
127
:
334
342
.

D’Souza
DC
,
Pittman
B
,
Perry
E
,
Simen
A
(
2009
)
Preliminary evidence of cannabinoid effects on brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels in humans
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
202
:
569
578
.

Duclot
F
,
Kabbaj
M
(
2013
)
Individual differences in novelty seeking predict subsequent vulnerability to social defeat through a differential epigenetic regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression
.
J Neurosci
33
:
11048
11060
.

Elliot
AJ
(
2005
)
A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct
. In:
Handbook of competence and motivation
(
Elliot
A
,
Dweck
C
, eds.), pp
52
72
.
New York: The Guilford Press
.

Elliot
AJ
(
2008
)
Approach and avoidance motivation
. In:
Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation
(
Elliot
A
, ed), pp
3
14
.
New York
:
Taylor and Francis Group Psychology Press
.

Ferris
LT
,
Williams
JS
,
Shen
CL
(
2007
)
The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function
.
Med Sci Sports Exerc
39
:
728
734
.

Franklin
K
,
Paxinos
G
(
1997
)
The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates
.
San Diego
:
Academic Press
.

Gatt
JM
,
Nemeroff
CB
,
Dobson-Stone
C
,
Paul
RH
,
Bryant
RA
,
Schofield
PR
,
Gordon
E
,
Kemp
AH
,
Williams
LM
(
2009
)
Interactions between BDNF val66met polymorphism and early life stress predict brain and arousal pathways to syndromal depression and anxiety
.
Mol Psychiatry
14
:
681
695
.

Glowinski
J
,
Iversen
LL
(
1966
)
Regional studies of catecholamines in the rat brain. I. The disposition of [3H]norepinephrine, [3H]dopamine and [3H]dopa in various regions of the brain
.
J Neurochem
13
:
655
669
.

Greenberg
R
(
2003
)
The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds
. In:
Animal innovation
(
Reader
SM
,
Laland
KN
, eds.), pp
175
196
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.

Groppe
SE
,
Gossen
A
,
Rademacher
L
,
Hahn
A
,
Westphal
L
,
Gründer
G
,
Spreckelmeyer
KN
(
2013
)
Oxytocin influences processing of socially relevant cues in the ventral tegmental area of the human brain
.
Biol Psychiatry
74
:
172
179
.

Helfinstein
SM
,
Benson
B
,
Perez-Edgar
K
,
Bar-Haim
Y
,
Detloff
A
,
Pine
DS
,
Fox
NA
,
Ernst
M
(
2011
)
Striatal responses to negative monetary outcomes differ between temperamentally inhibited and non-inhibited adolescents
.
Neuropsychologia
49
:
479
485
.

Hoshi
E
,
Tremblay
L
,
Féger
J
,
Carras
PL
,
Strick
PL
(
2005
)
The cerebellum communicates with the basal ganglia
.
Nat Neurosci
8
:
1491
1493
.

Huang
Y
,
Yasuda
H
,
Sarihi
A
,
Tsumoto
T
(
2008
)
Roles of endocannabinoids in heterosynaptic long-term depression of excitatory synaptic transmission in visual cortex of young mice
.
J Neurosci
28
:
7074
7083
.

Ito
M
(
2006
)
Cerebellar circuitry as a neuronal machine
.
Prog Neurobiol
78
:
272
303
.

Ito
M
(
2008
)
Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
9
:
304
313
.

Ito
M
(
2013
)
Error detection and representation in the olivo-cerebellar system
.
Front Neural Circuits
7
:
1
.

Kaminitz
A
,
Barzilay
R
,
Segal
H
,
Taler
M
,
Offen
D
,
Gil-Ad
I
,
Mechoulam
R
,
Weizman
A
(
2014
)
Dominant negative DISC1 mutant mice display specific social behaviour deficits and aberration in BDNF and cannabinoid receptor expression
.
World J Biol Psychiatry
15
:
76
82
.

Kaplan
DR
,
Miller
FD
(
2007
)
Developing with BDNF: a moving experience
.
Neuron
55
:
1
2
.

Kawa
R
,
Pisula
E
(
2010
)
Locomotor activity, object exploration and space preference in children with autism and down syndrome
.
Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars)
70
:
131
140
.

Kazlauckas
V
,
Schuh
J
,
Dall’Igna
OP
,
Pereira
GS
,
Bonan
CD
,
Lara
DR
(
2005
)
Behavioral and cognitive profile of mice with high and low exploratory phenotypes
.
Behav Brain Res
162
:
272
278
.

Kazlauckas
V
,
Pagnussat
N
,
Mioranzza
S
,
Kalinine
E
,
Nunes
F
,
Pettenuzzo
L
,
Souza
DO
,
Portela
LV
,
Porciúncula
LO
,
Lara
DR
(
2011
)
Enriched environment effects on behavior, memory and BDNF in low and high exploratory mice
.
Physiol Behav
102
:
475
480
.

Kelly
RM
,
Strick
PL
(
2003
)
Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate
.
J Neurosci
23
:
8432
8444
.

Khaspekov
LG
,
Brenz Verca
MS
,
Frumkina
LE
,
Hermann
H
,
Marsicano
G
,
Lutz
B
(
2004
)
Involvement of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in cannabinoid receptor-dependent protection against excitotoxicity
.
Eur J Neurosci
19
:
1691
1698
.

Kulikov
AV
,
Fursenko
DV
,
Khotskin
NV
,
Bazovkina
DV
,
Kulikov
VA
,
Naumenko
VS
,
Bazhenova
EY
,
Popova
NK
(
2014
)
Spatial learning in the morris water maze in mice genetically different in the predisposition to catalepsy: the effect of intraventricular treatment with brain-derived neurotrophic factor
.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav
122
:
266
272
.

Lalonde
R
,
Lamarre
Y
,
Smith
AM
(
1988
)
Does the mutant mouse lurcher have deficits in spatially oriented behaviours
?
Brain Res
455
:
24
30
.

Lalonde
R
,
Manseau
M
,
Botez
MI
(
1988
)
Spontaneous alternation and exploration in staggerer mutant mice
.
Behav Brain Res
27
:
273
276
.

Lalonde
R
,
Strazielle
C
(
2003
)
Motor coordination, exploration, and spatial learning in a natural mouse mutation (nervous) with Purkinje cell degeneration
.
Behav Genet
33
:
59
66
.

Laricchiuta
D
(
2015
)
Editorial: individual differences: from neurobiological bases to new insight on approach and avoidance behavior
.
Front Syst Neurosci
9
:
125
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Musella
A
,
Rossi
S
,
Centonze
D
(
2014
)
Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of endocannabinoid and dopaminergic systems on salient stimuli
.
Front Behav Neurosci
8
:
183
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Petrosini
L
(
2014
)
Individual differences in response to positive and negative stimuli: endocannabinoid-based insight on approach and avoidance behaviors
.
Front Syst Neurosci
8
:
238
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Petrosini
L
,
Picerni
E
,
Cutuli
D
,
Iorio
M
,
Chiapponi
C
,
Caltagirone
C
,
Piras
F
,
Spalletta
G
(
2015
)
The embodied emotion in cerebellum: a neuroimaging study of alexithymia
.
Brain Struct Funct
220
:
2275
2287
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Petrosini
L
,
Piras
F
,
Cutuli
D
,
Macci
E
,
Picerni
E
,
Chiapponi
C
,
Caltagirone
C
,
Spalletta
G
(
2014
)
Linking novelty seeking and harm avoidance personality traits to basal ganglia: volumetry and mean diffusivity
.
Brain Struct Funct
219
:
793
803
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Petrosini
L
,
Piras
F
,
Macci
E
,
Cutuli
D
,
Chiapponi
C
,
Cerasa
A
,
Picerni
E
,
Caltagirone
C
,
Girardi
P
,
Tamorri
SM
,
Spalletta
G
(
2014
)
Linking novelty seeking and harm avoidance personality traits to cerebellar volumes
.
Hum Brain Mapp
35
:
285
296
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Rojo
ML
,
Rodriguez-Gaztelumendi
A
,
Ferlazzo
F
,
Petrosini
L
,
Fowler
CJ
(
2012
)
CB1 receptor autoradiographic characterization of the individual differences in approach and avoidance motivation
.
Plos One
7
:
e42111
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Rossi
S
,
Musella
A
,
De Chiara
V
,
Cutuli
D
,
Centonze
D
,
Petrosini
L
(
2012
)
Differences in spontaneously avoiding or approaching mice reflect differences in CB1-mediated signaling of dorsal striatal transmission
.
Plos One
7
:
e33260
.

Laricchiuta
D
,
Saba
L
,
De Bartolo
P
,
Caioli
S
,
Zona
C
,
Petrosini
L
(
2016
)
Maintenance of aversive memories shown by fear extinction-impaired phenotypes is associated with increased activity in the amygdaloid-prefrontal circuit
.
Sci Rep
6
:
21205
.

Linfoot
I
,
Gray
M
,
Bingham
B
,
Williamson
M
,
Pinel
JP
,
Viau
V
(
2009
)
Naturally occurring variations in defensive burying behavior are associated with differences in vasopressin, oxytocin, and androgen receptors in the male rat
.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry
33
:
1129
1140
.

Lista
I
,
Sorrentino
G
(
2010
)
Biological mechanisms of physical activity in preventing cognitive decline
.
Cell Mol Neurobiol
30
:
493
503
.

Maison
P
,
Walker
DJ
,
Walsh
FS
,
Williams
G
,
Doherty
P
(
2009
)
BDNF regulates neuronal sensitivity to endocannabinoids
.
Neurosci Lett
467
:
90
94
.

Mandolesi
L
,
Leggio
MG
,
Spirito
F
,
Petrosini
L
(
2003
)
Cerebellar contribution to spatial event processing: do spatial procedures contribute to formation of spatial declarative knowledge
?
Eur J Neurosci
18
:
2618
2626
.

Martire
SI
,
Maniam
J
,
South
T
,
Holmes
N
,
Westbrook
RF
,
Morris
MJ
(
2014
)
Extended exposure to a palatable cafeteria diet alters gene expression in brain regions implicated in reward, and withdrawal from this diet alters gene expression in brain regions associated with stress
.
Behav Brain Res
265
:
132
141
.

McNaughton
N
,
Gray
JA
(
2000
)
Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety
.
J Affect Disord
61
:
161
176
.

Meng
H
,
Larson
SK
,
Gao
R
,
Qiao
X
(
2007
)
BDNF transgene improves ataxic and motor behaviors in stargazer mice
.
Brain Res
1160
:
47
57
.

Middleton
FA
,
Strick
PL
(
1997
)
Cerebellar output channels
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
41
:
61
82
.

Middleton
FA
,
Strick
PL
(
2000
)
Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits
.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev
31
:
236
250
.

Middleton
FA
,
Strick
PL
(
2001
)
Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate
.
J Neurosci
21
:
700
712
.

Minelli
A
,
Zanardini
R
,
Bonvicini
C
,
Sartori
R
,
Pedrini
L
,
Gennarelli
M
,
Bocchio-Chiavetto
L
(
2011
)
BDNF serum levels, but not BDNF val66met genotype, are correlated with personality traits in healthy subjects
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
261
:
323
329
.

Mogi
K
,
Ooyama
R
,
Nagasawa
M
,
Kikusui
T
(
2014
)
Effects of neonatal oxytocin manipulation on development of social behaviors in mice
.
Physiol Behav
133
:
68
75
.

Molinari
M
,
Petrosini
L
,
Misciagna
S
,
Leggio
MG
(
2004
)
Visuospatial abilities in cerebellar disorders
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
75
:
235
240
.

Nasser
HM
,
McNally
GP
(
2012
)
Appetitive-aversive interactions in Pavlovian fear conditioning
.
Behav Neurosci
126
:
404
422
.

Naumenko
VS
,
Kondaurova
EM
,
Bazovkina
DV
,
Tsybko
AS
,
Tikhonova
MA
,
Kulikov
AV
,
Popova
NK
(
2012
)
Effect of brain-derived neurotrophic factor on behavior and key members of the brain serotonin system in genetically predisposed to behavioral disorders mouse strains
.
Neuroscience
214
:
59
67
.

Nelson
EE
,
Panksepp
J
(
1998
)
Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
22
:
437
452
.

Noblett
KL
,
Swain
RA
(
2003
)
Pretraining enhances recovery from visuospatial deficit following cerebellar dentate nucleus lesion
.
Behav Neurosci
117
:
785
798
.

Okuno
K
,
Yoshimura
R
,
Ueda
N
,
Ikenouchi-Sugita
A
,
Umene-Nakano
W
,
Hori
H
,
Hayashi
K
,
Katsuki
A
,
Chen
HI
,
Nakamura
J
(
2011
)
Relationships between stress, social adaptation, personality traits, brain-derived neurotrophic factor and 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol plasma concentrations in employees at a publishing company in japan
.
Psychiatry Res
186
:
326
332
.

Oliff
HS
,
Berchtold
NC
,
Isackson
P
,
Cotman
CW
(
1998
)
Exercise-induced regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) transcripts in the rat hippocampus
.
Brain Res Mol Brain Res
61
:
147
153
.

O’Reilly
JX
,
Mesulam
MM
,
Nobre
AC
(
2008
)
The cerebellum predicts the timing of perceptual events
.
J Neurosci
28
:
2252
2260
.

Petrosini
L
,
Molinari
M
,
Dell’Anna
ME
(
1996
)
Cerebellar contribution to spatial event processing: morris water maze and T-maze
.
Eur J Neurosci
8
:
1882
1896
.

Petrosini
L
,
Leggio
MG
,
Molinari
M
(
1998
)
The cerebellum in the spatial problem solving: a co-star or a guest star
?
Prog Neurobiol
56
:
191
210
.

Petrosini
L
,
Cutuli
D
,
Picerni
E
,
Laricchiuta
D
(
2015
)
Cerebellum and personality traits
.
Cerebellum
14
:
43
46
.

Petrosini
L
,
Cutuli
D
,
Picerni
E
,
Laricchiuta
D
(
2017
)
Viewing the personality traits through a cerebellar lens: a focus on the constructs of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, and alexithymia
.
Cerebellum
16
:
178
190
.

Picerni
E
,
Petrosini
L
,
Piras
F
,
Laricchiuta
D
,
Cutuli
D
,
Chiapponi
C
,
Fagioli
S
,
Girardi
P
,
Caltagirone
C
,
Spalletta
G
(
2013
)
New evidence for the cerebellar involvement in personality traits
.
Front Behav Neurosci
7
:
133
.

Pickering
AD
,
Gray
JA
(
2001
)
Dopamine, appetitive reinforcement and the neuropsychology of human learning: an individual differences approach
. In:
Advances in individual differences research
(
Angleitner
A
, ed), pp
113
149
.
Lengerich, Germany
:
PABST Science Publishers
.

Pierce
K
,
Courchesne
E
(
2001
)
Evidence for a cerebellar role in reduced exploration and stereotyped behavior in autism
.
Biol Psychiatry
49
:
655
664
.

Restuccia
D
,
Della Marca
G
,
Valeriani
M
,
Leggio
MG
,
Molinari
M
(
2007
)
Cerebellar damage impairs detection of somatosensory input changes. A somatosensory mismatch-negativity study
.
Brain
130
:
276
287
.

Rossi
S
,
Mataluni
G
,
De Bartolo
P
,
Prosperetti
C
,
Foti
F
,
De Chiara
V
,
Musella
A
,
Mandolesi
L
,
Bernardi
G
,
Centonze
D
,
Petrosini
L
(
2008
)
Cerebellar control of cortico-striatal LTD
.
Restor Neurol Neurosci
26
:
475
480
.

Saylor
AJ
,
McGinty
JF
(
2010
)
An intrastriatal brain-derived neurotrophic factor infusion restores striatal gene expression in bdnf heterozygous mice
.
Brain Struct Funct
215
:
97
104
.

Schmidt
HD
,
Duman
RS
(
2010
)
Peripheral BDNF produces antidepressant-like effects in cellular and behavioral models
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
35
:
2378
2391
.

Schutter
DJ
,
Hofman
D
,
Hoppenbrouwers
SS
,
Kenemans
JL
(
2011
)
Corticospinal state variability and hemispheric asymmetries in motivational tendencies
.
Biol Psychol
87
:
450
452
.

Sleiman
SF
,
Henry
J
,
Al-Haddad
R
,
El Hayek
L
,
Abou Haidar
E
,
Stringer
T
,
Ulja
D
,
Karuppagounder
SS
,
Holson
EB
,
Ratan
RR
,
Ninan
I
,
Chao
MV
(
2016
)
Exercise promotes the expression of brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) through the action of the ketone body β-hydroxybutyrate
.
Elife
5pii:
e15092
.

Sullivan
RM
,
Toufexis
DJ
,
Wilson
DA
(
2008
)
Development of olfactory modulated approach and avoidance motivated behaviors
. In:
Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation
(
Elliot
A
, ed), pp
127
147
.
New York
:
Taylor and Francis Group Psychology Press
.

Terracciano
A
,
Martin
B
,
Ansari
D
,
Tanaka
T
,
Ferrucci
L
,
Maudsley
S
,
Mattson
MP
,
Costa
PT
Jr
(
2010
)
Plasma BDNF concentration, val66met genetic variant and depression-related personality traits
.
Genes Brain Behav
9
:
512
518
.

Tikhonova
M
,
Kulikov
AV
(
2012
)
Antidepressant-like effects of central BDNF administration in mice of antidepressant sensitive catalepsy (ASC) strain
.
Chin J Physiol
55
:
284
293
.

Vasuta
C
,
Caunt
C
,
James
R
,
Samadi
S
,
Schibuk
E
,
Kannangara
T
,
Titterness
AK
,
Christie
BR
(
2007
)
Effects of exercise on NMDA receptor subunit contributions to bidirectional synaptic plasticity in the mouse dentate gyrus
.
Hippocampus
17
:
1201
1208
.

Vazquez-Sanroman
D
,
Sanchis-Segura
C
,
Toledo
R
,
Hernandez
ME
,
Manzo
J
,
Miquel
M
(
2013
)
The effects of enriched environment on BDNF expression in the mouse cerebellum depending on the length of exposure
.
Behav Brain Res
243
:
118
128
.

Vaynman
S
,
Ying
Z
,
Gomez-Pinilla
F
(
2004
)
Hippocampal BDNF mediates the efficacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition
.
Eur J Neurosci
20
:
2580
2590
.

Verhagen
M
,
van der Meij
A
,
van Deurzen
PA
,
Janzing
JG
,
Arias-Vásquez
A
,
Buitelaar
JK
,
Franke
B
(
2010
)
Meta-analysis of the BDNF val66met polymorphism in major depressive disorder: effects of gender and ethnicity
.
Mol Psychiatry
15
:
260
271
.

Wei
L
,
Duan
X
,
Yang
Y
,
Liao
W
,
Gao
Q
,
Ding
JR
,
Zhang
Z
,
Zeng
W
,
Li
Y
,
Lu
G
,
Chen
H
(
2011
)
The synchronization of spontaneous BOLD activity predicts extraversion and neuroticism
.
Brain Res
1419
:
68
75
.

Wei
SM
,
Eisenberg
DP
,
Nabel
KG
,
Kohn
PD
,
Kippenhan
JS
,
Dickinson
D
,
Kolachana
B
,
Berman
KF
(
2017
)
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects the relationship between an anxiety-related personality trait and resting regional cerebral blood flow
.
Cereb Cortex
27
:
2175
2182
.

Yasui-Furukori
N
,
Tsuchimine
S
,
Kaneda
A
,
Sugawara
N
,
Ishioka
M
,
Kaneko
S
(
2013
)
Association between plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and personality traits in healthy Japanese subjects
.
Psychiatry Res
210
:
220
223
.

Ye
Y
,
Wang
G
,
Wang
H
,
Wang
X
(
2011
)
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) infusion restored astrocytic plasticity in the hippocampus of a rat model of depression
.
Neurosci Lett
503
:
15
19
.

Zhu
L
,
Scelfo
B
,
Tempia
F
,
Sacchetti
B
,
Strata
P
(
2006
)
Membrane excitability and fear conditioning in cerebellar purkinje cell
.
Neuroscience
140
:
801
810
.

Zoratto
F
,
Fiore
M
,
Ali
SF
,
Laviola
G
,
Macrì
S
(
2013
)
Neonatal tryptophan depletion and corticosterone supplementation modify emotional responses in adult male mice
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
38
:
24
39
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]

Supplementary data