Abstract

A growing body of evidence indicates that facilitation of serotonin-2C receptor (5-HT2CR)-mediated neurotransmission in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) is involved in anxiety generation. We investigated here whether BLA 5-HT2CRs exert a differential role in the regulation of defensive behaviours related to generalized anxiety (inhibitory avoidance) and panic (escape) disorders. We also evaluated whether activation of BLA 5-HT2CRs accounts for the anxiogenic effect caused by acute systemic administration of the antidepressants imipramine and fluoxetine. Male Wistar rats were tested in the elevated T-maze after intra-BLA injection of the endogenous agonist 5-HT, the 5-HT2CR agonist MK-212 or the 5-HT2CR antagonist SB-242084. This test allows the measurement of inhibitory avoidance acquisition and escape expression. We also investigated whether intra-BLA administration of SB-242084 interferes with the acute anxiogenic effect caused by imipramine and fluoxetine in the Vogel conflict test, and imipramine in the elevated T-maze. While intra-BLA administration of 5-HT and MK-212 facilitated inhibitory avoidance acquisition, suggesting an anxiogenic effect, SB-242084 had the opposite effect. None of these drugs affected escape performance. Intra-BLA injection of a sub-effective dose of SB-242084 fully blocked the anxiogenic effect caused either by the local microinjection of 5-HT or the systemic administration of imipramine and fluoxetine. Our findings indicate that 5-HT2CRs in BLA are selectively involved in the regulation of defensive behaviours associated with generalized anxiety, but not panic. The results also provide the first direct evidence that activation of BLA 5-HT2CRs accounts for the short-term aversive effect of antidepressants.

Introduction

The serotonin-2C receptor (5-HT2CR) plays an important role in emotional processing (Berg et al.2008; Giorgetti & Tecott, 2004; Landolt & Wehrle, 2009; Millan, 2005). This receptor is a member of the family of G-protein-coupled receptors and was first identified in the choroid plexus (Bockaert et al.2010; Hoyer et al.2002; Pazos et al.1984). High levels of 5-HT2CR-binding sites and 5-HT2CR mRNA are also observed in the cortex and limbic areas such as the hippocampus, amygdala and periaqueductal grey (Clemett et al.2000; Mengod et al.2010; Pompeiano et al.1994).

Compelling evidence from clinical and animal studies shows that facilitation of 5-HT2CR-mediated neurotransmission increases anxiety. This has been reported after the peripheral administration of both non-selective 5-HT2CR agonists such as m-CPP and TMFPP as well as of more selective, recently developed agonists such as MK-212 and RO 60-0175 (Bagdy et al.2001; Broocks et al.1997; Jan et al.2010; Kennett et al.1989; Overstreet et al.2003; Setem et al.1999; Walker et al.2005). On the other hand, treatment with 5-HT2CR antagonists such as ritanserin (non-selective) or SB-242084 (selective) causes the opposite effect (Bressa et al.1987; Ceulemans et al.1985; Kennett et al.1997; Martin et al.2002). Clinical studies indicate that agomelatine, a melatonin receptor agonist and 5-HT2CR antagonist that has recently been marked as an antidepressant, also has anxiolytic properties (de Bodinat et al.2010; Stein et al.2008). In animal models, the anxiolytic effect of agomelatine has been mainly attributed to its interaction with 5-HT2CRs (Millan et al.2005).

The involvement of 5-HT2CRs in the genesis/regulation of anxiety has also been revealed by genetic approaches. It has been reported that whereas transgenic mice overexpressing 5-HT2CRs in the cerebral cortex and different limbic areas (e.g. amygdala, hippocampus) show increased anxiety in the elevated plus-maze (Kimura et al.2009), 5-HT2CR knockout mice display an anxiolytic-like phenotype as measured by different experimental models (Heisler et al.2007).

Among the brain areas showing abundant distribution of 5-HT2CRs, the amygdala has been seen as the critical neuronal substrate for this receptor-mediated effect on anxiety. For instance, 5-HT2CR mRNA is overexpressed in this area following a single session of prolonged exposure to stressors, a procedure shown to increase anxiety (Harada et al.2008). Higher levels of anxiety are also observed in rats after exposure to unavoidable tail-shocks, systemic injection of MK-212 or repeated withdrawal from chronic ethanol diets, and in all cases this effect is fully blocked by the injection of 5-HT2CR antagonists such as SB-242084 into the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) (Christianson et al.2010; de Mello Cruz et al.2005; Overstreet et al.2006). The BLA has long been associated with anxiety processing (Pesold & Treit, 1995, Rosen, 2004) and receives dense 5-HT innervation from the dorsal raphe nucleus (Vertes, 1991).

It has also been proposed (Burghardt et al.2007; Millan, 2005) that activation of BLA 5-HT2CRs accounts for the anxiogenic effect found clinically during the early stages of treatment with antidepressants such as SSRIs (e.g. fluoxetine, sertraline) or tricyclic compounds (e.g. imipramine, clomipramine). While this hypothesis has been put forward based on evidence showing that systemic injection of 5-HT2CR antagonists fully counteracts the acute anxiogenesis caused by antidepressants in different animal models of anxiety (Bagdy et al.2001; Greenwood et al.2008; Yamauchi et al.2004), no study has directly investigated this proposal by showing that intra-BLA injection of 5-HT2CR antagonists exerts the same inhibitory effect.

In the present study, we further addressed the role played by BLA 5-HT2CRs in anxiety. More specifically, we first investigated whether this receptor exerts a differential role in the regulation of defensive behaviours that have been related to different subtypes of clinically recognized anxiety. To this end, the effect caused by intra-BLA microinjection of the endogenous agonist 5-HT, the selective 5-HT2CR agonist MK-212 or the 5-HT2CR antagonist SB-242084 was investigated in the elevated T-maze. This animal model, derived from the elevated plus-maze, allows measurement, in the same rat, of an anxiety- and a fear-related defensive response, respectively, inhibitory avoidance and escape (for a review of this test, see McNaughton & Zangrossi, 2008; Pinheiro et al.2007; Zangrossi & Graeff, 1997). Based on a series of pharmacological studies (for a review see Blanchard et al.2001; Graeff & Zangrossi, 2002; McNaughton & Gray, 2000), behavioural inhibition responses such as inhibitory avoidance have been linked with anxiety while escape has been associated with fear. In terms of psychopathology, these responses have been related to generalized anxiety and panic disorders, respectively (Blanchard et al.2001, 2003; Graeff & Zangrossi, 2002; Gray & McNaughton, 2000; McNaughton & Corr, 2004).

In a previous study from our group, we reported that whereas a single systemic injection of imipramine facilitated inhibitory avoidance acquisition in the elevated T-maze, indicating an anxiogenic effect, a 21-d treatment caused the opposite effect. Chronic, but not acute, treatment with imipramine inhibited escape expression, suggesting a panicolytic-like effect (Teixeira et al.2000). These results are in full agreement with clinical evidence showing that the beneficial effects of antidepressants on generalized anxiety and panic disorders are only seen after prolonged administration (Bakker et al.2005; Millan, 2005).

In a second experiment performed, we investigated whether activation of 5-HT2CRs in the BLA accounts for the acute anxiogenic effect caused by imipramine in the elevated T-maze inhibitory avoidance task. For comparison, we verified whether these receptors were also implicated in the acute imipramine and fluoxetine anxiogenic effect detected in another generalized anxiety-associated model, the Vogel conflict test (for further details of this test see Graeff & Zangrossi, 2002; Millan & Brocco, 2003). Imipramine and fluoxetine were chosen as being the prototype drugs for the tricyclic antidepressants and SSRIs, respectively. These two classes of drugs are shown to be equally effective in treating depression and anxiety disorders such as generalized anxiety and panic, even though the use of the latter has supplanted the former due to its better safety, tolerability and cost (Dupuy et al.2011; Ravindran & Stein, 2010).

Methods

Animals

Male Wistar rats weighing 290–310 g were group-housed (n=5 per group) under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on 07:00 hours) at 22±1°C with free access to food throughout the experiment, except during testing. Water was also freely available, except in expts 2b and 2c where periods of deprivation were followed (see below). The experiments reported in this paper were performed in compliance with the recommendations of the SBNeC (Brazilian Society of Neuroscience and Behaviour), which are based on the US National Institutes of Health Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Apparatus

The elevated T-maze was made of wood and had three arms of equal dimensions (50×12 cm). One arm, enclosed by 40-cm-high walls, was perpendicular to two opposed open arms. To avoid falls, the open arms were surrounded by a 1 cm high Plexiglas rim. The whole apparatus was elevated 50 cm above the floor.

The open-field test was performed in a wooden square arena (60×60 cm), with 30-cm-high walls. Luminosity at the level of the T-maze arms or open field was 60 lx.

The Vogel conflict test was performed as described by Pelosi et al. (2009) in a Plexiglas box (length 42 cm, width 25 cm, height 20 cm) with a stainless-steel grid floor. A metallic spout of a drinking bottle containing water projected into the box. The contact of the animal with the spout and grid floor closed an electrical circuit controlled by a sensor (Insight Instruments, Brazil), which produced 7 pulses/s whenever the animal was in contact with both components. Each pulse was considered as a lick, and every 20 licks the animal received a 0.5 mA shock for 2 s. The sensor recorded the total number of licks and shocks delivered during the test period. The apparatus was located inside a sound-attenuated cage.

After each experimental session, the models were cleaned with a 10% ethanol solution.

Drugs

The following drugs were used: 5-hydroxytryptamine creatinine sulphate (5-HT; Sigma, USA), 6-chloro-2-(1-piperazinyl)pyrazine hydrochloride (MK-212; Tocris, USA), 6-chloro-2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-N-[6-[(2-methyl-3-pyridinyl)oxy]-3-pyridinyl]-1H-indole-1-carboxyamide dihydrochloride (SB-242084; Tocris), imipramine hydrochloride (Sigma) and fluoxetine hydrochloride (EMS, Brazil). All drugs were dissolved in sterile saline, except fluoxetine, which was dissolved in a solution containing sterile saline with 2% Tween-80.

Surgery

The animals were anaesthetized with 2,2,2-tribromoethanol (250 mg/kg i.p.) and placed in a stereotaxic frame. In all experiments, two guide cannulae made of stainless steel (0.6 mm outer diameter, 0.4 mm inner diameter) were bilaterally implanted in the brain aimed at the BLA. The following coordinates from bregma were used (Paxinos & Watson, 2007), BLA: AP −2.5 mm, lateral ±5.1 mm, ventral −6.2 mm. The guide cannulae were fixed to the skull with acrylic resin and two stainless-steel screws. Stylets, the same length as the guide cannulae, were introduced inside them to prevent obstruction.

At the end of the surgery, all animals were injected intramuscularly with 0.3 ml of antibiotic preparation (benzylpenicillin and streptomycin, Pentabiotico Veterinário Pequeno Porte, Brazil) to prevent possible infections. In addition, fluxinin meglumine (Schering–Plough, Brazil; 3 mg/kg), a drug with analgesic, antipyretic and anti-inflammatory properties, was administered subcutaneously for post-surgery analgesia. The animals were left undisturbed for 5–7 d after the surgery, except for normal handling during cage cleaning.

Procedure

Elevated T-maze and open-field tests

One day before the test, the rats were exposed to one of the open arms of the T-maze for 30 min. A wooden barrier mounted on the border between the central area of the maze and the proximal end of the open arm isolated this arm from the rest of the maze. It has been shown that this pre-exposure to the open arm renders the escape task more sensitive to the effects of antipanic drugs, because it shortens the latencies of withdrawal from the open arm during the test (Poltronieri et al.2003; Teixeira et al.2000).

On the test day, in expt 1a, independent groups of test-naive rats received an intra-BLA injection of MK-212 (0, 0.01, 0.1 or 1 nmol; n=8–9), 5-HT (0 or 16 nmol; n=10–11) or SB-242084 (0, 0.1, 1, 10 nmol; n=7–9). Ten minutes after the microinjections, the animals were submitted to the behavioural tests.

In order to investigate if SB-242084 was able to block the effects of 5-HT, in expt 1b the animals were microinjected in the BLA with SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) or saline, 5 min before microinjection of 5-HT (16 nmol) or saline. Thus, the following groups (n=6–7) were formed: Sal/Sal, SB/Sal, Sal/5-HT and SB/5-HT. Ten minutes after the last microinjection, the animals were submitted to the behavioural tests.

In expt 2a, the animals were microinjected in the BLA with SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) or saline, 10 min before the systemic injection (i.p.) of imipramine (10 mg/kg, 1 ml/kg) or saline. Thus, the following groups (n=7–10) were formed: Sal/Sal, SB/Sal, Sal/Imi and SB/Imi. The animals were tested in the elevated T-maze and open-field tests 30 min after the systemic injections.

The test in the elevated T-maze was initiated by inhibitory avoidance measurement. Each animal was placed at the distal end of the enclosed arm of the elevated T-maze facing the intersection of the arms. The time taken by the rat to leave this arm with the four paws was recorded (baseline latency). The same measurement was repeated in two subsequent trials (avoidance 1 and 2) at 30-s intervals. Following avoidance training (30 s), rats were placed at the end of the same previously experienced open arm and the latency to leave this arm with the four paws was recorded for three consecutive times (escape 1, 2 and 3) with 30-s inter-trial intervals. A cut-off time of 300 s was established for the avoidance and escape latencies.

In order to assess possible drug effects on locomotor activity, immediately after testing in the elevated T-maze, the total distance travelled by each rat in an open field was evaluated by a video-tracking system (Ethovision, The Netherlands) for 5 min.

Vogel conflict test

Animals were water-deprived for 48 h prior to the test. After the first 24 h of deprivation, they were allowed to drink freely for 3 min in the test cage in order to find the drinking bottle spout. Some animals that failed to find the spout were not included in the experiment. Twenty-four hours later, animals were injected in the BLA with SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) or saline, 10 min before the systemic injection (i.p.) of imipramine (10 mg/kg, 1 ml/kg) or saline (expt 2b, n=8–9) or fluoxetine (15 mg/kg, 1 ml/kg) or vehicle solution (expt 2c, n=8–11). Thirty minutes later, they were returned to the test cage. The test period lasted for 3 min, and the animals received a 0.5-mA shock for 2 s through the bottle spout every 20 licks.

In an independent group of non-operated rats (n=4–6), we evaluated the effect of a single injection (i.p.) of imipramine (10 mg/kg) or fluoxetine (15 mg/kg) on water consumption. The procedure adopted was the same used in the punished licking test described above, except that the electric-shock delivery system was inoperative.

The doses of the drugs were chosen on the basis of previously published studies (Alves et al.2004; Christianson et al.2010; Navailles et al.2006; Teixeira et al.2000; Zanoveli et al.2003).

For drug injection, needles (0.3 mm outer diameter) were introduced through guide cannulae until their tips were 2 mm below the end of the cannulae. The drugs were microinjected in the BLA in a volume of 0.2 µl (for each side) over a period of 120 s using a 10 µl microsyringe (Hamilton 701-RN, USA) attached to a microinfusion pump (KD Scientific, USA). The displacement of an air bubble inside the polyethylene catheters connecting the syringe needles to the intracerebral needles was used to monitor the microinjection. The intracerebral needles were removed 60 s after the end of the injections.

Histology

After the experiments, animals were sacrificed under deep urethane anaesthesia. The brain was perfused intracardially with saline solution (0.9%) followed by 10% formalin solution before being removed and fixed in 10% formalin. Brain slices of 60 µm were made by means of a microtome in order to localize the site of the drug injection, according to the atlas of Paxinos & Watson (2007) .

Statistical analysis

Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyse both avoidance and escape data. In expt 1a, treatment (saline, MK-212, 5-HT, SB-242084) was considered the independent factor and trial (baseline, avoidance 1 and 2 latencies, escape 1, 2 and 3 latencies), the dependent, repeated measure. In expt 1b, besides the dependent measure (trial), two independent factors were considered: pre-treatment with SB-242084 and treatment with 5-HT. In expt 2a, the two independent factors were pre-treatment with SB-242084 and treatment with imipramine. Locomotion data in the open-field were analysed by one-way ANOVA (expt 1a) or two-way ANOVA (expts 1b, 2a). The number of punished licks in the Vogel conflict test was analysed by two-way ANOVA (expts 2b, 2c). Multiple comparisons were performed by Duncan's post-hoc test.

Results

Figure 1 depicts the sites of drug injections into the BLA of animals tested in the study.

Diagrammatic representation of coronal sections of the rat brain showing the location of injection sites in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (•). Figures represent coordinates from the rat brain atlas of Paxinos & Watson (2007), with respect to bregma. The number of points in the figures is fewer than the total number of rats used because of several overlaps.
Fig. 1

Diagrammatic representation of coronal sections of the rat brain showing the location of injection sites in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (•). Figures represent coordinates from the rat brain atlas of Paxinos & Watson (2007), with respect to bregma. The number of points in the figures is fewer than the total number of rats used because of several overlaps.

Expt 1a: MK-212 effects

Figure 2a shows that MK-212 facilitated inhibitory avoidance acquisition (treatment effect: F3,31=3.31, p<0.05), suggesting an anxiogenic effect. Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a trial effect (F2,62=33.95, p<0.05), but not a trial×treatment interaction. The post-hoc test showed that MK-212 (0.01 and 0.1 nmol) significantly increased (p<0.05) avoidance 2 latency compared to the saline group. This agonist did not affect escape performance (Fig. 2b) or locomotion in the open field (Table 1).

MK-212 injection in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala facilitated (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition, without interfering with (b) escape expression in the elevated T-maze (n=8–10). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to the saline-injected group.
Fig. 2

MK-212 injection in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala facilitated (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition, without interfering with (b) escape expression in the elevated T-maze (n=8–10). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to the saline-injected group.

Table 1

Effect of treatment with intra-BLA or systemic injection of drugs on the distance travelled in the open-field test

Values are mean±s.e.m.

Table 1

Effect of treatment with intra-BLA or systemic injection of drugs on the distance travelled in the open-field test

Values are mean±s.e.m.

5-HT effects

Figure 3a shows that, similarly to MK-212, 5-HT facilitated inhibitory avoidance acquisition. There were significant effects of treatment (F1,19=4.57, p<0.05), trial (F2,38=18.96, p<0.05), and trial×treatment interaction (F2,38=3.99, p<0.05). Student's t test revealed that 5-HT significantly increased (p<0.05) avoidance 2 latency. 5-HT did not affect escape expression (Fig. 3b) or locomotion in the open field (Table 1).

5-HT injection (16 nmol) in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala facilitated (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition without interfering with (b) escape expression in the elevated T-maze (n=10–11). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to the saline-injected group.
Fig. 3

5-HT injection (16 nmol) in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala facilitated (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition without interfering with (b) escape expression in the elevated T-maze (n=10–11). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to the saline-injected group.

SB-242084 effects

Figure 4a shows that SB-242084 impaired inhibitory avoidance acquisition, indicating an anxiolytic effect. There was a significant effect of trial (F2,48=13.03, p<0.05), and trial×treatment interaction (F6,48=2.81, p<0.05). The effect of treatment was marginal for statistical significance (F3,24=2.71, p<0.07). SB-242084 at doses of 0.1 and 10 nmol significantly decreased (p<0.05) avoidance 2 latency compared to the saline group. This antagonist did not affect escape expression (Fig. 4b) or locomotion in the open field (Table 1).

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala injection of SB-242084 impaired (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition without affecting (b) escape expression in the elevated T-maze (n=6–8). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to the saline-injected group.
Fig. 4

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala injection of SB-242084 impaired (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition without affecting (b) escape expression in the elevated T-maze (n=6–8). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to the saline-injected group.

Expt 1b: previous intra-BLA administration of SB-242084 and 5-HT effects

As illustrated in Fig. 5a intra-BLA injection of 5-HT facilitated inhibitory avoidance acquisition. Previous microinjection of SB-242084 counteracted this effect. Repeated-measures ANOVA showed significant main effects of trial (F2,46=33.40, p<0.05) and treatment with the agonist (F1,23=9.00, p<0.05). There was also a significant interaction among the three factors analysed (agonist×antagonist×trial: F2,46=7.12, p<0.05). The post-hoc test showed that 5-HT significantly increased (p<0.05) avoidance 1 and 2 latencies. Prior intra-BLA administration of SB-242084 fully blocked this anxiogenic effect.

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the facilitatory effect on (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition caused by local microinjection of 5-HT (16 nmol) (n=6–7). (b) Lack of SB-242084 and 5-HT effect on escape performance (n=6–7). Values are mean±s.e.m. +p<0.05 compared to all other groups.
Fig. 5

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the facilitatory effect on (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition caused by local microinjection of 5-HT (16 nmol) (n=6–7). (b) Lack of SB-242084 and 5-HT effect on escape performance (n=6–7). Values are mean±s.e.m. +p<0.05 compared to all other groups.

None of the treatments employed altered escape expression (Fig. 5b) or locomotion in the open field (Table 1).

Expt 2a: previous intra-BLA administration of SB-242084 and systemic imipramine effect in the elevated T-maze

Figure 6a shows that imipramine significantly facilitated inhibitory avoidance acquisition and that this anxiogenic effect was blocked by prior intra-BLA administration of SB-242084. Repeated-measures ANOVA showed significant main effects of trial (F2,60=37.21, p<0.05) and antagonist treatment (F1,30=10.09, p<0.05). There was also a significant interaction among the three factors analysed (systemic drug×antagonist×trial: F2,60=3.42, p<0.05). The post-hoc test showed that imipramine significantly increased (p<0.05) avoidance 1 and 2 latencies. Prior intra-BLA administration of SB-242084 fully blocked this anxiogenic effect.

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the facilitatory effect on (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition caused by an acute systemic injection of imipramine (10 mg/kg) (n=7–10). (b) Lack of SB-242084 effect on the anti-escape consequence of imipramine administration (n=7–10). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to all other groups.
Fig. 6

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the facilitatory effect on (a) inhibitory avoidance acquisition caused by an acute systemic injection of imipramine (10 mg/kg) (n=7–10). (b) Lack of SB-242084 effect on the anti-escape consequence of imipramine administration (n=7–10). Values are mean±s.e.m. * p<0.05 compared to all other groups.

None of the treatments employed altered escape expression (Fig. 6b) or locomotion in the open field (Table 1).

Expt 2b: previous intra-BLA administration of SB-242084 and systemic imipramine effect in the Vogel conflict test

Figure 7 shows that imipramine significantly decreased the number of punished licks in the Vogel conflict test and that this anxiogenic effect was counteracted by previous microinjection of SB-242084. Two-way ANOVA showed a significant effect of treatment with the antagonist (F1,33=4.65, p<0.05) and a marginal effect of the systemic treatment (F1,33=3.70, p=0.06). There was also a significant interaction between the two factors (systemic drug×antagonist: F1,33=10.5, p<0.05).

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the decrease in the number of punished drinking caused by an acute systemic injection of imipramine (10 mg/kg) (n=8–9). Values are mean±s.e.m. +p<0.05 compared to all other groups.
Fig. 7

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the decrease in the number of punished drinking caused by an acute systemic injection of imipramine (10 mg/kg) (n=8–9). Values are mean±s.e.m. +p<0.05 compared to all other groups.

Expt 2c: previous intra-BLA administration of SB-242084 and systemic fluoxetine effect in the Vogel conflict test

As observed with imipramine, fluoxetine also decreased the number of punished licks in the Vogel conflict test and this anxiogenic effect was blocked by previous microinjection of SB-242084 (see Fig. 8). Two-way ANOVA showed a significant effect of treatment with the antagonist (F1,34=21.85, p<0.05) and the systemic injection of fluoxetine (F1,34=4.12, p<0.05). There was also a significant interaction between the two factors (systemic drug×antagonist: F1,34=5.14, p<0.05).

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the decrease in the number of punished drinking caused by an acute systemic injection of fluoxetine (15 mg/kg) (n=8–11). Values are mean±s.e.m. +p<0.05 compared to all other groups.
Fig. 8

Intra-basolateral nucleus of the amygdala administration of SB-242084 (0.01 nmol) blocked the decrease in the number of punished drinking caused by an acute systemic injection of fluoxetine (15 mg/kg) (n=8–11). Values are mean±s.e.m. +p<0.05 compared to all other groups.

As revealed by the additional control experiment performed (values are mean±s.e.m.), neither imipramine (Sal=1136.7±64.7, Imi=1152.7±62.9) nor fluoxetine (vehicle=1169.3±68.6, Flx=1200.2±79.7) affected the number of unpunished licks on the test day.

Discussion

The objective of the present study was to investigate the role played by 5-HT2CRs of the BLA in the regulation of anxiety- and panic-related defensive behaviours and their involvement in the mediation of the acute anxiogenic effect caused by the antidepressant drugs imipramine and fluoxetine.

The results showed that intra-BLA injection of the 5-HT2CR agonist MK-212 or the endogenous agonist 5-HT facilitated inhibitory avoidance acquisition, suggesting an anxiogenic effect, without affecting escape performance. On the other hand, microinjection of the selective 5-HT2CR antagonist SB-242084 caused the opposite effect, i.e. anxiolysis, also without changing escape expression. The rise in anxiety observed after 5-HT microinjection was fully blocked by previous intra-BLA injection of SB-242084, confirming the recruitment of 5-HT2CR for this effect.

Regarding the results of MK-212, it is of note that the highest dose tested did not share the anxiogenic effect observed with lower doses. This lack of effect may conceivably reflect the interaction of this drug with other sites besides 5-HT2CRs. Although MK-212 displays high affinity for 5-HT2CRs it also binds with moderate affinity to 5-HT2ARs (Knight et al.2004). It has been shown in our laboratory that intra-BLA administration of the preferential 5-HT2AR agonist DOI causes anxiolysis in the elevated T-maze (C. A. Strauss, M. A. Vicente, H. Zangrossi Jr., unpublished results). It is therefore possible that interaction of MK-212 with 5-HT2Rs, that exert an opposed role in anxiety modulation, may have contributed to the lack of effect observed with the highest dose.

None of the compounds tested in the present study interfered with the distance travelled in the open field, thereby excluding non-specific motor interference as the main factor accounting for the effects observed upon inhibitory avoidance acquisition.

Taken together these results are indicative that 5-HT2CRs in the BLA are selectively involved in the regulation of defensive behaviours associated with generalized anxiety (inhibitory avoidance), but not panic (escape).

The facilitatory role of BLA 5-HT2CRs in anxiety has also been demonstrated in other experimental models. Campbell & Merchant (2003) reported that bilateral injection of m-CPP or IL-639, non-selective and selective 5-HT2CR agonists, respectively, had anxiogenic consequences in the ultrasonic vocalization and open-field tests. This effect was blocked by the systemic injection of SB-242084. In the elevated plus-maze, systemic injection of MK-212 increased anxiety and intra-BLA injection of the non-selective 5-HT2CR antagonist ritanserin counteracted this effect (de Mello Cruz et al.2005).

The finding that SB-242084 has an anxiolytic effect when injected in the BLA raises two, possibly concurrent, interpretations: one is that 5-HT in this nucleus exerts a tonic facilitatory influence on anxiety-related behaviours (i.e. 5-HT is continuously released in the area) and the other is that BLA 5-HT2CRs are spontaneously active (constitutively active) in the regulation of these behaviours. Regarding the latter, a wealth of evidence indicates that 5-HT2CRs possess high levels of constitutive activity, i.e. they can activate intracellular signalling without the presence of the endogenous agonist (for a review see Aloyo et al.2009). If this is the case in the present study, SB-242084 could be acting as an inverse agonist, a mode of action already reported for this drug (Aloyo et al.2009). Further studies assessing, for instance, the behavioural effect of intra-BLA injection of SB-242084 in the brain of 5-HT-depleted animals (e.g. by intra-dorsal raphe nucleus injection of the neurotoxin 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine), will certainly contribute to the clarification of this question.

The effect of 5-HT2CR activation in the BLA in the elevated T-maze was opposite to that caused by the stimulation of 5-HT1ARs in this subnucleus. In test-naive rats, bilateral microinjection of 8-OH-DPAT, a 5-HT1AR agonist, or the benzodiazepine anxiolytic midazolam impaired inhibitory avoidance acquisition, without changing escape expression (Zangrossi et al.1999). More recently, we observed that in animals previously exposed to one of the elevated T-maze open arms, as performed in the present study, intra-BLA injection of 8-OH-DPAT retained its anxiolytic effect, but also inhibited escape expression, indicating a panicolytic-like effect (C. A. Strauss, M. A. Vicente, H. Zangrossi Jr., unpublished results). As mentioned before, the same pattern of results was observed after the intra-BLA injection of the preferential 5-HT2AR agonist DOI (C. A. Strauss, M. A. Vicente, H. Zangrossi Jr., unpublished results). Therefore, among those 5-HT receptor subtypes more consistently implicated in defence regulation, 5-HT2CRs in the BLA seem to be unique in their selective involvement with anxiety- but not panic-related behaviours.

The most important finding of this study is that intra-BLA administration of a sub-effective dose of SB-242084 fully blocked the acute anxiogenic effect caused by imipramine and fluoxetine in the Vogel conflict test and imipramine in the elevated T-maze. This observation adds to a series of evidence showing that 5-HT2CR in the BLA mediates the anxiogenic effect generated by different conditions/situations such as exposure to stressful stimuli (Christianson et al.2010) and discontinuation of chronic ethanol ingestion (Overstreet et al.2006). It also extends previous evidence showing that systemic injection of 5-HT2CR antagonists fully counteracted the acute anxiogenesis caused by antidepressants in different animal models of anxiety (Bagdy et al.2001; Burghardt et al.2007; Greenwood et al.2008; Yamauchi et al.2004).

Although no study has directly investigated the effect of imipramine or fluoxetine, in-vivo microdialysis studies have shown that acute systemic injection of the SSRI citalopram enhances 5-HT levels in the amygdala (Bosker et al.2001; Gobert et al.2009). The same effect on 5-HT concentration was observed when the SSRI fluvoxamine was injected directly into the BLA (van der Stelt et al.2005). In keeping with the present result with 5-HT, intra-BLA microinjection of fluoxetine enhanced anxiety in a contextual fear paradigm (Martinez et al.2007). Therefore, it is conceivable that facilitation of 5-HT neurotransmission in the BLA, through mediation of 5-HT2CRs, may account for the acute anxiogenic effect caused by different classes of antidepressants known to interfere with the extracellular availability of 5-HT. In the case of imipramine and fluoxetine, evidence in the literature shows that these antidepressants may interact directly with 5-HT2CRs, although with weak affinity (Ni & Miledi, 1997; Pälvimäki et al.1996). However, given evidence that these drugs exert antagonistic and not agonistic activity on these receptors (Ni & Miledi, 1997; Pälvimäki et al.1996) and that injection of SB-242084 in the BLA is anxiolytic (expt 1a), it is unlikely that this mechanism of action mediates their acute anxiogenic effect.

We are currently investigating whether prolonged treatment with imipramine or fluoxetine affects the reactivity of 5-HT2CRs in the BLA. According to Millan (2003, 2005), the shift from anxiogenesis to anxiolysis caused by these drugs over time involves both the desensitization of 5-HT2CRs and the sensitization of 5-HT1ARs located in limbic areas. In line with this hypothesis, as mentioned above, facilitation of 5-HT1AR-mediated neurotransmission in the BLA is anxiolytic in the elevated T-maze (Zangrossi et al.1999). It also remains to be investigated whether this effect is potentiated after chronic treatment with antidepressants.

In conclusion, our findings highlight the selective involvement of BLA 5-HT2CRs in the regulation of anxiety-related behaviours and provide the first direct evidence of their role in mediation of the short-term aversive effect of antidepressants.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Afonso Paulo Padovan for helpful technical support. The experiments described in this paper were funded by FAPESP and CNPq, Brazil.

Statement of Interest

None.

References

Aloyo
VJ
Berg
KA
Spampinato
U
Clarke
WP
et al. (
2009
).
Current status ofinverse agonism at serotonin2A (5-HT2A) and 5-HT2C receptors
.
Pharmacology and Therapeutics
121
,
160
173
.

Alves
SH
Pinheiro
G
Motta
V
Landeira-Fernandez
J
et al. (
2004
).
Anxiogenic effects in the rat elevated plus-maze of 5-HT(2C) agonists into ventral but not dorsal hippocampus
.
Behavioural Pharmacology
15
,
37
43
.

Bagdy
G
Graf
M
Anheuer
ZE
Modos
EA
et al. (
2001
).
Anxiety-like effects induced by acute fluoxetine, sertraline or m-CPP treatment are reversed by pretreatment with the 5-HT2C receptor antagonist SB-242084 but not the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY-100635
.
International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology
4
,
399
408
.

Bakker
A
van Balkom
AJ
Stein
DJ
(
2005
).
Evidence-based pharmacotherapy of panic disorder
.
International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology
8
,
473
482
.

Berg
KA
Clarke
WP
Cunningham
KA
Spampinato
U
(
2008
).
Fine-tuning serotonin2c receptor function in the brain: molecular and functional implications
.
Neuropharmacology
55
,
969
976
.

Blanchard
DC
Griebel
G
Blanchard
RJ
(
2001
).
Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
25
,
205
218
.

Blanchard
DC
Griebel
G
Blanchard
RJ
(
2003
).
The Mouse Defense Test Battery: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
463
,
97
116
.

Bockaert
J
Claeysen
S
Dumuis
A
Marin
P
(
2010
).
Classification and signaling characteristics of 5-HT receptors
. In:
Muller
CP
Jacobs
BL
(Eds),
Handbook of the Behavioral Neurobiology of Serotonin
(pp.
102
122
).
San Diego
:
Elsevier
.

Bosker
FJ
Cremers
TI
Jongsma
ME
Westerink
BH
et al. (
2001
).
Acute and chronic effects of citalopram on postsynaptic 5-hydroxytryptamine(1A) receptor-mediated feedback: a microdialysis study in the amygdala
.
Journal of Neurochemistry
6
,
1645
1653
.

Bressa
GM
Marini
S
Gregori
S
(
1987
).
Serotonin S2 receptors blockage and generalized anxiety disorders. A double-blind study on ritanserin and lorazepam
.
International Journal of Clinical Pharmacology Research
2
,
111
119
.

Broocks
A
Briggs
NC
Pigott
TA
Hill
JL
et al. (
1997
).
Behavioral, physiological and neuroendocrine responses in healthy volunteers to m-chlorophenylpiperazine (m-CPP) with and without ondansetron pretreatment
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
130
,
91
103
.

Burghardt
NS
Bush
DE
McEwen
BS
LeDoux
JE
(
2007
).
Acute selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors increase conditioned fear expression: blockade with a 5-HT(2C) receptor antagonist
.
Biological Psychiatry
62
,
1111
1118
.

Campbell
BM
Merchant
KM
(
2003
).
Serotonin 2C receptors within the basolateral amygdala induce acute fear-like responses in an open-field environment
.
Brain Research
993
,
1
9
.

Ceulemans
DL
Hoppenbrouwers
ML
Gelders
YG
Reyntjens
AJ
(
1985
).
The influence of ritanserin, a serotonin antagonist, in anxiety disorders: a double-blind placebo-controlled study vs. lorazepam
.
Pharmacopsychiatry
18
,
303
305
.

Christianson
JP
Ragole
T
Amat
J
Greenwood
BN
et al. (
2010
).
5-hydroxytryptamine 2C receptors in the basolateral amygdala are involved in the expression of anxiety after uncontrollable traumatic stress
.
Biological Psychiatry
67
,
339
345
.

Clemett
DA
Punhani
T
Duxon
MS
Blackburn
TP
et al. (
2000
).
Immunohistochemical localisation of the 5-HT2C receptor protein in the rat CNS
.
Neuropharmacology
39
,
123
132
.

de Bodinat
C
Guardiola-Lemaitre
B
Mocaër
E
Renard
P
et al. (
2010
).
Agomelatine, the first melatonergic antidepressant: discovery, characterization and development
.
Nature Reviews Drug Discovery
8
,
628
642
.

de Mello Cruz
AP
Pinheiro
G
Alves
SH
Ferreira
G
et al. (
2005
).
Behavioral effects of systemically administered MK-212 are prevented by ritanserin microinfusion into the basolateral amygdala of rats exposed to the elevated plus-maze
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
182
,
345
354
.

Dupuy
JM
Ostacher
MJ
Huffman
J
Perlis
RH
et al. (
2011
).
A critical review of pharmacotherapy for major depressive disorder
.
International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology
24
,
1
15
.

Giorgetti
M
Tecott
LH
(
2004
).
Contributions of 5-HT(2C) receptors to multiple actions of central serotonin systems
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
488
,
1
9
.

Gobert
A
Brocco
M
Dekeyne
A
Di Cara
B
et al. (
2009
).
Neurokinin1 antagonists potentiate antidepressant properties of serotonin reuptake inhibitors, yet blunt their anxiogenic actions: a neurochemical, electrophysiological, and behavioral characterization
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
34
,
1039
1056
.

Graeff
FG
Zangrossi
H
(
2002
).
Animal models of anxiety disorders.
In:
Dl'Haenen
H
den Boer
JA
Westenberg
H
Willner
P
(Eds),
Textbook of Biological Psychiatry
(pp.
879
893
).
London
:
Wiley and Sons
.

Gray
JA
McNaughton
N
(
2000
).
Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety
.
Journal Affect Disorders
61
,
161
176
.

Greenwood
BN
Strong
PV
Brooks
L
Fleshner
M
(
2008
).
Anxiety-like behaviors produced by acute fluoxetine administration in male Fischer 344 rats are prevented by prior exercise
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
199
,
209
222
.

Harada
K
Yamaji
T
Matsuoka
N
(
2008
).
Activation of the serotonin 5-HT2C receptor is involved in the enhanced anxiety in rats after single-prolonged stress
.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior
89
,
11
16
.

Heisler
LK
Zhou
L
Bajwa
P
Hsu
J
et al. (
2007
).
Serotonin 5-HT(2C) receptors regulate anxiety-like behavior
.
Genes Brain and Behavior
6
,
491
496
.

Hoyer
D
Hannon
JP
Martin
GR
(
2002
).
Molecular, pharmacological and functional diversity of 5-HT receptors
.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior
71
,
533
554
.

Jan
RK
Lin
JC
Lee
H
Sheridan
JL
et al. (
2010
).
Determining the subjective effects of TFMPP in human males
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
211
,
347
353
.

Kennett
GA
Whitton
P
Shah
K
Curzon
G
(
1989
).
Anxiogenic-like effects of mCPP and TFMPP in animal models are opposed by 5-HT1C receptor antagonists
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
164
,
445
454
.

Kennett
GA
Wood
MD
Bright
F
Trail
B
et al. (
1997
).
SB 242084, a selective and brain penetrant 5-HT2C receptor antagonist
.
Neuropharmacology
36
,
609
620
.

Kimura
A
Stevenson
PL
Carter
RN
Maccoll
G
et al. (
2009
).
Overexpression of 5-HT2C receptors in forebrain leads to elevated anxiety and hypoactivity
.
European Journal of Neuroscience
30
,
299
306
.

Knight
AR
Misra
A
Quirk
K
Benwell
K
et al. (
2004
).
Pharmacological characterisation of the agonist radioligand binding site of 5-HT(2A), 5-HT(2B) and 5-HT(2C) receptors
.
Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's Archives of Pharmacology
370
,
114
123
.

Landolt
HP
Wehrle
R
(
2009
).
Antagonism of serotonergic 5-HT2A/2C receptors: mutual improvement of sleep, cognition and mood?
European Journal of Neuroscience
29
,
1795
1809
.

Martin
JR
Ballard
TM
Higgins
GA
(
2002
).
Influence of the 5-HT2C receptor antagonist, SB-242084, in tests of anxiety
.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior
71
,
615
625
.

Martinez
RC
Ribeiro de Oliveira
A
Brandão
ML
(
2007
).
Serotonergic mechanisms in the basolateral amygdala differentially regulate the conditioned and unconditioned fear organized in the periaqueductal gray
.
European Neuropsychopharmacology
17
,
717
724
.

McNaughton
N
Corr
PJ
(
2004
).
A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
28
,
285
305
.

McNaughton
N
Gray
JA
(
2000
).
Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety
.
Journal of Affective Disorders
61
,
161
176
.

McNaughton
N
Zangrossi
HJ
(
2008
).
Animal models of anxiety, fear and defense: theoretical to the modeling of anxiety in animals
. In:
Blanchard
R
Blanchard
D
Griebel
G
Nutt
D
(Eds),
Handbook of Anxiety and Fear
(pp.
11
27
).
Amsterdam
:
Elsevier
.

Mengod
G
Cortés
R
Vilaró
MT
Hoyer
D
(
2010
).
Distribution of 5-HT receptors in the central nervous system
. In:
Muller
CP
Jacobs
BL
(Eds),
Handbook of the Behavioral Neurobiology of Serotonin
(pp.
123
139
).
San Diego
:
Elsevier
.

Millan
MJ
(
2003
).
The neurobiology and control of anxious states
.
Progress in Neurobiology
70
,
83
244
.

Millan
MJ
(
2005
).
Serotonin 5-HT2C receptors as a target for the treatment of depressive and anxious states: focus on novel therapeutic strategies
.
Therapie
60
,
441
460
.

Millan
MJ
Brocco
M
(
2003
).
The Vogel conflict test: procedural aspects, gamma-aminobutyric acid, glutamate and monoamines
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
463
,
67
96
.

Millan
MJ
Brocco
M
Gobert
A
Dekeyne
A
(
2005
).
Anxiolytic properties of agomelatine, an antidepressant with melatoninergic and serotonergic properties: role of 5-HT2C receptor blockade
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
177
,
448
458
.

Navailles
S
Moison
D
Ryczko
D
Spampinato
U
(
2006
).
Region-dependent regulation of mesoaccumbens dopamine neurons in vivo by the constitutive activity of central serotonin2C receptors
.
Journal of Neurochemistry
99
,
1311
1319
.

Ni
YG
Miledi
R
(
1997
).
Blockage of 5HT2C serotonin receptors by fluoxetine (Prozac)
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
94
,
2036
2040
.

Overstreet
DH
Knapp
DJ
Angel
RA
Navarro
M
et al. (
2006
).
Reduction in repeated ethanol-withdrawal-induced anxiety-like behavior by site-selective injections of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2C ligands
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
187
,
1
12
.

Overstreet
DH
Knapp
DJ
Moy
SS
Breese
GR
(
2003
).
A 5-HT1A agonist and a 5-HT2c antagonist reduce social interaction deficit induced by multiple ethanol withdrawals in rats
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
167
,
344
352
.

Pälvimäki
EP
Roth
BL
Majasuo
H
Laakso
A
et al. (
1996
).
Interactions of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors with the serotonin 5-HT2c receptor
.
Psychopharmacology (Berlin)
126
,
234
240
.

Paxinos
G
Watson
C
(
2007
).
The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates
, 6th edn.
San Diego
:
Academic Press
.

Pazos
A
Hoyer
D
Palacios
JM
(
1984
).
The binding of serotonergic ligands to the porcine choroid plexus: characterization of a new type of serotonin recognition site
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
106
,
539
546
.

Pelosi
GG
Resstel
LL
Soares
VP
Zangrossi
H
et al. (
2009
).
Anxiolytic-like effect of noradrenaline microinjection into the dorsal periaqueductal gray of rats
.
Behavioural Pharmacology
20
,
252
259
.

Pesold
C
Treit
D
(
1995
).
The central and basolateral amygdala differentially mediate the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines
.
Brain Research
671
,
213
221
.

Pinheiro
SH
Zangrossi
H
Del-Ben
CM
Graeff
FG
(
2007
).
Elevated mazes as animal models of anxiety: effects of serotonergic agents
.
Anais da Academia Brasileira de Ciências
79
,
71
85
.

Poltronieri
SC
Zangrossi
H
de Barros Viana
M
(
2003
).
Antipanic-like effect of serotonin reuptake inhibitors in the elevated T-maze
.
Behavioural Brain Research
147
,
185
192
.

Pompeiano
M
Palacios
JM
Mengod
G
(
1994
).
Distribution of the serotonin 5-HT2 receptor family mRNAs: comparison between 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors
.
Behavioural Brain Research
23
,
163
178
.

Ravindran
LN
Stein
MB
(
2010
).
The pharmacologic treatment of anxiety disorders: a review of progress
.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
7
,
839
854
.

Rosen
JB
(
2004
).
The neurobiology of conditioned and unconditioned fear: a neurobehavioral system analysis of the amygdala
.
Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews
3
,
23
41
.

Setem
J
Pinheiro
AP
Motta
VA
Morato
S
et al. (
1999
).
Ethopharmacological analysis of 5-HT ligands on the rat elevated plus-maze
.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior
62
,
515
521
.

Stein
DJ
Ahokas
AA
de Bodinat
C
(
2008
).
Efficacy of agomelatine in generalized anxiety disorder: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study
.
Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology
28
,
561
566
.

Teixeira
RC
Zangrossi
H
Graeff
FG
(
2000
).
Behavioral effects of acute and chronic imipramine in the elevated T-maze model of anxiety
.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior
65
,
571
576
.

van der Stelt
HM
Breuer
ME
Olivier
B
Westenberg
HG
(
2005
).
Permanent deficits in serotonergic functioning of olfactory bulbectomized rats: an in vivo microdialysis study
.
Biological Psychiatry
57
,
1061
1067
.

Vertes
RP
(
1991
).
A PHA-L analysis of ascending projections of the dorsal raphe nucleus in the rat
.
Journal of Comparative Neurology
313
,
643
668
.

Walker
EA
Kohut
SJ
Hass
RW
Brown
EK
et al. (
2005
).
Selective and nonselective serotonin antagonists block the aversive stimulus properties of MK212 and m-chloro-phenylpiperazine (mCPP) in mice
.
Neuropharmacology
49
,
1210
1219
.

Yamauchi
M
Tatebayashi
T
Nagase
K
Kojima
M
et al. (
2004
).
Chronic treatment with fluvoxamine desensitizes 5-HT2C receptor-mediated hypolocomotion in rats
.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior
78
,
683
689
.

Zangrossi
H
Graeff
FG
(
1997
).
Behavioral validation of the elevated T-maze, a new animal model of anxiety
.
Brain Research Bulletin
44
,
1
5
.

Zangrossi
H
Viana
MB
Graeff
FG
(
1999
).
Anxiolytic effect of intra-amygdala injection of midazolam and 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin in the elevated T-maze
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
369
,
267
270
.

Zanoveli
JM
Nogueira
RL
Zangrossi
H
(
2003
).
Serotonin in the dorsal periaqueductal gray modulates inhibitory avoidance and one-way escape behaviors in the elevated T-maze
.
European Journal of Pharmacology
473
,
153
161
.