Abstract

Background

Exaggerated dispersion of repolarization is likely to play a role in both the initiation and the maintenance of malignant ventricular arrhythmias. It was proposed that the inter-lead QT interval differences within a 12-lead ECG termed ‘QT dispersion’ might reflect regional differences in myocardial refractoriness, and that this might predict cardiac dysrhythmias.

Aim

The study aimed to perform a meta-analysis of the relevant available publications.

Methods

A MEDLINE search from 1990 to 2012 at PubMed (NLM) was performed. An all-fields search for index terms ‘QT dispersion’ and ‘anxiety’ was done. Case-control studies and surveys were included in the analysis provided they encompassed physically healthy subjects who had been evaluated for anxiety disorders with validated inventories. Outcome measures were either crude or heart rate-corrected QT dispersions.

Results

Five studies were included in the analysis, which encompassed 580 patients with calculated mean age of 36.8 years (standard error = 5.5). The values of crude and heart rate-corrected QT dispersions across the studies were statistically significantly higher in the patients with anxiety disorders. Crude values of QT dispersion were available in 164 patients with anxiety and in 273 controls. Summarized standardized difference in the means (random effect model) was equal to 1.472 (95% confidence interval: 1.034–1.911). Heart rate-corrected QT dispersion values were available in 193 patients with anxiety and in 282 controls, and the summarized standardized difference in the means was equal to 3.299 (95% confidence interval: 1.215–5.384).

Conclusion

High anxiety is associated with increased QT dispersion, which may predispose to cardiac arrhythmias.

Introduction

Exaggerated dispersion of repolarization is likely to play a role in both the initiation and the maintenance of malignant ventricular arrhythmias by generating a substrate for functional re-entry.13 It was proposed that the inter-lead QT interval differences within a 12-lead ECG might reflect regional differences in myocardial refractoriness, and that this might predict cardiac dysrhythmias. This range of the QT durations was termed ‘QT dispersion’.4 It was proposed that the different ECG leads magnify the ECG signal of different myocardial regions and that, consequently, QT dispersion (QTd) is an almost direct measure of the heterogeneity of myocardial repolarization.5 Animal6,7 and human8,9 studies supported this hypothesis.5

QTd is simply defined as the difference between the longest (QTmax) and the shortest (QTmin) QT intervals within a 12-lead ECG. Since the QT interval on its own may vary according to the heart rate, it may be corrected using Bazett's formula (QT/RR ) to give rise to QTc intervals, and the difference between QTcmax and QTcmin is the QTc dispersion (heart rate-corrected QTd).

The average normal value of QTd in normal subjects was ≤40 ms in 13 studies and ≥40 ms in eight studies.10 It is suggested that QTd in normal subjects should be ≤50 ms.11

Abnormal repolarization featuring as increased QTd is known to contribute to the fatal mechanisms associated with numerous cardiovascular pathologies.1215

Much less is known about possible disturbances of repolarization in patients with psychopathological conditions. Psychological factors, such as stress, exhaustion and negative emotions, can affect the electrical stability of the heart and trigger arrhythmias.16,17 Of particular interest is the potential pro-arrhythmic effect of anxiety disorders. Anxiety may be defined as a diffuse state characterized by an unpleasant affective experience marked by a significant degree of apprehensiveness about the potential occurrence of future adverse or harmful events.1820 There is evidence for dysregulation of serotonin, dopamine and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis systems,21 as well as differences in startle and autonomic reactivity22 in the central nervous system of patients with anxiety disorders. Spectral analysis of heart rate variability has confirmed increased indexes of sympathetic modulation, reduced indexes of parasympathetic modulation, or both in subjects with anxiety-spectrum disorders.2328 Patients with generalized anxiety disorders have increased levels of plasma norepinephrine and its metabolites.29,30 In all anxiety states the symptoms of palpitations, chest pain and shortness of breath reveal the involvement of the autonomic nervous system.31 It is speculated that decreased parasympathetic innervation exposes the heart to unopposed stimulation by sympathetic nerves and may be one link between psychopathology and heart disease.3235 Sympathetic hyperactivity, specifically an imbalance between left and right stellate ganglion activity, promotes a prolonged and increased dispersion of repolarization, thereby lowering the ventricular fibrillation threshold.36 It is not therefore surprising that anxiety and the reduced positive affect are associated with increased risk of death from coronary artery disease.34,37,38 These arguments lead to the assumption that the anxiety disorders may be associated with specific features of QT interval, specifically increased QTd on the surface ECG.

To address the issue, this study aimed to perform a meta-analysis of the available relevant publications. The primary aim of the meta-analysis was to study whether the physically healthy subjects characterized by high anxiety have increased QTd as compared with the low-anxiety counterparts.

Material and methods

Search strategy for identification of studies

A MEDLINE search from 1990 to 2012 at PubMed (NLM) was performed. An all-fields search for the index term ‘QT dispersion’ was done. EndNote X3 software was used as a search tool. The abstracts of the retrieved publications were further scrutinized for their relevance to the anxiety disorders, and the candidate papers were read in full. Case-control studies, as well as the surveys that included comparisons between the low and high anxiety groups, were included in the analysis, provided they encompassed physically healthy persons. The studies were excluded if the patients had a history or demonstrable evidence of cardiovascular, respiratory, renal, liver or gastrointestinal diseases, were receiving specific medications, or had electrolyte disturbances. As part of the studies’ protocols, the patients should have been evaluated for anxiety disorders with certain validated inventories. Types of ‘outcome measures’ were either crude or heart rate-corrected QTd.

Methods of the review

Data extraction

Decisions on which studies to include and which variables to use when more options were available for the same outcome were based on the methods sections of the studies only. Details of the methods used for evaluation of the signs of anxiety, number of patients who entered the studies, their age and gender, the respective numbers with high and with low anxiety levels, and crude and rate-corrected QTd values were extracted.

Statistics

The effect size was calculated as the Cohen’s d standardized mean differences with 95% confidence intervals (CIs).39 Since heterogeneity of the studies was expected because of various designs, settings and patients covered by the studies, and approaches to the identification of the signs of anxiety, a random effects model was used, as suggested. Two subgroups of outcome measures (either crude or rate-corrected QTd) were defined, and the analysis in the two subgroups was performed.40 Comprehensive meta-analysis (CMA) software version 2.0 was used for computations.41

Results

Description of studies

A MEDLINE search listed 1878 studies published from 1990 to 2012 that matched the search term ‘QT dispersion’. Of these, six studies met the inclusion criteria, that is, compared QTd values in physically healthy subjects with either high or low levels of anxiety. Five studies were published in English and one study in Korean with abstract and tables in English. One study42 was excluded from the analysis to avoid redundancy and publication bias, because a more recent paper from the same research group and covering a larger number of patients was available, and it was this latter publication that was included in the analysis.43  Table 1 is a summary of the papers that were included in the meta-analysis.4347

Table 1.

Studies included in the meta-analysis

SourcePatient characteristicsPsychometric tests used
Piccirillo et al.43Apparently healthy adult patients (N = 143: 72 men, 71 women) recruited from people presenting spontaneously to outpatient clinic for preventative reasons; 52 patients aged 77 ± 2 years, 91 patients aged 41 ± 2 years.The Cornell Anxiety self-rated subscale
Nahshoni et al.44Physically healthy and non-depressed outpatients with long-term social phobia (N = 16; age 37.9 ± 9.6 years) and 15 physically and mentally healthy age- and gender-matched controls.The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS)
Uyarel et al.45Physically and mentally healthy male volunteers (N = 726; age 21.23 ± 1.25 years). Of these, 83 patients (age 21.18 ± 0.95 years) had a high level of situation anxiety, 193 patients (age 21.09 ± 1.45 years) had low anxiety level.The Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
Kwon et al.46Physically healthy social phobia outpatients (N = 25: 16 men, nine women; average age 35.5 ± 12.9) and age and sex matched control group of mentally and physically healthy volunteers (N = 25: 16 men, 9 women; average age 34.83 ± 12.7).Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)
Atmaca et al.47Subjects diagnosed with panic disorder (N = 40: 24 female and 16 male; age 31.17 ± 6.79 years). The control group consisted of 40 healthy individuals (25 female and 15 male; 29.97 ± 4.89 years) matched by sex, age and smoking status. All subjects were normal on physical examination, 12-lead ECG and routine blood tests. The inclusion criteria included not using either vasoactive or psychotropic agents (e.g. antipsychotics, anxiolytics) for at least two weeks.The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) and the Panic Agoraphobia Scale (PAS)
SourcePatient characteristicsPsychometric tests used
Piccirillo et al.43Apparently healthy adult patients (N = 143: 72 men, 71 women) recruited from people presenting spontaneously to outpatient clinic for preventative reasons; 52 patients aged 77 ± 2 years, 91 patients aged 41 ± 2 years.The Cornell Anxiety self-rated subscale
Nahshoni et al.44Physically healthy and non-depressed outpatients with long-term social phobia (N = 16; age 37.9 ± 9.6 years) and 15 physically and mentally healthy age- and gender-matched controls.The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS)
Uyarel et al.45Physically and mentally healthy male volunteers (N = 726; age 21.23 ± 1.25 years). Of these, 83 patients (age 21.18 ± 0.95 years) had a high level of situation anxiety, 193 patients (age 21.09 ± 1.45 years) had low anxiety level.The Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
Kwon et al.46Physically healthy social phobia outpatients (N = 25: 16 men, nine women; average age 35.5 ± 12.9) and age and sex matched control group of mentally and physically healthy volunteers (N = 25: 16 men, 9 women; average age 34.83 ± 12.7).Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)
Atmaca et al.47Subjects diagnosed with panic disorder (N = 40: 24 female and 16 male; age 31.17 ± 6.79 years). The control group consisted of 40 healthy individuals (25 female and 15 male; 29.97 ± 4.89 years) matched by sex, age and smoking status. All subjects were normal on physical examination, 12-lead ECG and routine blood tests. The inclusion criteria included not using either vasoactive or psychotropic agents (e.g. antipsychotics, anxiolytics) for at least two weeks.The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) and the Panic Agoraphobia Scale (PAS)
Table 1.

Studies included in the meta-analysis

SourcePatient characteristicsPsychometric tests used
Piccirillo et al.43Apparently healthy adult patients (N = 143: 72 men, 71 women) recruited from people presenting spontaneously to outpatient clinic for preventative reasons; 52 patients aged 77 ± 2 years, 91 patients aged 41 ± 2 years.The Cornell Anxiety self-rated subscale
Nahshoni et al.44Physically healthy and non-depressed outpatients with long-term social phobia (N = 16; age 37.9 ± 9.6 years) and 15 physically and mentally healthy age- and gender-matched controls.The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS)
Uyarel et al.45Physically and mentally healthy male volunteers (N = 726; age 21.23 ± 1.25 years). Of these, 83 patients (age 21.18 ± 0.95 years) had a high level of situation anxiety, 193 patients (age 21.09 ± 1.45 years) had low anxiety level.The Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
Kwon et al.46Physically healthy social phobia outpatients (N = 25: 16 men, nine women; average age 35.5 ± 12.9) and age and sex matched control group of mentally and physically healthy volunteers (N = 25: 16 men, 9 women; average age 34.83 ± 12.7).Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)
Atmaca et al.47Subjects diagnosed with panic disorder (N = 40: 24 female and 16 male; age 31.17 ± 6.79 years). The control group consisted of 40 healthy individuals (25 female and 15 male; 29.97 ± 4.89 years) matched by sex, age and smoking status. All subjects were normal on physical examination, 12-lead ECG and routine blood tests. The inclusion criteria included not using either vasoactive or psychotropic agents (e.g. antipsychotics, anxiolytics) for at least two weeks.The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) and the Panic Agoraphobia Scale (PAS)
SourcePatient characteristicsPsychometric tests used
Piccirillo et al.43Apparently healthy adult patients (N = 143: 72 men, 71 women) recruited from people presenting spontaneously to outpatient clinic for preventative reasons; 52 patients aged 77 ± 2 years, 91 patients aged 41 ± 2 years.The Cornell Anxiety self-rated subscale
Nahshoni et al.44Physically healthy and non-depressed outpatients with long-term social phobia (N = 16; age 37.9 ± 9.6 years) and 15 physically and mentally healthy age- and gender-matched controls.The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS)
Uyarel et al.45Physically and mentally healthy male volunteers (N = 726; age 21.23 ± 1.25 years). Of these, 83 patients (age 21.18 ± 0.95 years) had a high level of situation anxiety, 193 patients (age 21.09 ± 1.45 years) had low anxiety level.The Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
Kwon et al.46Physically healthy social phobia outpatients (N = 25: 16 men, nine women; average age 35.5 ± 12.9) and age and sex matched control group of mentally and physically healthy volunteers (N = 25: 16 men, 9 women; average age 34.83 ± 12.7).Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)
Atmaca et al.47Subjects diagnosed with panic disorder (N = 40: 24 female and 16 male; age 31.17 ± 6.79 years). The control group consisted of 40 healthy individuals (25 female and 15 male; 29.97 ± 4.89 years) matched by sex, age and smoking status. All subjects were normal on physical examination, 12-lead ECG and routine blood tests. The inclusion criteria included not using either vasoactive or psychotropic agents (e.g. antipsychotics, anxiolytics) for at least two weeks.The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) and the Panic Agoraphobia Scale (PAS)

Overall, the analysis encompassed 580 patients with diagnosed high and low anxiety levels with calculated mean age of 36.8 years (standard error of 5.5 years). Table 2 summarizes raw data on QTd values extracted from the studies. In all the studies, the values of QTds (either crude or heart rate-corrected) were statistically significantly higher in the patients from anxiety groups.

Table 2.

Raw findings from the studies included in the meta-analysis

Study nameSubgroup within studyAnxiety mean (ms)Anxiety standard deviation (ms)Anxiety sample sizeControl mean (ms)Control standard deviation (ms)Control sample sizep-value
Piccirillo et al.43QTd rate corrected55.03.06935.00.149<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd70.021.01643.010.015<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd rate corrected75.023.01646.010.015<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd50.610.048333.059.03193<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd rate corrected54.1210.698334.79.5193<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd52.627.632527.5314.2925<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd rate corrected55.7927.982529.114.2425<0.001
Atmaca et al.47QTd62.1528.134036.5313.6940<0.001
Study nameSubgroup within studyAnxiety mean (ms)Anxiety standard deviation (ms)Anxiety sample sizeControl mean (ms)Control standard deviation (ms)Control sample sizep-value
Piccirillo et al.43QTd rate corrected55.03.06935.00.149<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd70.021.01643.010.015<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd rate corrected75.023.01646.010.015<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd50.610.048333.059.03193<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd rate corrected54.1210.698334.79.5193<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd52.627.632527.5314.2925<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd rate corrected55.7927.982529.114.2425<0.001
Atmaca et al.47QTd62.1528.134036.5313.6940<0.001

QTd: QT dispersion.

Table 2.

Raw findings from the studies included in the meta-analysis

Study nameSubgroup within studyAnxiety mean (ms)Anxiety standard deviation (ms)Anxiety sample sizeControl mean (ms)Control standard deviation (ms)Control sample sizep-value
Piccirillo et al.43QTd rate corrected55.03.06935.00.149<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd70.021.01643.010.015<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd rate corrected75.023.01646.010.015<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd50.610.048333.059.03193<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd rate corrected54.1210.698334.79.5193<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd52.627.632527.5314.2925<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd rate corrected55.7927.982529.114.2425<0.001
Atmaca et al.47QTd62.1528.134036.5313.6940<0.001
Study nameSubgroup within studyAnxiety mean (ms)Anxiety standard deviation (ms)Anxiety sample sizeControl mean (ms)Control standard deviation (ms)Control sample sizep-value
Piccirillo et al.43QTd rate corrected55.03.06935.00.149<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd70.021.01643.010.015<0.001
Nahshoni et al.44QTd rate corrected75.023.01646.010.015<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd50.610.048333.059.03193<0.001
Uyarel et al.45QTd rate corrected54.1210.698334.79.5193<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd52.627.632527.5314.2925<0.001
Kwon et al.46QTd rate corrected55.7927.982529.114.2425<0.001
Atmaca et al.47QTd62.1528.134036.5313.6940<0.001

QTd: QT dispersion.

Crude QTd values were available in 164 patients characterized as having a high anxiety level and in 273 controls, and a summary of the meta-analysis of the crude QTd estimates is provided in Figure 1. In all the studies included in the analysis, the values of QTd were statistically significantly higher in the patients characterized as having high anxiety. Aggregated standardized difference in the means was equal to 1.472 (95% CI: 1.034–1.911; SE = 0.224, p < 0.001).

Meta-analysis of QT dispersion (QTd) values.
Figure 1.

Meta-analysis of QT dispersion (QTd) values.

CI: confidence interval; Std diff: standardized difference.

Rate-corrected QTd values were available in 193 patients with a high anxiety level and in 282 controls (Figure 1). Aggregated standardized difference in the means was equal to 3.299 (95% CI: 1.215–5.384; SE = 1.064, p = 0.002).

Discussion

The study aimed to conduct a meta-analysis of the available publications on possible association between anxiety and QTd in physically healthy subjects. The principal findings were that, across the retrieved relevant publications, the patients characterized as high anxiety had increased QTd. In all these studies, the values of crude and rate-corrected QTds in the high-anxiety groups exceeded 50 ms, which is known as the upper normal limit.11 The estimated values of the aggregate standardized effect sizes (standardized differences between two means) for the crude and rate-corrected QTds may be characterized as large (as has been determined, the large effect sizes correspond to Cohen’s d values ≥0.848). The point-estimate of the aggregate standardized effect size for the rate-corrected QTd was higher than for the crude one. However, the values of the 95% CIs around the point estimates of these two characteristics were largely overlapping, therefore, both characteristics would seem to be informative. This is in accord with the opinion of some authors who have expressed their scepticism about the need to correct QT for the heart rate.49,50

The effect of anxiety on cardiac functions in healthy persons has been discussed over the past two decades.34,51 However, only in limited investigations of anxiety disorders have cardiac functions been investigated. Meanwhile, it is well-established that excessive anxiety itself affects cardiac autonomic balance in physically healthy subjects and increases the risk of coronary heart disease.44 A striking example of this comes from a prospective study with a large sample in which the risk of sudden cardiac death in men was significantly associated with anxiety.52 The use of QTd in psychiatry was first reported in the context of electroconvulsive therapy, in order to depict patient vulnerability to arrhythmic events immediately after treatment.53 It has been found that physically healthy subjects with major depression have significantly higher values of QTd than normal controls.54 These results support the hypothesis of reduced vagal modulation in major depression as a cause of sudden death in affected patients. Autonomic modulation in hypertensive subjects with anxiety was assessed, and both QTd and the indexes of heart-rate variability which point to a cardiac autonomic imbalance were found to be higher in the subjects with higher anxiety scores and correlated significantly with severity of anxiety.55 Studies revealed that elevated anxiety in physically healthy subjects was associated with a cardiac autonomic imbalance and increased risk of coronary heart disease.51,56 The authors also found that increased QTd was positively correlated with increased sympathetic and/or decreased vagal modulations.57 It has been shown that QTd is a significant predictor of cardiovascular mortality.58 Parasympathetic hypoactivity and sympathetic hyperactivity have also been detected in studies of depression and panic disorder,59 and decreased parasympathetic activity has been shown in studies of anorexia nervosa.60 Depression and negative emotions, such as worry, anger, hostility and sadness, are strongly associated with the neurohormonal imbalance related to the pathogenesis of the cardiac arrhythmias, and these conditions are considered both primary and secondary risk factors.16 Stress itself can cause changes such as increased cardiac sympathetic activity and decreased parasympathetic activity, making the myocardium prone to arrhythmias, and may be responsible for some of the autonomic changes seen in these patients,61 including paediatric groups.62 An elevated variability of the QT interval and elevated norepinephrine spill-over were found in patients with panic disorder and depression.63 These findings are corroborated by the results of this present meta-analysis.

Several limitations of the study should be acknowledged. First, the association between the QTd and the true dispersion of repolarization has been disputed. However, despite the difficulties with the concept and despite the fact that a clear publication bias exists towards positive findings, the large number of studies showing some meaningful results with QTd measurements cannot be dismissed lightly.64 Another limitation comes from the fact that measurement of the QT interval and its dispersion is not standardized. The studies are affected by fundamental limitations of the use of QTd measurements. The accuracy of the QT interval and dispersion measurements have been limited by difficulties with reliable identification of T-wave offset.7

This study has its strengths. As any meta-analysis, it was based on systematic reviews of the literature, allowing the conclusive synthesis of accumulating scientific evidence. Combining data from the independent studies enabled the analysis to encompass 580 subjects, which improved statistical precision. Validity was ensured by the high methodological quality of the included studies and strict eligibility criteria.

Conclusion

With all limitations in mind and with understanding that further studies in the field are desirable, the findings from this meta-analysis are indicative that high anxiety is associated with increased QTd, which may predispose to cardiac arrhythmias.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None declared.

References

1

Saksena
S
,
Camm
AJ
.
Electrophysiological disorders of the heart
, 2nd ed.
Philadelphia, PA
:
Elsevier/Saunders
,
2012
, pp.
1458
1458
.

2

Issa
ZF
,
Miller
JM
,
Zipes
DP
.
Clinical arrhythmology and electrophysiology: A companion to Braunwald's heart disease
, 2nd ed.
Philadelphia, PA
:
Elsevier/Saunders
,
2012
, pp.
502
502
.

3

Siscovick
DS
,
Raghunathan
TE
,
Rautaharju
P
et al. .
Clinically silent electrocardiographic abnormalities and risk of primary cardiac arrest among hypertensive patients
.
Circulation
 
1996
;
94
:
1329
1333
.

4

Day
CP
,
McComb
JM
,
Campbell
RW
.
QT dispersion: An indication of arrhythmia risk in patients with long QT intervals
.
Br Heart J
 
1990
;
63
:
342
344
.

5

Malik
M
,
Batchvarov
VN
.
Measurement, interpretation and clinical potential of QT dispersion
.
J Am Coll Cardiol
 
2000
;
36
:
1749
1766
.

6

Kuo
CS
,
Munakata
K
,
Reddy
CP
et al. .
Characteristics and possible mechanism of ventricular arrhythmia dependent on the dispersion of action potential durations
.
Circulation
 
1983
;
67
:
1356
1367
.

7

Zabel
M
,
Portnoy
S
,
Franz
MR
.
Electrocardiographic indexes of dispersion of ventricular repolarization: An isolated heart validation study
.
J Am Coll Cardiol
 
1995
;
25
:
746
752
.

8

Higham
PD
,
Hilton
DJ
,
Aitcheson
JD
.
QT dispersion: A measure of underlying dispersion of ventricular recovery?
 
Eur Heart J
 
1993
;
14
:
86
86
.

9

Zabel
M
,
Lichtlen
PR
,
Haverich
A
et al. .
Comparison of ECG variables of dispersion of ventricular repolarization with direct myocardial repolarization measurements in the human heart
.
J Cardiovasc Electrophysiol
 
1998
;
9
:
1279
1284
.

10

Surawicz
B
.
Will QT dispersion play a role in clinical decision-making?
 
J Cardiovasc Electrophysiol
 
1996
;
7
:
777
784
.

11

Statters
DJ
,
Malik
M
,
Ward
DE
et al. .
QT dispersion: Problems of methodology and clinical significance
.
J Cardiovasc Electrophysiol
 
1994
;
5
:
672
685
.

12

Galinier
M
,
Balanescu
S
,
Fourcade
J
et al. .
Prognostic value of ventricular arrhythmias in systemic hypertension
.
J Hypertens
 
1997
;
15
:
1779
1783
.

13

Cin
VG
,
Celik
M
,
Ulucan
S
.
QT dispersion ratio in patients with unstable angina pectoris (a new risk factor?)
.
Clin Cardiol
 
1997
;
20
:
533
535
.

14

Barr
CS
,
Naas
A
,
Freeman
M
et al. .
QT dispersion and sudden unexpected death in chronic heart failure
.
Lancet
 
1994
;
343
:
327
329
.

15

Galinier
M
,
Vialette
JC
,
Fourcade
J
et al. .
QT interval dispersion as a predictor of arrhythmic events in congestive heart failure. Importance of aetiology
.
Eur Heart J
 
1998
;
19
:
1054
1062
.

16

Sánchez Bonomo
AM
,
Cavalcanti Ferreira de Araujo
TC
 
Psychological approach to the cardiac arrhythmias: Focus on the emotions
. In:
Breijo-Marquez
FR
(eds).
Cardiac arrhythmias – new considerations
,
Rijeka, Croatia
:
InTech
,
2012
, pp.
307
324
.

17

Lampert
R
,
Joska
T
,
Burg
MM
et al. .
Emotional and physical precipitants of ventricular arrhythmia
.
Circulation
 
2002
;
106
:
1800
1805
.

18

Barlow
DH
.
Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic
, 2nd ed.
New York
:
Guilford Press
,
2002
, pp.
704
704
.

19

DiTomasso
RA
,
Gosch
EA
.
Comparative treatments for anxiety disorders
,
New York
:
Springer
,
2002
, pp.
344
344
.

20

American Psychiatric Association
.
Anxiety disorders. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
, 4th ed.
Washington, DC
:
American Psychiatric Association
,
1994
, pp.
435
436
.

21

Argyropoulos
SV
,
Bell
CJ
,
Nutt
DJ
.
Brain function in social anxiety disorder
.
Psychiatr Clin North Am
 
2001
;
24
:
707
722
.

22

Cornwell
BR
,
Johnson
L
,
Berardi
L
et al. .
Anticipation of public speaking in virtual reality reveals a relationship between trait social anxiety and startle reactivity
.
Biol Psychiatry
 
2006
;
59
:
664
666
.

23

Piccirillo
G
,
Elvira
S
,
Bucca
C
et al. .
Abnormal passive head-up tilt test in subjects with symptoms of anxiety power spectral analysis study of heart rate and blood pressure
.
Int J Cardiol
 
1997
;
60
:
121
131
.

24

Laude
D
,
Girard
A
,
Consoli
S
et al. .
Anger expression and cardiovascular reactivity to mental stress: A spectral analysis approach
.
Clin Exp Hypertens
 
1997
;
19
:
901
911
.

25

Hayward
C
.
Psychiatric illness and cardiovascular disease risk
.
Epidemiol Rev
 
1995
;
17
:
129
138
.

26

Yayou
K
,
Annen
Y
,
Kuwahara
M
et al. .
Studies on the autonomic nervous function in Tsukuba emotional rats – with special reference to their cardiac function
.
J Vet Med Sci
 
1993
;
55
:
1017
1023
.

27

Friedman
BH
,
Thayer
JF
,
Borkovec
TD
et al. .
Autonomic characteristics of nonclinical panic and blood phobia
.
Biol Psychiatry
 
1993
;
34
:
298
310
.

28

Beauchaine
T
.
Vagal tone, development, and Gray's motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology
.
Dev Psychopathol
 
2001
;
13
:
183
214
.

29

Sevy
S
,
Papadimitriou
GN
,
Surmont
DW
et al. .
Noradrenergic function in generalized anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, and healthy subjects
.
Biol Psychiatry
 
1989
;
25
:
141
152
.

30

Garvey
MJ
,
Noyes
R
Jr
,
Woodman
C
et al. .
The association of urinary 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid and vanillylmandelic acid in patients with generalized anxiety
.
Neuropsychobiology
 
1995
;
31
:
6
9
.

31

Bystritsky
A
,
Craske
M
,
Maidenberg
E
et al. .
Autonomic reactivity of panic patients during a CO2 inhalation procedure
.
Depress Anxiety
 
2000
;
11
:
15
26
.

32

Gorman
JM
,
Sloan
RP
.
Heart rate variability in depressive and anxiety disorders
.
Am Heart J
 
2000
;
140
:
77
83
.

33

Albert
CM
,
Chae
CU
,
Rexrode
KM
et al. .
Phobic anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease and sudden cardiac death among women
.
Circulation
 
2005
;
111
:
480
487
.

34

Kawachi
I
,
Sparrow
D
,
Vokonas
PS
et al. .
Symptoms of anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease. The Normative Aging Study
.
Circulation
 
1994
;
90
:
2225
2229
.

35

Kawachi
I
,
Colditz
GA
,
Ascherio
A
et al. .
Prospective study of phobic anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease in men
.
Circulation
 
1994
;
89
:
1992
1997
.

36

Schwartz
PJ
.
Idiopathic long QT syndrome: Progress and questions
.
Am Heart J
 
1985
;
109
:
399
411
.

37

Kubzansky
LD
,
Kawachi
I
,
Spiro
A
, 3rd
et al. .
Is worrying bad for your heart? A prospective study of worry and coronary heart disease in the Normative Aging Study
.
Circulation
 
1997
;
95
:
818
824
.

38

Damen
NL
,
Pelle
AJ
,
Boersma
E
et al. .
Reduced positive affect (anhedonia) is independently associated with 7-year mortality in patients treated with percutaneous coronary intervention: Results from the RESEARCH registry
.
Eur J Prev Cardiol
 
2013
;
20
:
127
134
.

39

Hedges
LV
,
Olkin
I
.
Statistical methods for meta-analysis
,
Orlando, FL
:
Academic Press
,
1985
, pp.
369
369
.

40

Borenstein
M
.
Introduction to meta-analysis
,
Chichester, UK
:
John Wiley & Sons
,
2009
, pp.
421
421
.

41

Borenstein
M
,
Hedges
L
,
Higgins
J
et al. .
Comprehensive meta-analysis version 2
,
Englewood, NJ
:
Biostat
,
2005
, pp.
104
104
.

42

Piccirillo
G
,
Viola
E
,
Bucca
C
et al. .
QT interval dispersion and autonomic modulation in subjects with anxiety
.
J Lab Clin Med
 
1999
;
133
:
461
468
.

43

Piccirillo
G
,
Cacciafesta
M
,
Lionetti
M
et al. .
Influence of age, the autonomic nervous system and anxiety on QT-interval variability
.
Clin Sci (Lond)
 
2001
;
101
:
429
438
.

44

Nahshoni
E
,
Gur
S
,
Marom
S
et al. .
QT dispersion in patients with social phobia
.
J Affect Disord
 
2004
;
78
:
21
26
.

45

Uyarel
H
,
Okmen
E
,
Cobanoglu
N
et al. .
Effects of anxiety on QT dispersion in healthy young men
.
Acta Cardiol
 
2006
;
61
:
83
87
.

46

Kwon
YD
,
Lim
SW
,
Oh
KS
.
Higher QT dispersion in patients with Social Anxiety Disorder
.
Korean J Biol Psychiatry
 
2010
;
17
:
44
48
.

47

Atmaca
M
,
Yavuzkir
M
,
İzci
F
et al. .
QT wave dispersion in patients with panic disorder
.
Neurosci Bull
 
2012
;
28
:
247
252
.

48

Cohen
J
.
Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
, 2nd ed.
Hillsdale, NJ
:
L Erlbaum Associates
,
1988
, pp.
567
567
.

49

Kautzner
J
,
Yi
G
,
Camm
AJ
et al. .
Short- and long-term reproducibility of QT, QTc, and QT dispersion measurement in healthy subjects
.
Pacing Clin Electrophysiol
 
1994
;
17
:
928
937
.

50

Malik
M
,
Camm
AJ
.
Mystery of QTc interval dispersion
.
Am J Cardiol
 
1997
;
79
:
785
787
.

51

Kawachi
I
,
Sparrow
D
,
Vokonas
PS
et al. .
Decreased heart rate variability in men with phobic anxiety (data from the Normative Aging Study)
.
Am J Cardiol
 
1995
;
75
:
882
885
.

52

Morris
A
,
Baker
B
,
Devins
GM
et al. .
Prevalence of panic disorder in cardiac outpatients
.
Can J Psychiatry
 
1997
;
42
:
185
190
.

53

Guler
N
,
Bilge
M
,
Eryonucu
B
et al. .
The effect of electroconvulsive therapy on QT dispersion
.
Acta Cardiol
 
1998
;
53
:
355
358
.

54

Nahshoni
E
,
Aizenberg
D
,
Strasberg
B
et al. .
QT dispersion in the surface electrocardiogram in elderly patients with major depression
.
J Affect Disord
 
2000
;
60
:
197
200
.

55

Piccirillo
G
,
Viola
E
,
Nocco
M
et al. .
Autonomic modulation and QT interval dispersion in hypertensive subjects with anxiety
.
Hypertension
 
1999
;
34
:
242
246
.

56

Kawachi
I
,
Colditz
GA
,
Ascherio
A
et al. .
Prospective study of phobic anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease in men
.
Circulation
 
1994
;
89
:
1992
1997
.

57

Ishida
S
,
Nakagawa
M
,
Fujino
T
et al. .
Circadian variation of QT interval dispersion: Correlation with heart rate variability
.
J Electrocardiol
 
1997
;
30
:
205
210
.

58

Okin
PM
,
Devereux
RB
,
Howard
BV
et al. .
Assessment of QT interval and QT dispersion for prediction of all-cause and cardiovascular mortality in American Indians (The Strong Heart Study)
.
Circulation
 
2000
;
101
:
61
66
.

59

Yeragani
VK
,
Pohl
R
,
Jampala
VC
et al. .
Increased QT variability in patients with panic disorder and depression
.
Psychiatry Res
 
2000
;
93
:
225
235
.

60

Rechlin
T
,
Weis
M
,
Ott
C
et al. .
Alterations of autonomic cardiac control in anorexia nervosa
.
Biol Psychiatry
 
1998
;
43
:
358
363
.

61

Yeragani
VK
,
Tancer
M
,
Uhde
T
.
Heart rate and QT interval variability: Abnormal alpha-2 adrenergic function in patients with panic disorder
.
Psychiatry Res
 
2003
;
121
:
185
196
.

62

Yeragani
VK
,
Rao
KA
,
Pohl
R
et al. .
Heart rate and QT variability in children with anxiety disorders: A preliminary report
.
Depress Anxiety
 
2001
;
13
:
72
77
.

63

Baumert
M
,
Lambert
GW
,
Dawood
T
et al. .
QT interval variability and cardiac norepinephrine spillover in patients with depression and panic disorder
.
Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol
 
2008
;
295
:
H962
H968
.

64

Malik
M
,
Bigger
JT
,
Camm
AJ
et al. .
Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology
.
Eur Heart J
 
1996
;
17
:
354
381
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.