Abstract

The study of brain-function relationships is undergoing a conceptual and methodological transformation due to the emergence of network neuroscience and the development of multivariate methods for lesion-deficit inferences. Anticipating this process, in 1998 Godefroy and co-workers conceptualized the potential of four elementary typologies of brain-behaviour relationships named ‘brain modes’ (unicity, equivalence, association, summation) as building blocks able to describe the association between intact or lesioned brain regions and cognitive processes or neurological deficits. In the light of new multivariate lesion inference and network approaches, we critically revisit and update the original theoretical notion of brain modes, and provide real-life clinical examples that support their existence. To improve the characterization of elementary units of brain-behavioural relationships further, we extend such conceptualization with a fifth brain mode (mutual inhibition/masking summation). We critically assess the ability of these five brain modes to account for any type of brain-function relationship, and discuss past versus future contributions in redefining the anatomical basis of human cognition. We also address the potential of brain modes for predicting the behavioural consequences of lesions and their future role in the design of cognitive neurorehabilitation therapies.

Introduction

One of the most important endeavours inspiring neuroscientists has been the characterization of the functional neuroanatomy of the human brain. In this mission, the advent of neuroimaging proved paramount for supporting the exploration of brain-behaviour relationships, which for the past 20 years has been providing powerful insights concerning the anatomical bases of normal and pathological cognition. As a result of this effort, the study of brain-function relationships has experienced profound transformations. Conceptually, the adoption of a connectional (hodological) systems perspective has derived a formulation of cognition subserved by hierarchically organized interactive brain networks (Bassett et al., 2018). Methodologically, the development of novel computational approaches has begun the characterization of complex interaction patterns subtended by structural and functional brain networks and addressed their roles in cognition and behaviour. Additionally, this novel perspective has instigated the compilation of brain maps, integrating information on interactive properties operating dynamically across complex hierarchical neural systems.

Anticipating this transformation, pioneering work published 20 years ago in Brain (Godefroy et al., 1998) conceptualized the regularities characterizing different classes of elementary associations between brain structures and their ability to substantiate specific human behaviours (or the lack thereof) as ‘brain modes’.

Classical modes of brain-behavioural relationships

In the original paper, brain modes were defined as pre-established sets of functional interactions between cerebral regions contributing to the emergence of a neurological symptom. These entities were conceived as the means to unveil the brain’s functional organization. Each mode was considered with regards to specific cognitive processes or behaviours. Anticipating the importance of a connectional perspective, this notion and the framework it generated must now be considered among the first attempts to conceptualize neurological symptoms as emerging from interactions between multiple anatomical structures organized in networks (Geschwind, 1965; Catani and Ffytche, 2005; Bartolomeo, 2011), as opposed to models emphasizing individual regional contributions.

Originally, four classes of brain modes: unicity, equivalence, association and summation, were intended to capture the full array of potential brain-behaviour relationships (Fig. 1 and Table 1). The unicity mode defined the simplest brain-function association in which a single function was linked to a single injured brain structure. The equivalence mode, the most frequently observed in clinical practice, epitomized a situation in which damage to two separate structures provoked in both cases a similar behavioural deficit. The association mode described the scenario involving two brain regions in which both had to be damaged in order to generate a neurological deficit. Finally, the summation mode characterized an interaction type in which lesions in two or more brain regions resulted in specific behavioural or clinical deficits; nonetheless, when these same structures were all simultaneously damaged, deficits proved greater than the sum of the individual lesion effects.

Table 1

‘Brain modes’ (Godefroy et al., 1998) and their equivalents in a multivariate approach (game-theory MSA).

Types of ‘brain modes’ originally reported by Godefroy et al. (1998) (modes 1–4) and an additional 5th brain mode (5)Examples of ‘brain modes’ from lesion cases in specific regions and behavioural domains (see Godefroy et al., 1998 for applications to post-stroke motor weakness)Game-theory MSA equivalents to originally reported ‘brain modes’Application of ‘brain modes’ to interactive networks subtending the cognitive neuroanatomy of visuospatial attention and post-stroke hemineglect (Toba et al., 2017)
1. Unicity ‘brain mode’ A behavioural deficit is always linked to damage of a single brain structureNot documented (it might be an oversimplification of the motor system)Only a single brain structure in a given coalition of players is the contributor to a functionPlausible, but not documented in this study
2. Equivalencebrain mode’ A behavioural deficit or a given level of deficit appears after damage of one, among several brain structuresDocumented theoretically and clinically in aphasia, memory deficits, motor function and its deficits (Alexander et al., 1987; Mesulam, 1990; Kremin, 1994; Kreisler et al., 2000; Godefroy et al., 2009; Martinaud et al., 2009; Arnoux et al., 2018; Toba et al., 2018a, b)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present some degree of functional overlapPossible, but not documented in this study
3. Associationbrain mode’ A deficit is present only when two brain structures are both damaged simultaneouslyDocumented theoretically but remained to be better documented clinically: Balint syndrome, reduplicative paramnesia, confabulations, global aphasia, dysexecutive functions (Shallice, 1988; Godefroy et al., 1992; Wolfe et al., 1994; Caplan, 1995)Several brain structures are positive contributors and present a joint functional contributionPossible, but not documented in this study
4. Summationbrain mode’ A severe deficit appears when several structures are simultaneously damaged. A moderate deficit appears when only one individual region is damagedDocumented both theoretically and clinically in language (Kreisler et al., 2000; Seghier et al., 2014)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present redundant and synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Positive contributors: BA7/IPS, BA6/FEF, BA39-40/TPJ, BA44-45/ IFG Synergistic interactions: BA7/IPS - BA39/TPJ, BA7/IPS -BA19/IOG, BA39/TPJ - BA19/IOG; BA39/TPJ - BA45/IFG, BA39/TPJ - BA40/TPJ Redundant interactions: BA44-45/IFG - BA7/IPS, BA7/IPS - BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF - BA7/IPS; BA6/FEF - BA19/IOG; BA19/IOG - BA40/TPJ
5. Mutual inhibition/masking summationbrain mode’ A second lesion produces paradoxical behavioural improvement from a deficit generated by an earlier lesionNot discussed in the original paper by Godefroy et al., (1998), but documented clinically and theoretically (Sprague, 1966; Vuilleumier et al., 1996; Weddell et al., 2004; Zavaglia and Hilgetag, 2016)Several brain structures are negative contributors to a given function and they subtend synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Negative contributors: BA44-45/IFG, BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF
Types of ‘brain modes’ originally reported by Godefroy et al. (1998) (modes 1–4) and an additional 5th brain mode (5)Examples of ‘brain modes’ from lesion cases in specific regions and behavioural domains (see Godefroy et al., 1998 for applications to post-stroke motor weakness)Game-theory MSA equivalents to originally reported ‘brain modes’Application of ‘brain modes’ to interactive networks subtending the cognitive neuroanatomy of visuospatial attention and post-stroke hemineglect (Toba et al., 2017)
1. Unicity ‘brain mode’ A behavioural deficit is always linked to damage of a single brain structureNot documented (it might be an oversimplification of the motor system)Only a single brain structure in a given coalition of players is the contributor to a functionPlausible, but not documented in this study
2. Equivalencebrain mode’ A behavioural deficit or a given level of deficit appears after damage of one, among several brain structuresDocumented theoretically and clinically in aphasia, memory deficits, motor function and its deficits (Alexander et al., 1987; Mesulam, 1990; Kremin, 1994; Kreisler et al., 2000; Godefroy et al., 2009; Martinaud et al., 2009; Arnoux et al., 2018; Toba et al., 2018a, b)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present some degree of functional overlapPossible, but not documented in this study
3. Associationbrain mode’ A deficit is present only when two brain structures are both damaged simultaneouslyDocumented theoretically but remained to be better documented clinically: Balint syndrome, reduplicative paramnesia, confabulations, global aphasia, dysexecutive functions (Shallice, 1988; Godefroy et al., 1992; Wolfe et al., 1994; Caplan, 1995)Several brain structures are positive contributors and present a joint functional contributionPossible, but not documented in this study
4. Summationbrain mode’ A severe deficit appears when several structures are simultaneously damaged. A moderate deficit appears when only one individual region is damagedDocumented both theoretically and clinically in language (Kreisler et al., 2000; Seghier et al., 2014)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present redundant and synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Positive contributors: BA7/IPS, BA6/FEF, BA39-40/TPJ, BA44-45/ IFG Synergistic interactions: BA7/IPS - BA39/TPJ, BA7/IPS -BA19/IOG, BA39/TPJ - BA19/IOG; BA39/TPJ - BA45/IFG, BA39/TPJ - BA40/TPJ Redundant interactions: BA44-45/IFG - BA7/IPS, BA7/IPS - BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF - BA7/IPS; BA6/FEF - BA19/IOG; BA19/IOG - BA40/TPJ
5. Mutual inhibition/masking summationbrain mode’ A second lesion produces paradoxical behavioural improvement from a deficit generated by an earlier lesionNot discussed in the original paper by Godefroy et al., (1998), but documented clinically and theoretically (Sprague, 1966; Vuilleumier et al., 1996; Weddell et al., 2004; Zavaglia and Hilgetag, 2016)Several brain structures are negative contributors to a given function and they subtend synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Negative contributors: BA44-45/IFG, BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF

Explanations of ‘Brain modes’ types are illustrated with studies presenting lesions on specific systems and the application of Toba et al. (2017) on visuospatial attention obtained with the multiperturbation Shapley value analysis (MSA) method (Keinan et al., 2004). Specifically, the MSA approach builds on the analysis of isolated and combined regional functional contributions to a clinical deficit from a series of players (brain structures), which make part of a complex coalition (network or system). This approach allows the characterization of both positive and negative contributions of a given brain structure, which might either facilitate or hinder a selected behaviour, respectively. MSA also reveals redundant interactions (between areas with functional overlap) and synergistic interactions (between areas presenting complementary functions). Recently published studies concerning the use of MSA in post-stroke lesion data have revealed plausible evidence in favour of a causal implication of different grey and white matter structures to specific cognition domains, illustrating the potential of this approach in clinical datasets (Zavaglia et al., 2015, 2019; Toba et al., 2017). BA = Brodmann area; IPS = intraparietal sulcus; FEF = frontal eye field; IOG = inferior occipital gyrus; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; TPJ = temporo-parietal junction.

Table 1

‘Brain modes’ (Godefroy et al., 1998) and their equivalents in a multivariate approach (game-theory MSA).

Types of ‘brain modes’ originally reported by Godefroy et al. (1998) (modes 1–4) and an additional 5th brain mode (5)Examples of ‘brain modes’ from lesion cases in specific regions and behavioural domains (see Godefroy et al., 1998 for applications to post-stroke motor weakness)Game-theory MSA equivalents to originally reported ‘brain modes’Application of ‘brain modes’ to interactive networks subtending the cognitive neuroanatomy of visuospatial attention and post-stroke hemineglect (Toba et al., 2017)
1. Unicity ‘brain mode’ A behavioural deficit is always linked to damage of a single brain structureNot documented (it might be an oversimplification of the motor system)Only a single brain structure in a given coalition of players is the contributor to a functionPlausible, but not documented in this study
2. Equivalencebrain mode’ A behavioural deficit or a given level of deficit appears after damage of one, among several brain structuresDocumented theoretically and clinically in aphasia, memory deficits, motor function and its deficits (Alexander et al., 1987; Mesulam, 1990; Kremin, 1994; Kreisler et al., 2000; Godefroy et al., 2009; Martinaud et al., 2009; Arnoux et al., 2018; Toba et al., 2018a, b)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present some degree of functional overlapPossible, but not documented in this study
3. Associationbrain mode’ A deficit is present only when two brain structures are both damaged simultaneouslyDocumented theoretically but remained to be better documented clinically: Balint syndrome, reduplicative paramnesia, confabulations, global aphasia, dysexecutive functions (Shallice, 1988; Godefroy et al., 1992; Wolfe et al., 1994; Caplan, 1995)Several brain structures are positive contributors and present a joint functional contributionPossible, but not documented in this study
4. Summationbrain mode’ A severe deficit appears when several structures are simultaneously damaged. A moderate deficit appears when only one individual region is damagedDocumented both theoretically and clinically in language (Kreisler et al., 2000; Seghier et al., 2014)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present redundant and synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Positive contributors: BA7/IPS, BA6/FEF, BA39-40/TPJ, BA44-45/ IFG Synergistic interactions: BA7/IPS - BA39/TPJ, BA7/IPS -BA19/IOG, BA39/TPJ - BA19/IOG; BA39/TPJ - BA45/IFG, BA39/TPJ - BA40/TPJ Redundant interactions: BA44-45/IFG - BA7/IPS, BA7/IPS - BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF - BA7/IPS; BA6/FEF - BA19/IOG; BA19/IOG - BA40/TPJ
5. Mutual inhibition/masking summationbrain mode’ A second lesion produces paradoxical behavioural improvement from a deficit generated by an earlier lesionNot discussed in the original paper by Godefroy et al., (1998), but documented clinically and theoretically (Sprague, 1966; Vuilleumier et al., 1996; Weddell et al., 2004; Zavaglia and Hilgetag, 2016)Several brain structures are negative contributors to a given function and they subtend synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Negative contributors: BA44-45/IFG, BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF
Types of ‘brain modes’ originally reported by Godefroy et al. (1998) (modes 1–4) and an additional 5th brain mode (5)Examples of ‘brain modes’ from lesion cases in specific regions and behavioural domains (see Godefroy et al., 1998 for applications to post-stroke motor weakness)Game-theory MSA equivalents to originally reported ‘brain modes’Application of ‘brain modes’ to interactive networks subtending the cognitive neuroanatomy of visuospatial attention and post-stroke hemineglect (Toba et al., 2017)
1. Unicity ‘brain mode’ A behavioural deficit is always linked to damage of a single brain structureNot documented (it might be an oversimplification of the motor system)Only a single brain structure in a given coalition of players is the contributor to a functionPlausible, but not documented in this study
2. Equivalencebrain mode’ A behavioural deficit or a given level of deficit appears after damage of one, among several brain structuresDocumented theoretically and clinically in aphasia, memory deficits, motor function and its deficits (Alexander et al., 1987; Mesulam, 1990; Kremin, 1994; Kreisler et al., 2000; Godefroy et al., 2009; Martinaud et al., 2009; Arnoux et al., 2018; Toba et al., 2018a, b)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present some degree of functional overlapPossible, but not documented in this study
3. Associationbrain mode’ A deficit is present only when two brain structures are both damaged simultaneouslyDocumented theoretically but remained to be better documented clinically: Balint syndrome, reduplicative paramnesia, confabulations, global aphasia, dysexecutive functions (Shallice, 1988; Godefroy et al., 1992; Wolfe et al., 1994; Caplan, 1995)Several brain structures are positive contributors and present a joint functional contributionPossible, but not documented in this study
4. Summationbrain mode’ A severe deficit appears when several structures are simultaneously damaged. A moderate deficit appears when only one individual region is damagedDocumented both theoretically and clinically in language (Kreisler et al., 2000; Seghier et al., 2014)Several brain structures are positive contributors to the function and present redundant and synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Positive contributors: BA7/IPS, BA6/FEF, BA39-40/TPJ, BA44-45/ IFG Synergistic interactions: BA7/IPS - BA39/TPJ, BA7/IPS -BA19/IOG, BA39/TPJ - BA19/IOG; BA39/TPJ - BA45/IFG, BA39/TPJ - BA40/TPJ Redundant interactions: BA44-45/IFG - BA7/IPS, BA7/IPS - BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF - BA7/IPS; BA6/FEF - BA19/IOG; BA19/IOG - BA40/TPJ
5. Mutual inhibition/masking summationbrain mode’ A second lesion produces paradoxical behavioural improvement from a deficit generated by an earlier lesionNot discussed in the original paper by Godefroy et al., (1998), but documented clinically and theoretically (Sprague, 1966; Vuilleumier et al., 1996; Weddell et al., 2004; Zavaglia and Hilgetag, 2016)Several brain structures are negative contributors to a given function and they subtend synergistic interactionsConfirmed with MSA approaches: Negative contributors: BA44-45/IFG, BA19/IOG, BA6/FEF

Explanations of ‘Brain modes’ types are illustrated with studies presenting lesions on specific systems and the application of Toba et al. (2017) on visuospatial attention obtained with the multiperturbation Shapley value analysis (MSA) method (Keinan et al., 2004). Specifically, the MSA approach builds on the analysis of isolated and combined regional functional contributions to a clinical deficit from a series of players (brain structures), which make part of a complex coalition (network or system). This approach allows the characterization of both positive and negative contributions of a given brain structure, which might either facilitate or hinder a selected behaviour, respectively. MSA also reveals redundant interactions (between areas with functional overlap) and synergistic interactions (between areas presenting complementary functions). Recently published studies concerning the use of MSA in post-stroke lesion data have revealed plausible evidence in favour of a causal implication of different grey and white matter structures to specific cognition domains, illustrating the potential of this approach in clinical datasets (Zavaglia et al., 2015, 2019; Toba et al., 2017). BA = Brodmann area; IPS = intraparietal sulcus; FEF = frontal eye field; IOG = inferior occipital gyrus; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; TPJ = temporo-parietal junction.

Brain modes. Schematic illustration of the brain modes proposed by Godefroy et al. (1998) and expanded here (unicity, equivalence, association, summation, mutual inhibition/masking summation) emerging from different patterns of interactions between two hypothetical cerebral nodes (A and B), here characterized with regards to their final impact on behavioural performance. Histograms display the hypothetical effect on behavioural performance, presented as the percentage of correct responses in a given task, depending on whether nodes A and B are intact or lesioned. (1) In the unicity mode, the behavioural deficit is linked to the lesion of a single brain region, hence 100% performance is obtained solely when node A is intact, irrespective of whether node B is intact or damaged. (2) In the equivalence mode, the behavioural deficit is the same if either one of two brain regions is lesioned; accordingly, 100% performance is obtained only if both nodes, A and B, remain intact. (3) In the association mode, the behavioural deficit is observed only when two or more brain regions are simultaneously damaged; therefore 100% of performance occurs when node A or node B are intact, but not when both are lesioned. (4) In the summation mode, a severe behavioural deficit is observed only when two or more brain regions are simultaneously damaged, whereas a moderate deficit is observed when only one individual region is selectively damaged; therefore, 100% performance occurs only when both nodes A and B are intact; however, when either node A or node B are lesioned there is a moderate deficit; whereas when both nodes A and B are damaged, then the deficit becomes severe. Error bars represent possible standard deviation values. (5) In the mutual inhibition/masking summation mode, the behavioural deficit appears when one among several regions is lesioned; whereas paradoxical behavioural recovery from a deficit occurs when both regions are jointly damaged; in this scenario, 100% correct performance occurs when both A and B nodes are intact, or paradoxically, also when both node A and node B are lesioned.
Figure 1

Brain modes. Schematic illustration of the brain modes proposed by Godefroy et al. (1998) and expanded here (unicity, equivalence, association, summation, mutual inhibition/masking summation) emerging from different patterns of interactions between two hypothetical cerebral nodes (A and B), here characterized with regards to their final impact on behavioural performance. Histograms display the hypothetical effect on behavioural performance, presented as the percentage of correct responses in a given task, depending on whether nodes A and B are intact or lesioned. (1) In the unicity mode, the behavioural deficit is linked to the lesion of a single brain region, hence 100% performance is obtained solely when node A is intact, irrespective of whether node B is intact or damaged. (2) In the equivalence mode, the behavioural deficit is the same if either one of two brain regions is lesioned; accordingly, 100% performance is obtained only if both nodes, A and B, remain intact. (3) In the association mode, the behavioural deficit is observed only when two or more brain regions are simultaneously damaged; therefore 100% of performance occurs when node A or node B are intact, but not when both are lesioned. (4) In the summation mode, a severe behavioural deficit is observed only when two or more brain regions are simultaneously damaged, whereas a moderate deficit is observed when only one individual region is selectively damaged; therefore, 100% performance occurs only when both nodes A and B are intact; however, when either node A or node B are lesioned there is a moderate deficit; whereas when both nodes A and B are damaged, then the deficit becomes severe. Error bars represent possible standard deviation values. (5) In the mutual inhibition/masking summation mode, the behavioural deficit appears when one among several regions is lesioned; whereas paradoxical behavioural recovery from a deficit occurs when both regions are jointly damaged; in this scenario, 100% correct performance occurs when both A and B nodes are intact, or paradoxically, also when both node A and node B are lesioned.

Illustrating ‘modes’ of brain-behavioural relationships with clinical examples

The originally described brain modes can be illustrated by examples of neurological and neuropsychological observations in humans, yet some exceptions and limitations apply.

The simplest brain mode, unicity, could depict the functional contributions of isolated nodes, which are hardly present in the highly and intricately connected mammalian nervous systems. Thus, this mode has been theoretically hypothesized but remains to be documented clinically. The equivalence brain mode has been documented theoretically and also clinically. Indeed, in the original paper describing brain modes, single lesions localized at two different levels along the cortico-spinal tract were characterized as equally responsible for motor weakness (Godefroy et al., 1998; see also Arnoux et al., 2018). Likewise, other associations compatible with this brain mode have been documented as subtending symptoms such as aphasia, memory loss, executive and attentional deficits (Alexander et al., 1987; Mesulam, 1990; Kremin, 1994; Kreisler et al., 2000; Godefroy et al., 2009; Martinaud et al., 2009; Toba et al., 2018a, b). For instance, verbal paraphasias have been linked to either temporal or caudate lesions, whereas non-fluent aphasia depends on the presence of frontal or putamen lesions (Kreisler et al., 2000). The association brain mode has been identified theoretically but remains to be better documented clinically, as it requires rare-to-find patients with selective lesions damaging multiple regions within the same network. This mode was originally illustrated in patients with unilateral lenticulostriate lesions (Godefroy et al., 1992) showing executive function impairment only when, additionally, they suffered an associated cortical infarct. It has also been documented in cases of Balint syndrome, reduplicative paramnesia, confabulations and global aphasia (Shallice, 1988; Wolfe et al., 1994; Caplan, 1995). For example, Wolfe et al. (1994) explored a patient with bilateral subcortical lacunae in the basal ganglia and periventricular white matter, who presented a critical level of dysexecutive syndrome after the occurrence of posterior cortical lesions. Finally, the summation mode has been documented both theoretically and clinically. For example, in language impairments, non-fluent aphasia was associated with lesions of putamen and surrounding structures while mutism was associated with large lesion of the three frontal gyri and putamen (Kreisler et al., 2000; Seghier et al., 2014).

Importantly, confirming the accuracy of naturally occurring lesions to illustrate specific brain modes requires detailed documentation concerning the role of lesion-spared regions, which has not always been available in neurological clinical records, as it requires the detailed whole-brain characterization of structurally and functionally sound versus impaired areas.

Limitations of classical brain mode conceptualizations

As with any pioneering work, the first conceptualization of brain modes suffered from a number of limitations, in part caused by shortcomings in the statistical and computational approaches available at the time they were first characterized. Importantly, many of these limitations can now be overcome with the use of more current methodological approaches. First, original lesion analyses were conducted only on regions of interest corresponding to anatomical structures, or to regions demarcated by brain landmarks such as sulci or gyri, as attempts to decrease region of interest size led to poor interexaminer agreement (Godefroy et al., 1998). In contrast, recent lesion analyses can now be conducted at the voxel level, hence delineated at a much higher spatial resolution, allowing a more precise characterization of brain modes. Second, the past use of structural instead of functional parcellation schemes resulted in regions with mixed functions, providing less informative evidence concerning the behavioural contributions of the analysed regions of interest. This risk can be limited by using parcellation schemes based on both structural and functional MRI (see Glasser et al., 2016 for an example), further verified with perturbational approaches such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) (Toba et al., 2017). Third, the original characterization of brain modes could not distinguish between grey and white matter, a limitation that is especially meaningful for the equivalence brain mode associated with white matter disconnections. Importantly, however, the advent of diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and the adoption of hodological approaches to address brain-behavioural relationships (Boes et al., 2015; Fox, 2018) has provided new insights to overcome this original limitation. Fourth, lesion characterization approaches used in the late 1990s were unable to grade the magnitude of the neural damage, hence ignoring the crucial role of lesion volume threshold effects on neurological deficits. As such information becomes available for other anatomical grey and white matter systems, this important aspect will be better controlled (see Arnoux et al., 2018 for an example of the threshold level of cortico-spinal tract injury generating significantly enduring motor deficits at 6 months). Fifth, multivariate analyses proposed in the original characterization of brain modes were based on one particular type of statistical approach, the so-called Classification and Regression Tree (CART) (Breiman et al., 1984). This method was able to disentangle important factors in terms of explanatory power in a model, and provided naturally the type of logical relationships between selected regions. However, the CART approach requires large datasets and additional statistical analyses to assess the strength of brain-behavioural associations. The latter limitations can now be overcome by current multivariate methods for lesion-deficit mapping providing more appropriate and reliable analysis tools (see below).

New brain modes: the case of mutual inhibition/masking summation

At the time they were first described, brain modes represented a major conceptual advance in the field, aiming to represent any known lesion effects by their four original types. Nonetheless, multivariate CART approaches originally used for their characterization failed to identify ‘paradoxical lesion cancellation’ effects, initially reported in feline models (Sprague, 1966; Payne et al., 1996; Hilgetag et al., 2002) and later extended to human neurological patients (Kinsbourne, 1977, 1987; Vuilleumier et al., 1996; Weddell, 2004; Johnson et al., 2008; Jha and Brown, 2011), known as the ‘Sprague Effect’ (Box 1).

Box 1 The Sprague effect

Historical aspects and definition of the concept

Experimental animal studies in the 1960s were the first to illustrate that interactions between brain regions were unpredictable. In an effort to understand the independent and joint contributions of the visual cortex and the midbrain superior colliculus to visual behaviour, James Sprague subjected either region to lesions, and then sequentially combined the lesions. A surprising result emerged when the longstanding and seemingly intractable deficit in the detection and orienting to right-sided visual targets produced by lesion of left visual cortex was cancelled by ablation of the right superior colliculus (Sprague, 1966). This result was subsequently referred to as the Sprague effect, and was conceived by Sprague and others to be a product of manipulating mutually inhibitory bilateral interactions between brain structures (Sprague, 1966; Lomber and Payne, 1996; Hilgetag et al., 1999; Lomber et al., 2002; Rushmore et al., 2006; Valero-Cabré et al., 2019).

Confirmatory aspects

This experimental work was used in part to formulate mechanistic understanding of visuospatial neglect (Kinsbourne, 1974, 1977, 1987, 1993). Later findings in neurologically intact human participants (Hilgetag et al., 2001) and in human clinical cases (Vuilleumier et al., 1996; Weddell, 2004) provided considerable evidence that the mechanisms underlying the Sprague effect contributed to analogous interactions in humans. Kinsbourne’s ideas and the aforementioned clinical cases exerted major influences in understanding the brain-wide impact of unilateral brain damage in humans, wherein damage was presumed to reduce activity in connected brain regions in the ipsilateral hemisphere and increase activity in the contralateral hemisphere through disruption of inhibitory circuits (Murase et al., 2004; Nowak et al., 2009). Until now, the best evidence of the Sprague effect has been observed at the interhemispheric level.

Consequences in therapy and in the comprehension of the human functional neuroanatomy

This approach guided the later use and interpretation of non-invasive brain stimulation studies, in which focal inhibitory stimulation [including low frequency TMS, continuous theta burst TMS, or cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)] aims to transiently take offline focal cortical regions while recording behavioural performance (Valero-Cabré et al., 2017). This approach has been successfully applied to the intact brain hemisphere in patients with unilateral brain damage (Oliveri et al., 1999; Brighina et al., 2003; Koch et al., 2008; Sparing et al., 2009; Koch et al., 2012; Cazzoli et al., 2012) and proved to restore neurological deficits caused by focal brain lesions, such as hemispatial neglect. All these studies demonstrate that the mutual inhibition/masking summation brain mode has already opened new avenues for therapeutic uses of non-invasive brain stimulation technologies. However, this new brain mode has also opened perspectives of improving visuospatial orienting abilities in healthy humans (Hilgetag et al., 2001). Looking to the future, we should envision that reversible neuromodulation technologies [TMS, repetitive TMS or tDCS/tACS), which are currently helping to investigate the cognitive role of cortical regions embedded in brain networks, could also be used to probe or unearth existing and novel brain modes. Moreover, the use of these same technologies for the modulation of specific site or sites implementing a given brain mode may prove useful to develop novel strategies for the restitution of neurological function.

This phenomenon described the paradoxical improvement of performance deficits caused by a circumscribed lesion thanks to a reversible or permanent suppression of activity in a second brain area interacting with the former (for reviews see Payne and Rushmore, 2004; Valero-Cabré et al., 2019). Inspired by predictions of interhemispheric rivalry in spatial attention in the feline visual system, this brain mode was also characterized in humans using causal reversible perturbation by TMS (Hilgetag et al., 2001). Intending to transiently impair lateralized visual detection performance in one visual hemifield with inhibitory repetitive TMS on the contralateral intraparietal sulcus, these authors found paradoxical improvements for ipsilateral visual targets. This unexpected result revealed that a reversible focal suppression of human brain activity could produce attentional and perceptual gains. Further studies in animals reported paradoxical cancellation of lesion-induced deficits with secondary focal lesions, direct cortical cooling or non-invasive TMS deactivations of spared cortical and subcortical attentional orienting systems (Rushmore et al., 2006; reviewed in Valero-Cabré et al., 2019).

We here propose to name this additional brain mode ‘mutual inhibition’ or alternatively ‘masking summation’ and add it to the palette of existing typologies.

Logic operators and circuit motifs for characterizing brain modes

Since their inception, brain modes were conceived of as elementary building blocks that, adequately combined in complex fashions, could account for any specific neurological deficits. Here we revisit brain modes from the perspectives of logic and propositional calculus as well as graph theory approaches and the concept of circuit ‘motifs’. We suggest that analogies between brain modes and elementary units used especially in the graph theory framework are particularly suited for analysing and extending the existing typologies, and further exploring their implications for cognitive neuroanatomy.

From the perspective of logic, some of the brain modes can be characterized as standard forms of logical relationships between unary and binary factors, and represented with a standard truth table. For example, with respect to a brain function, ‘unicity’ can be associated with identity; ‘equivalence’ may be equated to conjunction (AND); ‘association’ could be associated to disjunction (OR); and ‘mutual inhibition/masking summation’ corresponds to the opposite of an exclusive disjunction (NOT XOR or XNOR). The ‘summation’ brain mode may not be fully captured by binary logical operators as its outcome depends on the graded interactions between the nodes. Of note, other authors have proposed weighted extensions of logical functions that can express partial conjunctions and disjunctions (Dujmović and Larsen, 2007) and which could be used to describe such relations in the context of the summation mode. Generally, in order to characterize the graded effects of brain modes, one might also use arithmetic (rather than logical) operations, which can be extended and turned into actual circuit models capturing the observed effects (cf. Zavaglia and Hilgetag, 2016).

In the graph theory framework, brain modes can be based on different ‘brain motifs’ (Fig. 2). These represent plausible types of interregional relationships with the ability to influence a given brain function. In this regard, the unicity mode would be the equivalent of a node (i.e. brain region) defined as the only contributor to a brain function. Damage of a unique contributor generates a level of behavioural deficit that depends on the magnitude of the impaired contribution and the extent of the lesion. Hence, the unicity mode could be well suited to describe brain-behaviour interactions in which function emerges from single nodes. The equivalence mode is observed when behavioural performance in a given task benefits from multiple contributions by different nodes, which contribute similarly to a behaviour. The association mode represents the case of joint contributions of two (or several) nodes to a given behaviour, in which a critical level of clinical deficit manifests only when both nodes are injured. The summation mode is observed when two conditions apply to a given set of nodes: (i) each node provides a specific contribution to a given behaviour; and (ii) their contributions are both redundant (overlapping) and synergistic (complementary). Last, in the masking summation mode, two nodes interacting with each other contribute to a brain function. Clinical deficits appear present only when one of the two nodes is lesioned; however, symptoms can paradoxically wear off when the two nodes are jointly damaged, by the fact that they are linked by rivalrous mutually inhibitory interactions. Taken together, the equivalence, association, summation and/masking summation modes characterize a hodological framework in which behaviours (or the lack thereof during neurological diseases) emerge from nodes organized in networks. Such interactions can be summarized in a simple two-node circuit model (Fig. 2) that formalizes arithmetically the strength of functional interactions and contributions of the nodes. Importantly, the number and complexity of brain motifs, and the elementary types of brain modes they implement, depends on the number of regions involved. As the number of nodes increases, the complexity of directed network motifs and their contribution patterns escalates (Fig. 2).

From brain modes to brain motifs. (A) Brain motifs. Graphical representations of ‘brain motifs’ defined as plausible connectional patterns (i.e. brain circuits or networks) established by interacting brain regions and their ability to influence a given brain function, denoted as Fn. Brain motifs can be considered as the anatomically or functionally plausible implementation of brain modes as brain circuits or networks. Here we decided to present brain motifs based on combinations of only two, either independent or potentially interacting brain regions, referred to as nodes A and B. Nonetheless, the number and connectional complexity of brain motifs, hence the number and types of brain modes that they implement, can easily scale up when considering interactions between a higher number of regions, as it is often the case in the brain. For example, three and 13 brain motifs define all possible relation types between two or three nodes, respectively. However, a full characterization of networks composed of four nodes will demand 199 motifs, and such numbers would then have to be multiplied by all possible combinations of potential contributions (i.e. three combinations for systems of two nodes: just node ‘A’ or node ‘B’ or both ‘A’ and ‘B’ contributing, seven combinations for three nodes, etc.). Thin black horizontal arrows represent interactions (unidirectional or bidirectional) between brain nodes A and B; larger grey arrows represent contributions (or influences) of nodes A and B to a given brain function, denoted Fn. (1) Top row: Three brain motifs between two independent (non-interacting) nodes in three different scenarios. Left: Only node A contributes to a brain function Fn; middle: two nodes (A and B) contribute to a brain function Fn, hence nodes could present functional overlap; right: only node B contributes to brain function Fn. (2) Top, middle and bottom rows, from left to right: Plausible brain motifs between two interacting nodes according to nine different scenarios. Three different general patterns can characterize interactions between nodes A and B via directed connections to influence function Fn. Top row: Node A projects to node B; middle row: nodes A and B mutually project to each other; bottom row: node B projects to node A. Moreover, these three patterns can be combined with three types of nodal functional outputs to influence function Fn. Top row: Only node A directly contributes to function Fn; middle row: both nodes A and B contribute to function Fn; bottom row: only node B contributes to function Fn. Single black arrows represent a node (A or B) interacting unidirectionally and serially with the other node, to ultimately influence brain function Fn; bidirectional black arrows represent nodes contributing in parallel to ultimately influence brain function Fn. Depending on the type of interaction, the functional contribution provided by nodes A and B could be higher (synergy) than the sum of their individual functional contributions. Joint contributions may not necessarily be the exact sum of individual contributions; indeed, this scenario would be equivalent to the absence of functional interaction between nodes A and B, hence depicted as independent nodes. Notice that for synergistic (complementary) functional contributions, nodes should interact via structural connections. Importantly, several brain motifs represented here may produce similar behavioural outcomes despite different underlying network structures. The full set of directed two-node motifs is represented for symmetry reasons. It is tempting to compare brain modes with brain motifs (particularly for those depicted above involving two brain regions) and search for common features that could substantiate the former as being the same as the latter. In the ‘unicity’ mode, an independent (non-interacting) node A should be the unique contributor to a brain function Fn. In the ‘equivalence’ mode, both nodes A and B should contribute to a brain function Fn without redundancy, for instance, by A contributing to Fn serially through B. In the ‘association’ mode, both nodes A and B should contribute in parallel to brain function Fn. Finally, in the ‘summation’ mode, both nodes should also contribute in parallel to a brain function Fn. Additionally, however, they should interact, redundantly or synergistically. In any case, the mapping between these two notions, brain modes and brain motifs, is not always straightforward and some of the brain motifs presented in this figure and the many that could be theoretically conceived with more than two nodes, cannot be unequivocally characterized in terms of a single ‘brain mode’, especially if one aims to take into account the number and type of functional interactions between the nodes. A possibility to reveal the rich set of functional interactions of some brain motifs consist in removing connections between nodes. In particular, this is possible in computationally implemented ‘ground-truth’ models, by systematically exploring the parametric space of possibilities, and ‘virtually’ damaging nodes and links in all possible ways to then use such evidence to infer the network structure from associated changes in brain function. However, it is more difficult to infer all these different ‘motifs’ directly from changes in brain function after perturbing different nodes and/or the links between them. (B) Algorithmic summary of brain motifs and modes. Summary of the minimal circuit interactions of two brain nodes A and B potentially contributing to a brain function Fn. The equation formalizes the strength of the interactions and functional contributions of the nodes, and appropriate settings of the path coefficients might capture all brain modes and motifs displayed in (B). Specifically, setting all paths except kAF to zero corresponds to unicity; setting kAB and kBA to zero correspond to association; while leaving all coefficients non-zero can yield, with the right parameter settings, equivalence, summation or masked summation.
Figure 2

From brain modes to brain motifs. (A) Brain motifs. Graphical representations of ‘brain motifs’ defined as plausible connectional patterns (i.e. brain circuits or networks) established by interacting brain regions and their ability to influence a given brain function, denoted as Fn. Brain motifs can be considered as the anatomically or functionally plausible implementation of brain modes as brain circuits or networks. Here we decided to present brain motifs based on combinations of only two, either independent or potentially interacting brain regions, referred to as nodes A and B. Nonetheless, the number and connectional complexity of brain motifs, hence the number and types of brain modes that they implement, can easily scale up when considering interactions between a higher number of regions, as it is often the case in the brain. For example, three and 13 brain motifs define all possible relation types between two or three nodes, respectively. However, a full characterization of networks composed of four nodes will demand 199 motifs, and such numbers would then have to be multiplied by all possible combinations of potential contributions (i.e. three combinations for systems of two nodes: just node ‘A’ or node ‘B’ or both ‘A’ and ‘B’ contributing, seven combinations for three nodes, etc.). Thin black horizontal arrows represent interactions (unidirectional or bidirectional) between brain nodes A and B; larger grey arrows represent contributions (or influences) of nodes A and B to a given brain function, denoted Fn. (1) Top row: Three brain motifs between two independent (non-interacting) nodes in three different scenarios. Left: Only node A contributes to a brain function Fn; middle: two nodes (A and B) contribute to a brain function Fn, hence nodes could present functional overlap; right: only node B contributes to brain function Fn. (2) Top, middle and bottom rows, from left to right: Plausible brain motifs between two interacting nodes according to nine different scenarios. Three different general patterns can characterize interactions between nodes A and B via directed connections to influence function Fn. Top row: Node A projects to node B; middle row: nodes A and B mutually project to each other; bottom row: node B projects to node A. Moreover, these three patterns can be combined with three types of nodal functional outputs to influence function Fn. Top row: Only node A directly contributes to function Fn; middle row: both nodes A and B contribute to function Fn; bottom row: only node B contributes to function Fn. Single black arrows represent a node (A or B) interacting unidirectionally and serially with the other node, to ultimately influence brain function Fn; bidirectional black arrows represent nodes contributing in parallel to ultimately influence brain function Fn. Depending on the type of interaction, the functional contribution provided by nodes A and B could be higher (synergy) than the sum of their individual functional contributions. Joint contributions may not necessarily be the exact sum of individual contributions; indeed, this scenario would be equivalent to the absence of functional interaction between nodes A and B, hence depicted as independent nodes. Notice that for synergistic (complementary) functional contributions, nodes should interact via structural connections. Importantly, several brain motifs represented here may produce similar behavioural outcomes despite different underlying network structures. The full set of directed two-node motifs is represented for symmetry reasons. It is tempting to compare brain modes with brain motifs (particularly for those depicted above involving two brain regions) and search for common features that could substantiate the former as being the same as the latter. In the ‘unicity’ mode, an independent (non-interacting) node A should be the unique contributor to a brain function Fn. In the ‘equivalence’ mode, both nodes A and B should contribute to a brain function Fn without redundancy, for instance, by A contributing to Fn serially through B. In the ‘association’ mode, both nodes A and B should contribute in parallel to brain function Fn. Finally, in the ‘summation’ mode, both nodes should also contribute in parallel to a brain function Fn. Additionally, however, they should interact, redundantly or synergistically. In any case, the mapping between these two notions, brain modes and brain motifs, is not always straightforward and some of the brain motifs presented in this figure and the many that could be theoretically conceived with more than two nodes, cannot be unequivocally characterized in terms of a single ‘brain mode’, especially if one aims to take into account the number and type of functional interactions between the nodes. A possibility to reveal the rich set of functional interactions of some brain motifs consist in removing connections between nodes. In particular, this is possible in computationally implemented ‘ground-truth’ models, by systematically exploring the parametric space of possibilities, and ‘virtually’ damaging nodes and links in all possible ways to then use such evidence to infer the network structure from associated changes in brain function. However, it is more difficult to infer all these different ‘motifs’ directly from changes in brain function after perturbing different nodes and/or the links between them. (B) Algorithmic summary of brain motifs and modes. Summary of the minimal circuit interactions of two brain nodes A and B potentially contributing to a brain function Fn. The equation formalizes the strength of the interactions and functional contributions of the nodes, and appropriate settings of the path coefficients might capture all brain modes and motifs displayed in (B). Specifically, setting all paths except kAF to zero corresponds to unicity; setting kAB and kBA to zero correspond to association; while leaving all coefficients non-zero can yield, with the right parameter settings, equivalence, summation or masked summation.

To conclude, the generalization and combination of brain-behaviour relationships by means of propositional calculus allows a partial description of the brain modes space, but does not facilitate the modelling of non-binary relationships, which grows in complexity as the number of interacting areas increases. In this context, other frameworks, such as graph theory, should be explored to better characterize these entities and further extend the variety of their typologies.

Revisiting brain modes from a multivariate analysis perspective

Multivariate modelling integrating different anatomical variables and considering the presence or absence of damage to multiple brain regions simultaneously may help to extend the analysis of brain modes (Godefroy et al., 1992, 1998; Keinan et al., 2004; Chen et al., 2008; Chen and Herskovits, 2010; Smith et al., 2013; Mah et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2014; Rondina et al., 2016; Yourganov et al., 2016; Toba et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2018). These methods, which are committed to replace univariate approaches, are now widely used in the field, and may allow a better characterization of the canonical brain modes while helping to describe new typologies (see Table 1 showing applications of a multivariate method to explore brain-behaviour relationships and Box 2 for critical issues concerning formalization of lesion parameters in multivariate modelling) (Bzdok et al., 2017; Price et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2018). Moreover, the use of brain modes as sets of elementary types of associations on brain-function relationships would constrain multivariate approaches increasing their ability to deliver reliable outcomes. However, promoting a rigorous use of multivariate methods for such endeavours requires raising awareness of some critical issues impacting on their practical implementation.

Box 2 Critical issues to be considered for the formalization of lesion parameters to characterize brain-behaviour relationships with multivariate models
  • Ponder carefully the risk of characterizing regions of interest as being either totally lesioned or completely spared by using fixed binarized damage thresholds.

  • Employ a conservative lesion parametrization strategy, particularly when operating at the voxel level; this is a particularly sensitive issue for perilesional areas in the absence of functional MRI data informing on tissue viability.

  • Implement whenever possible a transnosological approach, which compares complementary lesion datasets from distinct neurological conditions characterized by different symptoms.

  • Prioritize the use of continuous over discrete or binarized variables when estimating the magnitude of behavioural performance or quantifying the severity of clinical scores.

  • Consider two potential scenarios with regards to the localization of neuronal clusters (voxels) responsible for a given function (or the lack thereof):

    • A cognitive process enabled during a clinical task is subserved in a large majority of studied individuals by neuronal clusters localized within the same brain region/regions.

    • The localization and magnitude of activations linked to a cognitive process enabled during a clinical task presents as highly variable across studied individuals, and is subserved by neuronal clusters localized in very different brain region/regions.

For example, across the same cohort of individuals, neuronal clusters (brain regions) activated during the access to word meaning are more consistently located and activated in magnitude than those required to access the meaning of specific words, which may highly differ across individuals (Binder et al., 2009; Lambon Ralph et al., 2017).

First, defining the ‘hypothesis space’ is an essential step for making accurate inferences and predictions of brain-behaviour relationships in the context of complex models such as those introduced by multivariate methods. Recently published attempts have used hypothesis-based approaches pre-selecting a few sets of regions, composing a coalition of contributors, which can then be tested together as part of the same model (Toba et al., 2017). Nonetheless, more advanced methodological avenues use bias-free identification strategies, that can test sets of regional contributors, grouped as part of specific coalitions, without a priori assumptions (Zavaglia et al., 2019).

Considering the large spectrum of multivariate methods, Bayesian approaches based on a priori defined hypotheses have been useful to narrow down the number of relevant brain sites included in multivariate models (Chen and Herskovits, 2010; Duering et al., 2013, 2014; Arnoux et al. 2018). Nonetheless, hypothesis-free identification methods based on machine learning algorithms play a major role in formal hypothesis and model generation (Xu et al., 2018). For example, in the equivalence brain mode, when multiple lesion sites cause the same impairment, machine learning tools can generate predictive multivariate models, incorporating simultaneously information from multiple voxels without the need of a priori hypotheses (Hope et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2013; Price et al., 2017). Interestingly, this same approach can be used to identify key variables and generate predictive models, contributing to the identification and classification of new brain modes. Moreover, by integrating structural/functional neuroimaging, neurophysiological recordings and non-invasive brain stimulation, multivariate machine learning tools will reach full potential and drive a better understanding of functional anatomy (Price et al., 2017). Nonetheless, these approaches are also costly in terms of computational time and need of independent training datasets. Moreover, machine learning algorithms operating as ‘black boxes’ often run into serious risks of inferring irrelevant relations due to overfitting. Last but not least, to date, even the most sophisticated multivariate machine learning lesion analysis methods are unable to disambiguate the independent effects of different lesion sites in absence of patient datasets showing such dissociations.

Second, artificial ground-truth models (Mah et al., 2014) may help overcome some of the former limitations, and may become the ‘gold standard’ method to characterize and validate past and future brain mode typologies. Indeed, the original conceptualization of brain modes was tested via simulated populations of patients, and later confirmed in datasets from real-life stroke patients with motor disabilities. Different ground truth models able to manipulate the variability of anatomical patterns have been proposed (Inoue et al., 2014; Mah et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2014; Sperber and Karnath, 2017; Pustina et al., 2018). In particular, computationally implemented ground-truth synthetic models have the potential to reveal complex functional interactions between brain regions and explore the parametric space of possibilities by removing, with graded levels of severity in all possible ways, ‘virtual’ nodes and/or connections between sets of chosen regions of interest. Such evidence can then be used to infer network structure from associated changes in brain function. Last but not least, these same methods can prove useful for lesion-deficit anatomical mapping and ultimately for clinical predictions.

Future directions

Two decades after its first theorization in 1998, the concept of brain modes remains a guiding framework in which to better characterize brain-behaviour associations and study their implementation in plausible structural and functional networks. Significant conceptual and methodological advances in multivariate analysis place the systems neuroscience community in an excellent position to reframe the inventory of the brain modes and critically evaluate the exhaustiveness of these relationships to define a comprehensive set of brain-behaviour associations. In particular, current brain mode typologies could be refined by comparing predictions made by computational lesion studies implementing a theoretical model (based on simulations of reliable structural and functional interactions among brain regions) with empirical results from brain lesion datasets. Moreover, as we look to the future, we can anticipate the need to apply brain modes to personalized clinical rehabilitation programmes (Nachev et al., 2018). More specifically, data on brain modes hold the potential to inform computational modelling and web-based platforms able to simulate large-scale networks using biologically plausible structural and functional connectivities (Sanz Leon et al., 2013). This type of environment (for example in The Virtual Brain; thevirtualbrain.org) allows the construction of personalized configurations of structural and functional systems, and the study of brain dynamics under the influence of focal and network perturbations or disorders, simulating, for example, epilepsy or the cognitive consequences of stroke (Falcon et al., 2015). They may provide predictive tools to investigate the impact of brain structural alterations on neural dynamics (Alstott et al., 2009; Aerts et al., 2016), study the remapping of function-specific brain modes and contribute to the planning of better strategies for cognitive rehabilitation or neurosurgical interventions. We look forward to the realization of the potential of the brain modes approach over the next 20 years.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Daniel Margulies and two anonymous reviewers for enriching comments and suggestions on previous versions of the manuscript.

Funding

Groundwork leading to this paper has been funded in part by past EU project eraNET-NEURON 2009 “BEYONDVIS” to C.C.H. and A.V-C, as well as grants from the German Research Council DGF HI 1286/3-1, SFB 936/A1, Z3 and TRR 169/A2 to C.C.H. It is also supported by research grants by the IHU-A-ICM-Translationnel, ANR projet Générique “OSCILOSCOPUS”, ANR project Flag Era-JTC-HBP “CAUSALTOMICS”, PHRC Regional “NEGLECT”, PHRC National “STIM-SD” to A.V-C., and travel funds by the Naturalia & Biologia Foundation; grants from Amiens University Hospital (France) and the French Department of Health (France; reference: DGOS R1/2013/144) to O.G.; and NIH R01MH112748.

Competing interests

In the past 5 years, O.G. has served on scientific advisory boards and as a speaker (Novartis, CSL-Behring, Biogen, Genzyme, Lilly, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Covidien, Teva and Astra Zeneca), and has received funding for travel and meetings from Novartis, Lilly, Genzyme, Astrazeneca, Biogen, Teva, Pfizer, CSL-Behring, GSK, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Ipsen, Covidien and Bristol-Myers Squibb.

References

Aerts
H
,
Fias
W
,
Caeyenberghs
K
,
Marinazzo
D
.
Brain networks under attack: robustness properties and the impact of lesions
.
Brain
2016
;
139
:
3063
83
.

Alexander
MP
,
Naeser
MA
,
Palumbo
CL
.
Correlations of subcortical CT lesion sites and aphasia profiles
.
Brain
1987
;
110
:
961
91
.

Alstott
J
,
Breakspear
M
,
Hagmann
P
,
Cammoun
L
,
Sporns
O
.
Modeling the impact of lesions in the human brain
.
PLoS Comput Biol
2009
;
5
:
e1000408
.

Arnoux
A
,
Toba
MN
,
Duering
M
,
Diouf
M
,
Daouk
J
,
Constans
J-M
, et al.
Is VLSM a valid tool for determining the functional anatomy of the brain? Usefulness of additional Bayesian network analysis
.
Neuropsychologia
2018
;
12
:
69
78
.

Bartolomeo
P
.
The quest for the ‘critical lesion site’ in cognitive deficits: problems and perspectives
.
Cortex
2011
;
47
:
1010
2
.

Bassett
DS
,
Zurn
P
,
Gold
JI
.
On the nature and use of models in network neuroscience
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
2018
;
19
:
566
78
.

Bates
E
,
Wilson
SM
,
Saygin
AP
,
Dick
F
,
Sereno
MI
,
Knight
RT
et al.
Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping
.
Nat Neurosci
2003
;
6
:
448
50
.

Binder
JR
,
Desai
RH
,
Graves
WW
,
Conant
LL
.
Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies
.
Cereb Cortex
2009
;
19
:
2767
96
.

Boes
AD
,
Prasad
S
,
Liu
H
,
Liu
Q
,
Pascual-Leone
A
,
Caviness
VS
Jr
,
Fox
MD
.
Network localization of neurological symptoms from focal brain lesions
.
Brain
2015
;
138
:
3061
75
.

Breiman
L
,
Friedman
JH
,
Olshen
RA
,
Stone
CJ
, editors.
Classification and regression trees
.
Belmont, CA
:
Wadsworth
;
1984
.

Brighina
F
,
Bisiach
E
,
Oliveri
M
,
Piazza
A
,
La Bua
V
,
Daniele
O
, et al.
1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere ameliorates contralesional visuospatial neglect in humans
.
Neurosci Lett
2003
;
336
:
131
3
.

Bzdok
D
,
Yeo
BTT
.
Inference in the age of big data: Future perspectives on neuroscience
.
Neuroimage
2017
;
155
:
549
64
.

Caplan
LR
. Visual perceptual abnormalities (cerebral). In:
Bogousslavsky
J
,
Caplan
LR
, editors.
Stroke syndromes
.
Cambridge
:
Cambridge University Press
;
1995
. p.
57
67
.

Catani
M
,
Ffytche
DH
.
The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes
.
Brain
2005
;
128
:
2224
39
.

Cazzoli
D
,
Muri
RM
,
Schumacher
R
,
von Arx
S
,
Chaves
S
,
Gutbrod
K
, et al.
Theta burst stimulation reduces disability during the activities of daily living in spatial neglect
.
Brain
2012
;
135
:
3426
39
.

Chen
R
,
Herskovits
EH
.
Voxel-based Bayesian lesion-symptom mapping
.
Neuroimage
2010
;
49
:
597
602
.

Chen
R
,
Hillis
AE
,
Pawlak
M
,
Herskovits
EH
.
Voxelwise Bayesian lesion deficit analysis
.
Neuroimage
2008
;
40
:
1633
42
.

Duering
M
,
Gesierich
B
,
Seiler
S
,
Pirpamer
L
,
Gonik
M
,
Hofer
E
, et al.
Strategic white matter tracts for processing speed deficits in age-related small vessel disease
.
Neurology
2014
;
82
:
1946
50
.

Duering
M
,
Gonik
M
,
Malik
R
,
Zieren
N
,
Reyes
S
,
Jouvent
E
, et al.
Identification of a strategic brain network underlying processing speed deficits in vascular cognitive impairment
.
NeuroImage
2013
;
66
:
177
83
.

Dujmović
JJ
,
Larsen
HL
.
Generalized conjunction/disjunction
.
Int J Approx Reason
2007
;
46
:
423
46
.

Falcon
MI
,
Riley
JD
,
Jirsa
V
,
McIntosh
AR
,
Shereen
AD
,
Chen
EE
, et al.
The virtual brain: modeling biological correlates of recovery after chronic stroke
.
Front Neurol
2015
;
6
:
228
.

Fox
MD
.
Mapping symptoms to brain networks with the human connectome
.
N Engl J Med
2018
;
379
:
2237
45
.

Geschwind
N
.
Disconnection syndromes in animals and man
.
Brain
1965
;
88
:
585
644
.

Glasser
MF
,
Coalson
TS
,
Robinson
EC
,
Hacker
CD
,
Harwell
J
,
Yacoub
E
, et al.
A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex
.
Nature
2016
;
536
:
171
8
.

Godefroy
O
,
Duhamel
A
,
Leclerc
X
,
Saint Michel
T
,
Henon
H
,
Leys
D
.
Brain behaviour relationships. Some models and related statistical procedures for the study of brain-damaged patients
.
Brain
1998
;
121
:
1545
56
.

Godefroy
O
,
Rousseaux
M
,
Leys
D
,
Destee
A
,
Scheltens
P
,
Pruvo
JP
.
Frontal lobe dysfunction in unilateral lenticulostriate infarcts
.
Arch Neurol
1992
;
49
:
1285
9
.

Godefroy
O
,
Roussel
M
,
Leclerc
X
,
Leys
D
.
Deficit of episodic memory: anatomy and related patterns in stroke patients
.
Eur Neurol
2009
;
61
:
223
9
.

Hilgetag
CC
,
Kötter
R
,
Young
MP
.
Inter-hemispheric competition of sub-cortical structures is a crucial mechanism in paradoxical lesion effects and spatial neglect
.
Prog Brain Res
1999
;
121
:
121
41
.

Hilgetag
CC
,
Lomber
S
,
Rushmore
R
,
Payne
B
.
Topographic restoration of visual spatial attention in the cortically blind cat
.
Neurocomputing
2002
;
44
:
831
5
.

Hilgetag
CC
,
Theoret
H
,
Pascual-Leone
A
.
Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced ‘virtual lesions’ of human parietal cortex
.
Nat Neurosci
2001
;
4
:
953
7
.

Hope
TM
,
Seghier
ML
,
Leff
AP
,
Price
CJ
.
Predicting outcome and recovery after stroke with lesions extracted from MRI images
.
Neuroimage Clin
2013
;
2
:
424
33
.

Inoue
K
,
Madhyastha
T
,
Rudrauf
D
,
Mehta
S
,
Grabowski
T
.
What affects detectability of lesion–deficit relationships in lesion studies?
NeuroImage Clin
2014
;
6
:
388
97
.

Jha
A
,
Brown
P
. Paradoxes in Parkinson's disease and other movement disorders. In:
Kapur
N
, editors.
The paradoxical brain
.
Cambridge University Press
;
2011
. p.
189
203
.

Johnson
MD
,
Miocinovic
S
,
Mc Intyre
CC
,
Vitek
JL
.
Mechanisms and targets of deep brain stimulation in movement disorders
.
Neurotherapeutics
2008
;
5
:
294
308
.

Keinan
A
,
Sandbank
B
,
Hilgetag
CC
,
Meilijson
I
,
Ruppin
E
.
Fair attribution of functional contribution in artificial and biological networks
.
Neural Comput
2004
;
16
:
1887
915
.

Kinsbourne
M
. Lateral interactions in the brain. In:
Kinsbourne
M
,
Smith
WL
, editors.
Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function
.
Oxford
:
Charles C Thomas
;
1974
. p.
239
59
.

Kinsbourne
M
.
Hemi-neglect and hemisphere rivalry
.
Adv Neurol
1977
;
18
:
41
9
.

Kinsbourne
M
.
Mechanisms of unilateral neglect
.
Adv Psychol
1987
;
45
:
69
87
.

Kinsbourne
M
. Orientation bias model of unilateral neglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In
Robertson
IH
,
Marshall
JC
, editors.
Brain damage, behavior & cognition series
. Unilateral neglect: clinical and experimental studies.
Hillsdale
:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
;
1993
. p.
63
86
.

Koch
G
,
Bonnì
S
,
Giacobbe
V
,
Bucchi
G
,
Basile
B
,
Lupo
F
, et al.
Theta-burst stimulation of the left hemisphere accelerates recovery of hemispatial neglect
.
Neurology
2012
;
78
:
24
30
.

Koch
G
,
Oliveri
M
,
Cheeran
B
,
Ruge
D
,
Lo Gerfo
E
,
Salerno
S
, et al.
Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect
.
Brain
2008
;
131
:
3147
55
.

Kreisler
A
,
Godefroy
O
,
Delmaire
C
,
Debachy
B
,
Leclercq
M
,
Pruvo
JP
, et al.
The anatomy of aphasia revisited
.
Neurology
2000
;
54
:
1117
23
.

Kremin
H
. Perturbations lexicales: les troubles de la denomination. In:
Seron
X
,
Jeannerod
M
, editors.
Neuropsychologie humaine
.
Liege
:
Mardaga
;
1994
. p.
375
407
.

Lambon Ralph
MA
,
Jefferies
E
,
Patterson
K
,
Rogers
TT
.
The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
2017
;
18
:
42
55
.

Lomber
SG
,
Payne
BR
.
Removal of two halves restores the whole: reversal of visual hemineglect during bilateral cortical or collicular inactivation in the cat
.
Vis Neurosci
1996
;
13
:
1143
56
.

Lomber
SG
,
Payne
BR
,
Hilgetag
CC
,
Rushmore
J
.
Restoration of visual orienting into a cortically blind hemifield by reversible deactivation of posterior parietal cortex or the superior colliculus
.
Exp Brain Res
2002
;
142
:
463
74
.

Mah
YH
,
Husain
M
,
Rees
G
,
Nachev
P
.
Human brain lesion-deficit inference remapped
.
Brain
2014
;
137
:
2522
31
.

Martinaud
O
,
Perin
B
,
Gérardin
E
,
Proust
F
,
Bioux
S
,
Gars
DL
, et al.
Anatomy of executive deficit following ruptured anterior communicating artery aneurysm
.
Eur J Neurol
2009
.
16
:
595
601
.

Mesulam
MM
.
Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language and memory
.
Ann Neurol
1990
;
28
:
597
613
.

Murase
N
,
Duque
J
,
Mazzocchio
R
,
Cohen
LG
.
Influence of interhemispheric interactions on motor function in chronic stroke
.
Ann Neurol
2004
;
55
:
400
9
.

Nachev
P
,
Rees
G
,
Frackowiak
R
.
Lost in translation
.
F1000Res
2018
;
7
:
620
.

Nowak
DA
,
Grefkes
C
,
Ameli
M
,
Fink
GR
.
Interhemispheric competition after stroke: brain stimulation to enhance recovery of function of the affected hand
.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair
2009
;
23
:
641
56
.

Oliveri
M
,
Rossini
PM
,
Traversa
R
,
Cicinelli
P
,
Filippi
MM
,
Pasqualetti
P
, et al.
Left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces contralesional extinction in patients with unilateral right brain damage
.
Brain
1999
;
122
:
1731
9
.

Payne
BR
,
Lomber
SG
,
Rushmore
RJ
,
Pascual-Leone
A
.
Cancellation of visuoparietal lesion-induced spatial neglect
.
Exp Brain Res
2003
;
150
:
395
8
.

Payne
BR
,
Lomber
SG
,
Villa
AE
,
Bullier
J
.
Reversible deactivation of cerebral network components
.
Trends Neurosci
1996
;
19
:
535
42
.

Payne
BR
,
Rushmore
RJ
.
Functional circuitry underlying natural and interventional cancellation of visual neglect
.
Exp Brain Res
2004
;
154
:
127
53
.

Price
CJ
,
Hope
TM
,
Seghier
ML
.
Ten problems and solutions when predicting individual outcome from lesion site after stroke
.
Neuroimage
2017
.
145
:
200
8
.

Pustina
D
,
Avants
B
,
Faseyitan
OK
,
Medaglia
JD
,
Coslett
HB
.
Improved accuracy of lesion to symptom mapping with multivariate sparse canonical correlations
.
Neuropsychologia
2018
;
115
:
154
66
.

Rondina
JM
,
Filippone
M
,
Girolami
M
,
Ward
NS
.
Decoding post-stroke motor function from structural brain imaging
.
NeuroImage Clin
2016
;
12
:
372
80
.

Rushmore
RJ
,
Valero-Cabré
A
,
Lomber
SG
,
Hilgetag
CC
,
Payne
BR
.
Functional circuitry underlying visual neglect
.
Brain
2006
;
129
:
1803
21
.

Sanz Leon
P
,
Knock
SA
,
Woodman
MM
,
Domide
L
,
Mersmann
J
,
McIntosh
, et al.
The virtual brain: a simulator of primate brain network dynamics
.
Front Neuroinform
2013
;
7
:
10
.

Seghier
ML
,
Bagdasaryan
J
,
Jung
DE
,
Price
CJ
.
The importance of premotor cortex for supporting speech production after left capsular-putaminal damage
.
J Neurosci
2014
;
34
:
14338
48
.

Shallice
T
.
From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge
:
Cambridge University Press
;
1988
.

Smith
DV
,
Clithero
JA
,
Rorden
C
,
Karnath
HO
.
Decoding the anatomical network of spatial attention
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci
2013
;
110
:
1518
23
.

Sparing
R
,
Thimm
M
,
Hesse
MD
,
Ku
J
,
Karbe
H
,
Fink
GR
.
Bidirectional alterations of interhemispheric parietal balance by non-invasive cortical stimulation
.
Brain
2009
;
132
:
3011
20
.

Sperber
C
,
Karnath
H-O
.
Impact of correction factors in human brain lesion behavior inference
.
Hum Brain Mapp
2017
.
38
:
1692
1701
.

Sporns
O
.
Networks of the brain
.
Cambridge
:
MIT Press
;
2011
.

Sprague
JM
.
Interaction of cortex and superior colliculus in mediation of visually guided behavior in the cat
.
Science
1966
;
153
:
1544
7
.

Toba
MN
,
Migliaccio
R
,
Batrancourt
B
,
Bourlon
C
,
Duret
C
,
Pradat-Diehl
P
, et al.
Common brain networks for distinct deficits in visual neglect. A combined structural and tractography MRI approach
.
Neuropsychologia
2018a
.
115
:
167
178
.

Toba
MN
,
Rabuffetti
M
,
Duret
C
,
Pradat-Diehl
P
,
Gainotti
G
,
Bartolomeo
P
.
Component deficits of visual neglect: “Magnetic” attraction of attention vs. impaired spatial working memory
.
Neuropsychologia
2018b
;
109
:
52
62
.

Toba
MN
,
Zavaglia
M
,
Rastelli
F
,
Valabregue
R
,
Pradat-Diehl
P
,
Valero-Cabré
, et al.
Game theoretical mapping of causal interactions underlying visuo-spatial attention in the human brain based on stroke lesions
.
Hum Brain Mapp
2017
;
38
:
3454
71
.

Valero-Cabré
A
,
Toba
MN
,
Hilgetag
CC
,
Rushmore
RJ
.
Perturbation-driven paradoxical facilitation of visuo-spatial function: revisiting the ‘Sprague effect
’.
Cortex
2019
, doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.01.031.

Valero-Cabré
A
,
Amengual
JL
,
Stengel
C
,
Pascual-Leone
A
,
Coubard
OA
.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation in basic and clinical neuroscience: a comprehensive review of fundamental principles and novel insights
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
2017
;
83
:
381
404
.

Verdon
V
,
Schwartz
S
,
Lovblad
KO
,
Hauert
CA
,
Vuilleumier
P
.
Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping
.
Brain
2010
;
133
:
880
94
.

Vuilleumier
P
,
Hester
D
,
Assal
G
,
Regli
F
.
Unilateral spatial neglect recovery after sequential strokes
.
Neurology
1996
;
46
:
184
9
.

Weddell
RA
.
Subcortical modulation of spatial attention including evidence that the Sprague effect extends to man
.
Brain Cogn
2004
;
55
:
497
506
.

Wolfe
N
,
Babikian
VL
,
Linn
RT
,
Knoefel
JE
,
D’Esposito
M
,
Albert
ML
.
Are multiple cerebral infarcts synergistic?
Arch Neurol
1994
;
51
:
211
5
.

Xu
T
,
Jha
A
,
Nachev
P
.
The dimensionalities of lesion-deficit mapping
.
Neuropsychologia
2018
;
115
:
134
41
.

Yourganov
G
,
Fridriksson
J
,
Rorden
C
,
Gleichgerrcht
E
,
Bonilha
L
.
Multivariate connectome-based symptom mapping in post-stroke patients: networks supporting language and speech
.
J Neurosci
2016
;
36
:
6668
79
.

Zavaglia
M
,
Forkert
ND
,
Cheng
B
,
Gerloff
C
,
Thomalla
G
,
Hilgetag
CC
.
Mapping causal functional contributions derived from the clinical assessment of brain damage after stroke
.
Neuroimage Clin
2015
;
9
:
83
94
.

Zavaglia
M
,
Hilgetag
CC
.
Causal functional contributions and interactions in the attention network of the brain: an objective multi-perturbation analysis
.
Brain Struct Funct
2016
;
221
:
2553
68
.

Zavaglia
M
,
Toba
MN
,
Moreau
T
,
Rastelli
F
,
Kaglik
A
,
Valabregue
R
, et al. Hodological correlates of human visuo-spatial attention and its disorders based on coalitional game theory-derived contributions of white matter bundles.
Poster presented at: CNS 2019; Cognitive Neuroscience Society
2019
.
San Francisco
.

Zhang
Y
,
Kimberg
DY
,
Coslett
HB
,
Schwartz
MF
,
Wang
Z
.
Multivariate lesion-symptom mapping using support vector regression
.
Hum Brain Mapp
2014
;
35
:
5861
76
.

Author notes

Claus C. Hilgetag and Antoni Valero-Cabré contributed equally to this work.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)