Abstract

Impulsivity is common in Parkinson’s disease even in the absence of impulse control disorders. It is likely to be multifactorial, including a dopaminergic ‘overdose’ and structural changes in the frontostriatal circuits for motor control. In addition, we proposed that changes in serotonergic projections to the forebrain also contribute to response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease, based on preclinical animal and human studies. We therefore examined whether the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram improves response inhibition, in terms of both behaviour and the efficiency of underlying neural mechanisms. This multimodal magnetic resonance imaging study used a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled crossover design with an integrated Stop-Signal and NoGo paradigm. Twenty-one patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (46–76 years old, 11 male, Hoehn and Yahr stage 1.5–3) received 30 mg citalopram or placebo in addition to their usual dopaminergic medication in two separate sessions. Twenty matched healthy control subjects (54–74 years old, 12 male) were tested without medication. The effects of disease and drug on behavioural performance and regional brain activity were analysed using general linear models. In addition, anatomical connectivity was examined using diffusion tensor imaging and tract-based spatial statistics. We confirmed that Parkinson’s disease caused impairment in response inhibition, with longer Stop-Signal Reaction Time and more NoGo errors under placebo compared with controls, without affecting Go reaction times. This was associated with less stop-specific activation in the right inferior frontal cortex, but no significant difference in NoGo-related activation. Although there was no beneficial main effect of citalopram, it reduced Stop-Signal Reaction Time and NoGo errors, and enhanced inferior frontal activation, in patients with relatively more severe disease (higher Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale motor score). The behavioural effect correlated with the citalopram-induced enhancement of prefrontal activation and the strength of preserved structural connectivity between the frontal and striatal regions. In conclusion, the behavioural effect of citalopram on response inhibition depends on individual differences in prefrontal cortical activation and frontostriatal connectivity. The correlation between disease severity and the effect of citalopram on response inhibition may be due to the progressive loss of forebrain serotonergic projections. These results contribute to a broader understanding of the critical roles of serotonin in regulating cognitive and behavioural control, as well as new strategies for patient stratification in clinical trials of serotonergic treatments in Parkinson’s disease.

Introduction

The problem of impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease is highlighted by the severity of impulse control disorders in 13.6% of patients (Weintraub et al., 2010). Impulsive actions (e.g. unsuccessful response inhibition, premature response) and choices (e.g. reward/risk-based decisions) exist even in the absence of impulse control disorders (Voon et al., 2011a; Bentivoqlio et al., 2012; Nombela et al., 2014) and are likely to be multifactorial. This study focuses on the serotonergic mechanisms of response inhibition, rather than the dopaminergic mechanisms of choice impulsivity.

In addition to dopaminergic ‘overdose’ (Rowe et al., 2008; Voon et al., 2011b) and structural changes in the frontostriatal circuits for motor control and decision-making (Rae et al., 2012), we propose that changes in serotonergic projections to the forebrain also exacerbate the impairment of response inhibition (Politis et al., 2010, 2012). Enhancing central serotonin transmission with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) might therefore provide an adjunctive treatment for motor impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. Citalopram is a SSRI that increases the extracellular levels of serotonin in the prefrontal cortex ∼4-fold, but not levels of noradrenaline or dopamine (Bymaster et al., 2002). We therefore investigated whether citalopram can improve response inhibition and enhance its neural systems in Parkinson’s disease.

We studied two forms of response inhibition: NoGo and Stop-Signal inhibition. NoGo inhibition requires action selection and restraint mechanisms for the prevention of a prepotent response, whereas Stop-Signal tasks invoke the cancellation of an initiated response and induce neuronal inhibition of motor actions. There is strong preclinical evidence from animal and human studies that serotonin plays an important role in regulating action restraint. For example, in rats and humans, enhancing cortical serotonin transmission (e.g. using the SSRI citalopram or serotonin receptor agonists) reduces impulsive responses (Homberg et al., 2007) and increases NoGo activations in the right inferior frontal gyrus (Anderson et al., 2002; Del-Ben et al., 2005; Völlm et al., 2006; Macoveanu et al., 2013). Serotonin efflux is also enhanced naturally in the dorsal raphe nucleus, which projects to the forebrain, when rats perform a task that requires waiting and action restraint (Miyazaki et al., 2011). In contrast, depleting central serotonin (e.g. by acute tryptophan depletion or a serotonin receptor antagonist M100907) increases premature responding (Fletcher et al., 2007; Robinson et al., 2008), impairs NoGo inhibition and right inferior frontal gyrus activation (Harrison et al., 1999; Rubia et al., 2005). In addition, several clinical disorders are associated with both impulsivity and reduction or deregulation of serotonin transmission, including Parkinson’s disease, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Zepf et al., 2008), frontotemporal dementia (Huey et al., 2006), Tourette’s syndrome (Haugbøl et al., 2007) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Chamberlain et al., 2005).

In contrast, the link between serotonin and action cancellation is less well established. Animal and human studies have often not shown an effect of serotonin on Stop-Signal tasks (as measured by the Stop-Signal reaction time, SSRT) (Clark et al., 2005; Chamberlain et al., 2006, 2007b; Bari et al., 2009; Eagle et al., 2009; Drueke et al., 2010; Nandam et al., 2011). However, the impact of serotonin manipulations on behaviour and cortical activation depends on individual differences in the serotoninergic system and susceptibility to impulsive behaviour (Crean et al., 2002; Macoveanu et al., 2013). In the later stages of Parkinson’s disease, therefore, the depletion of serotonin and other monoamine neurotransmitters to the frontal cortex and striatum (Politis et al., 2010, 2012; Goldstein et al., 2011) may predispose to a therapeutic effect of serotonin reuptake inhibition on action cancellation.

In this pharmacological functional MRI study, we examined the effect of citalopram (30 mg versus placebo) on response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease. The task incorporated NoGo and Stop-Signal trials while patients took their usual dopaminergic medication. We predicted that (i) both NoGo and Stop-Signal inhibition is impaired in Parkinson’s disease (cf. Gauggel et al., 2004; Obeso et al., 2011; Nombela et al., 2014); (ii) the behavioural effect of citalopram would depend on patients’ disease severity (cf.Cilia et al., 2011); and (iii) the behavioural effect may also relate to enhancement of frontal cortical activation after citalopram. We additionally examined other potential predictors of patients’ responsiveness to citalopram, such as preserved structural connections within the frontostriatal networks, by using diffusion tensor imaging.

Materials and methods

Participants

Twenty-one patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (UK Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank Clinical Diagnostic Criteria) were recruited through the Cambridge University Parkinson’s Disease Research Clinic. Inclusion criteria were: (i) Hoehn and Yahr stage 1.5–3; (ii) age 45–80 years; (iii) English speaking; (iv) right-handed; and (v) non-demented at last assessment (clinical impression and Mini-Mental State Examination score >26/30). Patients were excluded by (i) clinically significant current depression; (ii) contraindications to MRI or citalopram; (iii) epilepsy or past significant psychiatric disorders. None of our patients met criteria for an impulse control disorder. All were tested on regular dopaminergic medication, including levodopa (n = 19), non-ergot dopamine agonists (n = 17: eight pramipexole, eight ropinirole, one rotigotine), and other anti-parkinsonian medication (n = 8: seven amantadine, one rasagiline, one selegiline). Levodopa equivalent dose was calculated by the formula of Tomlinson et al. (2010). Demographic and clinical features of participants are given in Table 1.

Table 1

Demographic and clinical features, and neuropsychological measures (means, standard deviations and group differences)

Features / MeasuresParkinson’s diseaseControlGroup difference
Male:female11:1012:8n.s.
Age, years64.0 (8.1)65.3 (5.7)n.s.
Education, years14.6 (3.8)15.1 (2.5)n.s.
Mini-Mental State Examination28.9 (1.2)29.3 (0.9)n.s.
Duration of disease, years10.8 (4.9)
UPDRS (ON medication)
    I: mentation, behaviour and mood8.8 (4.9)
    III: motor20.6 (7.7)
Hoehn and Yahr1.9 (0.4)
Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living Scale86.5 (5.9)
Levodopa actual dose, mg/day393.4 (221.0)
Levodopa equivalent dose, mg/day632.6 (310.6)
Beck Depression Inventory II9.9 (5.5)3.8 (3.9)<0.001
REM Sleep Behaviour Disorder6.4 (2.9)2.0 (1.5)<0.001
Category fluency test20.6 (4.9)24.3 (6.7)0.05
Letter fluency test17.0 (5.6)18.1 (4.5)n.s.
Forward digit span7.0 (1.1)7.2 (0.8)n.s.
Backward digit span5.4 (1.5)5.7 (1.3)n.s.
Simple reaction time task
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.2)99.0 (1.3)n.s.
    Reaction time, ms293.7 (53.0)314.4 (71.8)n.s.
Choice reaction time
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.3)99.7 (0.5)<0.05
    Reaction time, ms353.4 (47.3)392.2 (70.0)<0.05
Features / MeasuresParkinson’s diseaseControlGroup difference
Male:female11:1012:8n.s.
Age, years64.0 (8.1)65.3 (5.7)n.s.
Education, years14.6 (3.8)15.1 (2.5)n.s.
Mini-Mental State Examination28.9 (1.2)29.3 (0.9)n.s.
Duration of disease, years10.8 (4.9)
UPDRS (ON medication)
    I: mentation, behaviour and mood8.8 (4.9)
    III: motor20.6 (7.7)
Hoehn and Yahr1.9 (0.4)
Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living Scale86.5 (5.9)
Levodopa actual dose, mg/day393.4 (221.0)
Levodopa equivalent dose, mg/day632.6 (310.6)
Beck Depression Inventory II9.9 (5.5)3.8 (3.9)<0.001
REM Sleep Behaviour Disorder6.4 (2.9)2.0 (1.5)<0.001
Category fluency test20.6 (4.9)24.3 (6.7)0.05
Letter fluency test17.0 (5.6)18.1 (4.5)n.s.
Forward digit span7.0 (1.1)7.2 (0.8)n.s.
Backward digit span5.4 (1.5)5.7 (1.3)n.s.
Simple reaction time task
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.2)99.0 (1.3)n.s.
    Reaction time, ms293.7 (53.0)314.4 (71.8)n.s.
Choice reaction time
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.3)99.7 (0.5)<0.05
    Reaction time, ms353.4 (47.3)392.2 (70.0)<0.05

Group difference: P-values of chi-squared or two-sample two-tailed t-tests, as appropriate; n.s. = not significant, P > 0.1.

Table 1

Demographic and clinical features, and neuropsychological measures (means, standard deviations and group differences)

Features / MeasuresParkinson’s diseaseControlGroup difference
Male:female11:1012:8n.s.
Age, years64.0 (8.1)65.3 (5.7)n.s.
Education, years14.6 (3.8)15.1 (2.5)n.s.
Mini-Mental State Examination28.9 (1.2)29.3 (0.9)n.s.
Duration of disease, years10.8 (4.9)
UPDRS (ON medication)
    I: mentation, behaviour and mood8.8 (4.9)
    III: motor20.6 (7.7)
Hoehn and Yahr1.9 (0.4)
Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living Scale86.5 (5.9)
Levodopa actual dose, mg/day393.4 (221.0)
Levodopa equivalent dose, mg/day632.6 (310.6)
Beck Depression Inventory II9.9 (5.5)3.8 (3.9)<0.001
REM Sleep Behaviour Disorder6.4 (2.9)2.0 (1.5)<0.001
Category fluency test20.6 (4.9)24.3 (6.7)0.05
Letter fluency test17.0 (5.6)18.1 (4.5)n.s.
Forward digit span7.0 (1.1)7.2 (0.8)n.s.
Backward digit span5.4 (1.5)5.7 (1.3)n.s.
Simple reaction time task
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.2)99.0 (1.3)n.s.
    Reaction time, ms293.7 (53.0)314.4 (71.8)n.s.
Choice reaction time
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.3)99.7 (0.5)<0.05
    Reaction time, ms353.4 (47.3)392.2 (70.0)<0.05
Features / MeasuresParkinson’s diseaseControlGroup difference
Male:female11:1012:8n.s.
Age, years64.0 (8.1)65.3 (5.7)n.s.
Education, years14.6 (3.8)15.1 (2.5)n.s.
Mini-Mental State Examination28.9 (1.2)29.3 (0.9)n.s.
Duration of disease, years10.8 (4.9)
UPDRS (ON medication)
    I: mentation, behaviour and mood8.8 (4.9)
    III: motor20.6 (7.7)
Hoehn and Yahr1.9 (0.4)
Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living Scale86.5 (5.9)
Levodopa actual dose, mg/day393.4 (221.0)
Levodopa equivalent dose, mg/day632.6 (310.6)
Beck Depression Inventory II9.9 (5.5)3.8 (3.9)<0.001
REM Sleep Behaviour Disorder6.4 (2.9)2.0 (1.5)<0.001
Category fluency test20.6 (4.9)24.3 (6.7)0.05
Letter fluency test17.0 (5.6)18.1 (4.5)n.s.
Forward digit span7.0 (1.1)7.2 (0.8)n.s.
Backward digit span5.4 (1.5)5.7 (1.3)n.s.
Simple reaction time task
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.2)99.0 (1.3)n.s.
    Reaction time, ms293.7 (53.0)314.4 (71.8)n.s.
Choice reaction time
    Correct trials, %98.9 (1.3)99.7 (0.5)<0.05
    Reaction time, ms353.4 (47.3)392.2 (70.0)<0.05

Group difference: P-values of chi-squared or two-sample two-tailed t-tests, as appropriate; n.s. = not significant, P > 0.1.

Twenty healthy control subjects with no history of significant neurological or psychiatric disorders were recruited from the Parkinson’s Disease Research Clinic database and the volunteer panel of the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit. This study was approved by the local research ethics committee and exempted from Clinical Trials status by the Medicines and Healthcare products Regulatory Authority. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Experimental design

This study used a double-blind randomized crossover design (Figure 1). Separate sessions at least 6 days apart included a neuropsychological battery and brain imaging, after either a 30 mg oral citalopram or an identically over-coated placebo capsule. Nineteen patients completed both sessions. Blood samples were taken 2 h after administration, immediately before functional MRI scanning to coincide with the estimated time of peak plasma concentration (Sangkuhl et al., 2011). Mean plasma concentration under citalopram: 46 ng/ml, range 11–70 mg/ml; under placebo: 0 mg/ml. Control subjects had one testing session only without drug.

The task included randomly interleaved NoGo and Stop-Signal trials (figure 1), which enabled us to examine both types of inhibition while matching for drug levels, practice effects and fatigue. There were 360 Go trials (75%), 40 NoGo trials (8%) and 80 Stop-Signal trials (17%). On Go trials, subjects responded to a left/right black arrow (1000 ms) by pressing buttons with their right hand. Responses were made with either index finger (for the left arrow) or middle finger (for the right arrow). In Stop-Signal trials, a response was initially cued by the left/right black arrow but the arrow colour changed to red concurrent with a tone after a Stop-Signal delay and subjects were required to not make the response. The Stop-Signal delay was varied from trial to trial by using a step-up/down algorithm with an initial estimate of 250 ms to maintain 50% successful inhibition (Chamberlain et al., 2007a). This is similar to the tracking algorithm used in previous imaging studies of the Stop-Signal task (Cubillo et al., 2012; Pauls et al., 2012; Costa et al., 2013). In NoGo trials, subjects were required to make no response to the red arrow (1000 ms) and beep, equivalent to a Stop-Signal delay of zero.

Subjects also completed the Beck Depression Inventory II, REM Sleep Behaviour Disorder, category (‘animals’, ‘items of clothing’ or ‘fruit/vegetables’) and letter fluency (‘P’, ‘S’ or ‘F’), digit span, and manual simple (one stimulus type, one response button) and choice reaction time tasks (two stimulus types, two response buttons; see Supplementary material for a detailed description). The versions of each fluency task were randomly given across drug and placebo sessions of each subject, after pilot studies demonstrated their equivalence in older adults.

We measured four behavioural parameters of interest in the functional MRI task: (i) mean reaction time of correct Go trials; (ii) SSRT; (iii) the rate of Go commission errors; and (iv) the rate of NoGo commission errors. A NoGo commission error means that participants pressed a button. A Go commission error means that participants pressed a wrong button. The SSRT was estimated using the integration method (Logan and Cowan, 1984), and adjusted for omission errors on Go trials using Equation 1 below, where P(respond | signal) is the probability of responding. The Go omission rate was 1% in controls, 7% in Parkinson’s disease-placebo, and 6% in Parkinson’s disease-citalopram (Parkinson’s disease-placebo versus control: two-sample t-test, P < 0.01; Parkinson’s disease-citalopram versus Parkinson’s disease-placebo: paired-sample t-test, not significant). The distribution of Go reaction times indicated that the omission errors were not a result of exceeding the response window, but rather motor impairments (e.g. slipping off the buttons or failure to press firmly). The omission rate of individual patients was used to adjust SSRT because the omission errors may lead to an underestimation of SSRT.
(1)

Disease effects were tested with two-sample t-tests, comparing Parkinson’s disease-placebo with controls. In this study, the ‘disease effect’ refers to treated Parkinson’s disease (cf.Lewis et al., 2003; Weintraub et al., 2011). In principle, differences between Parkinson’s disease-placebo and control subjects could be because of the presence of Parkinson’s disease, the use of anti-parkinsonian medication, or the additional use of a placebo tablet in the patient group. However, the concurrent use of dopaminergic drugs is unlikely to fully explain the group differences (Obeso et al., 2011).

Analysis of drug effects used repeat-measures ANOVAs with drug (citalopram versus placebo) as a within-subject factor and disease severity [Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) motor score], levodopa equivalent dose, age and plasma concentration of citalopram as covariates.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition and analysis

Functional images were acquired using a quiet echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence with a Siemens Trio 3 T scanner (2656-ms repetition time, 44-ms echo time, 78° flip angle, 32 sequential descending oblique axial slices, 192 × 192 mm2 field of view, 3-mm thickness, 0.75-mm slice gap, and 3 × 3 mm2 in-plane resolution; (Peelle et al., 2010). High-resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE images were also acquired (144 sequential sagittal slices, 240 × 240 mm2 field of view, 1.25-mm thickness, and 1.25 × 1.25 mm2 in-plane resolution).

Functional MRI preprocessing and analysis used SPM8 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Eleven volumes were discarded to allow magnetization equilibration. Functional images were realigned to the first image, sinc-interpolated to correct for differences in slice acquisition time, normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template using iterative segmentation, and denoised with a wavelet-based approach (Khullar et al., 2011).

General linear models were estimated for each subject by convolving a design matrix with the canonical haemodynamic response function. The design matrix included discrete events for correct Stop-Signal, NoGo and Go trials, commission errors (Stop-Signal, NoGo and Go trials) and omission errors (Go trials). Six standard movement parameters (x, y, z, pitch, roll, and yaw) were included as nuisance regressors. Classical parameter estimation was applied with a lag−1 autoregressive model and high-pass filter of 128 s. Contrast images of interest were computed for each subject: Stop-Signal, NoGo and Go versus implicit baseline, Stop-Signal > Go and NoGo > Go. The contrasts were entered into second-level ‘random-effect’ models to examine the effects of disease and citalopram on event-related activations.

Activation maps of the Stop-Signal > Go and NoGo > Go contrasts were computed for each group (one-sample t-tests, voxel-level P < 0.001 uncorrected, and cluster-level P < 0.05 family-wise-error correction for multiple comparison, FWE). We also constructed a study-specific region of interest for the right inferior frontal gyrus, in view of our previous hypotheses of the task-related activation and effects of Parkinson’s disease. This region included voxels from the Automated Anatomical Labelling (AAL)-defined right inferior frontal gyrus that were activated by Stop-Signal>Go or NoGo>Go contrasts in control subjects. This mask was used for small volume correction for multiple comparisons (voxel-wise P < 0.05 FWE-corrected).

The right inferior frontal gyrus mask was also used for a region of interest analysis in which parameter estimates (betas from subject-level models) were pooled (i) for repeated-measures ANOVAs with drug as a within-subject factor and UPDRS, levodopa equivalent dose, age and drug concentration as covariates; and (ii) for correlations between changes in behaviour and changes in right inferior frontal gyrus activation.

Diffusion magnetic resonance imaging

We additionally examined white matter connections of the frontostriatal circuits as a potential predictor of the citalopram effect on behavioural performance. Diffusion-weighted images were collected along 63 gradient directions (single acquisition, 63 sequential ascending axial slices, 192 × 192 mm2 field of view, 2-mm thickness, and 2 × 2 mm2 in-plane resolution) and analysed with FSL4.1 (www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Images were corrected for head movements and eddy currents, and smoothed with a 2.5-mm Gaussian kernel. Diffusion tensors were linearly fitted to the diffusion-weighted imaging, and fractional anisotropy maps were calculated for tract-based spatial statistics (Smith et al., 2006). Fractional anisotropy images were corrected for outlier values, registered to the FMRIB58 fractional anisotropy standard-space image, and normalized to MNI 152 space. A mean fractional anisotropy skeleton was derived and thresholded at fractional anisotropy > 0.2 to represent the centre of the white matter tracts common to all subjects. Permutation tests with threshold-free cluster enhancement (P < 0.05 TFCE-corrected) (Smith and Nichols, 2009) examined whether citalopram’s effects on behavioural performance (reduction in SSRT or NoGo error) varied with frontostriatal connectivity.

Results

Behavioural results

Details of subject demographics, clinical disease severity and neuropsychological scores are given in Table 1. The groups were well matched for age, sex and educational years. Higher scores on the Beck Depression Inventory-II and REM Sleep Behaviour Disorder questionnaires are typical of mild to moderate Parkinson’s disease. Note that higher scores on the Beck Depression Inventory-II do not in themselves establish a diagnosis of depression. The groups were also matched in reaction time and accuracy of the simple reaction time task. In the choice reaction time task, the patient group was faster but significantly less accurate than the control group (cf. Hughes et al., 2010).

(A) Task and stimuli. (B) Design and drug. PD = Parkinson’s disease; DA = dopamine.
Figure 1

(A) Task and stimuli. (B) Design and drug. PD = Parkinson’s disease; DA = dopamine.

Table 2 shows the SSRT, Go reaction time, Go error rates, and NoGo error rates from control subjects and from patients under citalopram and placebo, during performance of the functional MRI paradigm. Compared with control subjects, the Parkinson’s disease-placebo group had longer SSRTs, more NoGo errors and Go errors. The group difference in SSRT could not be attributed to strategic slowing of responses, given the similarity of Go reaction time.

Table 2

Performance on the Stop-Signal and NoGo task

ParametersControlParkinson’s disease
Disease effectsDrug effects
PlaceboCitalopramDrugDrug × UPDRS
SSRT (ms)142 (44)167 (50)180 (75)P < 0.05P < 0.05P < 0.01
Go reaction time (ms)532 (129)554 (108)555 (100)n.s.n.s.n.s.
NoGo error0.01 (0.01)0.04 (0.04)0.05 (0.09)P < 0.01n.s.P < 0.05
Go error0.01 (0.01)0.02 (0.02)0.02 (0.02)P < 0.001n.s.n.s.
ParametersControlParkinson’s disease
Disease effectsDrug effects
PlaceboCitalopramDrugDrug × UPDRS
SSRT (ms)142 (44)167 (50)180 (75)P < 0.05P < 0.05P < 0.01
Go reaction time (ms)532 (129)554 (108)555 (100)n.s.n.s.n.s.
NoGo error0.01 (0.01)0.04 (0.04)0.05 (0.09)P < 0.01n.s.P < 0.05
Go error0.01 (0.01)0.02 (0.02)0.02 (0.02)P < 0.001n.s.n.s.

Values are group means (standard deviations). Disease effects refer to the contrast ‘Parkinson’s disease-placebo versus control’ of two-sample t-tests (one-tailed). Drug effects refer to the main effect of drug (Parkinson’s disease-citalopram versus Parkinson’s disease-placebo) and the interaction of drug and UPDRS (Drug × UPDRS) from repeated-measures ANOVAs (with UPDRS, age, levodopa equivalent dose and plasma concentration as covariates). n.s. = not significant, P > 0.1.

Table 2

Performance on the Stop-Signal and NoGo task

ParametersControlParkinson’s disease
Disease effectsDrug effects
PlaceboCitalopramDrugDrug × UPDRS
SSRT (ms)142 (44)167 (50)180 (75)P < 0.05P < 0.05P < 0.01
Go reaction time (ms)532 (129)554 (108)555 (100)n.s.n.s.n.s.
NoGo error0.01 (0.01)0.04 (0.04)0.05 (0.09)P < 0.01n.s.P < 0.05
Go error0.01 (0.01)0.02 (0.02)0.02 (0.02)P < 0.001n.s.n.s.
ParametersControlParkinson’s disease
Disease effectsDrug effects
PlaceboCitalopramDrugDrug × UPDRS
SSRT (ms)142 (44)167 (50)180 (75)P < 0.05P < 0.05P < 0.01
Go reaction time (ms)532 (129)554 (108)555 (100)n.s.n.s.n.s.
NoGo error0.01 (0.01)0.04 (0.04)0.05 (0.09)P < 0.01n.s.P < 0.05
Go error0.01 (0.01)0.02 (0.02)0.02 (0.02)P < 0.001n.s.n.s.

Values are group means (standard deviations). Disease effects refer to the contrast ‘Parkinson’s disease-placebo versus control’ of two-sample t-tests (one-tailed). Drug effects refer to the main effect of drug (Parkinson’s disease-citalopram versus Parkinson’s disease-placebo) and the interaction of drug and UPDRS (Drug × UPDRS) from repeated-measures ANOVAs (with UPDRS, age, levodopa equivalent dose and plasma concentration as covariates). n.s. = not significant, P > 0.1.

Figure 2 shows the behavioural effects of citalopram on response inhibition. ANOVAs revealed a significant interaction between drug and UPDRS (motor) for both SSRT [F(1,14) = 10.54, P < 0.01] and NoGo error rate [F(1,14) = 5.71, P < 0.05]. This indicates that citalopram reduced SSRT and NoGo error rate in relatively more advanced disease (larger UPDRS-motor score). For NoGo error rate, there was also an interaction between drug and levodopa equivalent dose [F(1,14) = 11.34, P < 0.01], suggesting that ΔNoGo-error positively correlated with levodopa equivalent dose. Note that there was no correlation between patients’ UPDRS-motor score and levodopa equivalent dose (P > 0.9). We also included the Beck Depression Inventory-II score as a covariate in a separate ANOVA but observed no interaction between depression and drug effects.

In behaviour, citalopram reduced SSRT (left) and NoGo error rate (right) in relatively more advanced disease. The drug-induced changes of SSRT (ΔSSRT) and NoGo-error (ΔNoGo-error) negatively correlated with UPDRS-motor score (P < 0.05).
Figure 2

In behaviour, citalopram reduced SSRT (left) and NoGo error rate (right) in relatively more advanced disease. The drug-induced changes of SSRT (ΔSSRT) and NoGo-error (ΔNoGo-error) negatively correlated with UPDRS-motor score (P < 0.05).

Functional and diffusion magnetic resonance imaging

Figure 3A and B present inhibition-related activations in controls and Parkinson’s disease-placebo, and group differences. Controls showed typical activations of right inferior frontal gyrus for Stop-Signal > Go [peak coordinates in MNI space: (54 18 8), t = 7.17, 1621 voxels] and for NoGo > Go [peak: (48 18 −2), t = 6.00, 452 voxels]. The stop-related activations extended dorsally into the caudal part of the middle frontal gyrus, and adjacent premotor cortex, in keeping with previous imaging studies of Stop-Signal tasks (Rae et al., 2014). Stop- and NoGo-related activations were also seen in the superior temporal cortex and ventral occipitotemporal cortex, consistent with the presence of audiovisual cues. The stop-related right inferior frontal gyrus activation was significantly reduced in Parkinson’s disease-placebo compared with controls (‘disease effect’). However, no group difference was observed on right inferior frontal gyrus activation during successful NoGo trials.

(A) In functional MRI, controls showed greater stop-related (Stop-Signal > Go, orange) and NoGo-related activations (NoGo > Go, green) in the right inferior frontal gyrus (P < 0.05 corrected). Activation overlap is in red. Coordinates are in MNI space. (B) The stop-related right inferior frontal gyrus activation was significantly weaker in Parkinson’s disease-placebo (PD-PLA) than in controls (disease effect, P < 0.05 corrected). Colour scale indicates t-values. (C) Citalopram’s enhancement of right inferior frontal gyrus activation in successful stops positively correlated with UPDRS-motor score (P < 0.05). (D) The drug-induced changes of SSRT (ΔSSRT) and NoGo-error (ΔNoGo-error) respectively related to right inferior frontal gyrus activation changes in successful Stop-Signal trials (ΔStop-Signal-activation) and those in NoGo trials (ΔNoGo-activation).
Figure 3

(A) In functional MRI, controls showed greater stop-related (Stop-Signal > Go, orange) and NoGo-related activations (NoGo > Go, green) in the right inferior frontal gyrus (P < 0.05 corrected). Activation overlap is in red. Coordinates are in MNI space. (B) The stop-related right inferior frontal gyrus activation was significantly weaker in Parkinson’s disease-placebo (PD-PLA) than in controls (disease effect, P < 0.05 corrected). Colour scale indicates t-values. (C) Citalopram’s enhancement of right inferior frontal gyrus activation in successful stops positively correlated with UPDRS-motor score (P < 0.05). (D) The drug-induced changes of SSRT (ΔSSRT) and NoGo-error (ΔNoGo-error) respectively related to right inferior frontal gyrus activation changes in successful Stop-Signal trials (ΔStop-Signal-activation) and those in NoGo trials (ΔNoGo-activation).

As in the behavioural data, the regional analysis on right inferior frontal gyrus activation showed an interaction between drug and UPDRS for Stop-Signal trials [F(1,14) = 5.07, P < 0.05], suggesting that citalopram positively modulated right inferior frontal gyrus activation in more advanced disease (Fig. 3C). There was no main effect of drug for Stop-Signal trials (P = 0.25) or for NoGo trials (F < 1).

The right inferior frontal gyrus is the focus of this study, but the Stop-Signal and NoGo tasks were also associated with activation of the supplementary motor area in our study and previous reports. We additionally analysed the drug effects on the supplementary motor area and presented as the Supplementary material. In brief, we observed no drug effect on the supplementary motor area.

To determine whether citalopram’s effect on behavioural performance can be predicted by its enhancement of right inferior frontal gyrus activation in Parkinson’s disease, we conducted a correlation with the change of SSRT (ΔSSRT: SSRT-citalopram − SSRT-placebo) and the change of right inferior frontal gyrus activation on successful Stop-Signal trials (ΔStop-Signal-activation), as well as a correlation between the change of NoGo error rate (ΔNoGo-error) and the change of right inferior frontal gyrus activation on correct NoGo trials (ΔNoGo-activation). Figure 3D shows that citalopram’s effect on both restraint (ΔNoGo-error: r = −0.39, P = 0.05) and cancellation (ΔSSRT: r = −0.54, P < 0.01) depended on the change in right inferior frontal gyrus activation. This effect was preserved after exclusion of a single subject with high values of ΔNoGo and ΔSSRT.

Another potential predictor of citalopram’s effect on behavioural performance was the white matter connectivity, including the fibres connecting the right frontal lobe, striatum and thalamus through the anterior internal capsule (Fig. 4). There was a negative correlation of the drug-induced SSRT change with individual patients’ fractional anisotropy of white matter in the right internal capsule. Patients with greater anatomical frontostriatal connectivity showed a drug-induced reduction in SSRT. No such relation was obtained between NoGo error and fractional anisotropy.

Citalopram’s effect on response inhibition was also related to the strength of structural connectivity (fractional anisotropy, FA) between the frontal cortex and striatum, showing a negative correlation of the drug-induced SSRT change (ΔSSRT) with fractional anisotropy of the skeleton voxels in the right internal capsule (mean centred). Thresholded at P < 0.05 TFCE-corrected.
Figure 4

Citalopram’s effect on response inhibition was also related to the strength of structural connectivity (fractional anisotropy, FA) between the frontal cortex and striatum, showing a negative correlation of the drug-induced SSRT change (ΔSSRT) with fractional anisotropy of the skeleton voxels in the right internal capsule (mean centred). Thresholded at P < 0.05 TFCE-corrected.

Discussion

In this pharmacological functional MRI study, we used the integrated NoGo and Stop-Signal task to study action restraint and cancellation in Parkinson’s disease. The Stop-Signal and NoGo inhibition is assessed by SSRT and NoGo error rate, and associated with widespread activations including the activation on the right inferior frontal gyrus (Swick et al., 2011; Rae et al., 2014). As in previous patient studies (Gauggel et al., 2004; Obeso et al., 2011; Nombela et al., 2014) the behavioural impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease was evident in the absence of clinical impulse control disorders: our cohort had longer SSRT and more NoGo errors than controls. A single dose of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram has the potential to improve response inhibition, but only in a subset of patient with Parkinson’s disease.

Citalopram had no beneficial behavioural effect when averaged across all patients. The impact of citalopram on response inhibition (reducing SSRT and NoGo errors) was beneficial in patients with relatively more advanced disease (larger UPDRS-motor score) and related to the effect of citalopram on activation of the right inferior frontal gyrus. We cannot be certain whether the effect of serotonergic agents is direct, by selective serotonin ‘restoration’, or indirect, through serotonergic interactions with dopamine, noradrenaline or N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) (Higgins et al., 2003; Winstanley et al., 2005). Nonetheless, our findings reinforce preclinical and comparative studies of serotonergic modulations of response inhibition, and suggest that patients with more severe motor signs (UPDRS scores) and relatively preserved structural connectivity in frontostriatal networks (from diffusion-weighted imaging) could be stratified to receive citalopram in further clinical trials.

In the ‘Introduction’ section we highlighted the possible psychopharmacological differences between action restraint and action cancellation, with evidence for serotonergic regulation of the former, but not latter, type of inhibition (Eagle et al., 2008). These observations included studies of citalopram and other SSRIs in human (Del-Ben et al., 2005; Chamberlain et al., 2006; Macoveanu et al., 2013) and rodent versions of Stop-Signal tasks (Bari et al., 2009; Eagle et al., 2009) and tasks that require action restraint during waiting (Harrison et al., 1999; Miyazaki et al., 2011; Baarendse and Vanderschuren, 2012). This study confirmed that citalopram can reduce NoGo errors in patients with relatively advanced disease and those for whom citalopram increases right inferior frontal gyrus activation.

However, our study also showed a clear effect of citalopram on action cancellation and associated cortical activation, which, at first glance, contrasts with the literature on Stop-Signal tasks in healthy humans and animal models. However, two factors must be considered. First, is the nature of inhibitory processes required for Stop-Signal tasks and other forms of inhibitory control. Attentional reorienting to task-relevant stimuli is a feature of both NoGo and Stop-Signal trials (Levy and Wagner, 2011) but also of ‘continue’ trials, which are associated with right inferior frontal gyrus activation in the absence of inhibition (Sharp et al., 2010). A failure of attending away from the active ‘go’ response following the ‘stop’ cue may relate to the impulsivity of Parkinson’s disease. Nonetheless, inhibition of actions per se has been linked to right inferior frontal gyrus in health (Zhang et al., 2012). Moreover, reversal learning deficits after central serotonin depletion may be considered a failure to inhibit responding to a previously learned but task-irrelevant stimulus (Clarke et al., 2004, 2007; Walker et al., 2009). Citalopram may benefit response inhibition by targeting more than one of these processes in Stop-Signal and NoGo trials.

Second and perhaps more importantly, one must consider the relationship between the impact of serotonin manipulation and the baseline serotonin functioning. Previous studies showed that central serotonin depletion only impaired action restraint and cancellation in adults who had a family history of alcoholism and significantly lower central serotonin concentration, but not in those with a low risk of alcoholism and no family history of alcoholism (LeMarquand et al., 1999; Crean et al., 2002). It is possible that individuals with serotonergic deficits are more likely to respond to the acute change of serotonin transmission. This explains why we observed the behavioural benefit of citalopram in patients with Parkinson’s disease (especially those with relatively more advanced disease) but previous studies with healthy humans did not (Chamberlain et al., 2006; Drueke et al., 2010; Nandam et al., 2011).

The observed effect of a selective reuptake inhibitor such as citalopram is unlikely to be mediated by directly increasing the cortical extracellular level of dopamine or noradrenaline, as levels of both remain at baseline after an acute citalopram administration (Bymaster et al., 2002). However, postsynaptic effects of SSRIs may not be restricted to serotonin receptor signalling pathways. Recent evidence has shown interactions between serotonin 5-HT2A and dopamine D2 receptors through forming heteromeric complexes in prefrontal and striatal regions (Borroto-Escuela et al., 2010; Albizu et al., 2011), between cortical 5-HT1A and NMDA receptors by downregulating microtubule/kinesin-based dendritic transport of NMDA receptors (Yuen et al., 2005), and between 5-HT3 and noradrenaline receptors in the locus coeruleus (Ortega et al., 2012). We therefore cannot rule out a contribution of such secondary mechanisms to the effect of citalopram, given the correlation between levodopa dose equivalent and drug-induced reduction of NoGo errors. Such a correlation was not observed on Stop-Signal trials, consistent with the literature showing that dopaminergic influences on SSRT performance are minor (Robbins, 2007).

One may argue that acute administration of SSRIs such as citalopram may reduce serotonergic transmission through stimulating presynaptic 5-HT1A inhibitory autoreceptors in the raphe nucleus. This is unlikely because the net effect of acute citalopram is an increase of extracellular serotonin levels, although the increase is not as great as that introduced by chronic citalopram or by a combination of citalopram and 5-HT autoreceptor antagonists (Moret and Briley, 1996).

We are also aware of the differences between acute and chronic treatments of SSRIs. In the context of depression, single-dose SSRIs enhanced positive biases (e.g. citalopram) and decreased threat-relevant biases (e.g. mirtazapine) in emotional perception and memory (Harmer et al., 2003; Browning et al., 2007; Arnone et al., 2009; Rawlings et al., 2010). However, clinical effects of SSRIs are usually observed after a couple of weeks of chronic treatment. The delay in the clinical effects of SSRIs may be because of the relearning of emotional association, a process which takes time and exposure to a more positive emotional environment (Harmer and Cowen, 2013). In fear conditioning tasks, acute and chronic SSRI administrations even produced opposite effects (Burghardt et al., 2004).

In the alternative context of impulse control, acute citalopram treatments benefit NoGo type (for a review, see Eagle et al., 2008) and other forms of response inhibition in animal models (Eagle et al., 2009; Miyazaki et al., 2011; Baarendse and Vanderschuren, 2012; Humpston et al., 2013). In healthy adults, the acute effect of citalopram was observed on frontal activations but not always in behaviour (Del-Ben et al., 2005; Chamberlain et al., 2006; Macoveanu et al., 2013) while recognizing the complexity of serotonergic systems in impulsive action (Kirby et al., 2011). On tasks that require inhibition of response sets, such as reversal learning task, the acute and chronic effects of citalopram are comparable (Danet et al., 2012). However, the acute and chronic treatments may have different mechanisms, either pharmacologically or in terms of the networks and neural targets they engage. For example, single-dose citalopram may reduce impulsive actions by directly increasing extracellular serotonin levels in the prefrontal cortex, as we suggest is likely to occur in this study. But we cannot rule out stimulation of frontal 5-HT2A receptors as occurs in chronic citalopram treatment (Furr et al., 2012).

It has been proposed that the seemingly separate roles of serotonin in depression and inhibition are indeed related (Cools et al., 2011). For example, recent studies suggested that the link between negative mood and serotonin decrease is indirect and likely mediated by damaged inhibition and/or through associative learning (Dayan and Huys, 2008; Robinson and Sahakian, 2008). Other studies showed that serotonin mediates behavioural inhibition and impulsivity in an affective context (Cools et al., 2005; Crockett et al., 2009).

Serotonin has also been linked with semantic memory (Mendelsohn et al., 2009). We examined the effect of citalopram on category fluency (as measured by the number of words produced in a category), which was impaired in Parkinson’s disease (Table 1). A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed an interaction between drug and disease severity [F(1,14) = 7.56, P < 0.05], indicating that citalopram improved category fluency in patients with more advanced disease. Citalopram may facilitate the inhibition of simultaneously active conceptual representations during lexical selection, which is mediated by the inferior frontal cortex (Badre and Wagner, 2007), rather than the simple articulatory process. In support of this, letter fluency, which requires articulatory process but not selection among concepts, was not improved by citalopram. However, a single framework that integrates the roles of neurotransmitters in cognitive control and language processing is beyond the scope of this study. Further studies are needed to test to what extent neurocognitive models of attentional control in perception and action can explain inhibition and shifting in comprehension and production of word, sentence and discourse.

This study has several methodological limitations. First, we used functional MRI blood oxygen level-dependent signals, which could in principle be influenced by citalopram. However, this is unlikely because frontal regions retain normal blood flow on a NoGo task after an acute treatment of citalopram (Macoveanu et al., 2013). Second, we focused on the right inferior frontal gyrus because this region is consistently associated with motor inhibition in many different versions of NoGo and Stop-Signal tasks (Rubia et al., 2001). The right inferior frontal gyrus may also provide a cortical ‘readout’ of subcortical effects (Agnoli and Carli, 2012): it would be interesting to examine the effects of citalopram on the striatum but the striatum had relatively poor signal-to-noise ratio compared to cortical regions using the quiet-EPI sequence. A role of the striatum in mediating the effects of citalopram, in conjunction with prefrontal cortex, is, however, suggested by diffusion MRI that the preserved structural connectivity between frontal and striatal regions was a significant predictor of the effectiveness of citalopram treatment. Third, Parkinson’s disease may also affect other processes of cognitive control on NoGo trials, such as action selection, in addition to inhibition (Hughes et al., 2010). Fourth, the heterogeneity of Parkinson’s disease may reduce the power of group-level analysis. We therefore used a powerful within-subject design and emphasized individual differences, together with strong control of the type I error. However, we cannot rule out a type II error, especially for the lack of effects of disease and citalopram on NoGo trials, as the activity pattern of NoGo was less extensive and less intensive even in healthy controls. Still the positive results of this study are large by comparison with many functional MRI studies in Parkinson’s disease. Fifth, our patients were in the mid-stage of Parkinson’s disease, but all were taking dopaminergic medication (81% were taking dopamine receptor agonists). To identify the effect of dopamine agonists, we conducted a post hoc ANOVA with target drug (citalopram versus placebo) and dopamine agonist (taking versus not taking). There was no interaction for either NoGo (P = 0.52) or Stop-Signal trials (P = 0.27), suggesting that the effect of citalopram is not strongly influenced by concurrent dopamine agonists. We also cannot comment on the application of citalopram for drug-naïve patients immediately after diagnosis, except to note that the positive correlation between citalopram-induced reduction of NoGo error and levodopa equivalent dose suggests that those patients may also benefit from citalopram, at least on action restraint. Finally, we did not include patients with impulse control disorders although impulse control disorder was not an exclusion criterion. This reflects perhaps the low prevalence and high awareness of impulse control disorders in a specialist service, combined with possible patient recruitment biases regarding patients with severe impulse control disorders.

In conclusion, we have shown that the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram can improve behavioural impulsivity in terms of action restraint and cancellation, but only in a subset of patients with Parkinson’s disease (patients with relatively more severe disease). The behavioural benefits of citalopram relate to enhanced activations in the inferior frontal cortex. These findings contribute to the broader understanding of the role of serotonergic systems in regulating cognitive and behavioural control, and indicate the need for patient stratification in clinical trials of serotonergic treatments in Parkinson’s disease.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr G. Logan for advice on SSRT estimation and to Dr Marta Correia for advice on DTI analysis.

Funding

This work was primarily funded by the Wellcome Trust (088324) with additional support from the Medical Research Council (MC_US_A060_0016, and RG62761), the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre and Parkinson’s UK. The BCNI is supported by the Wellcome Trust and Medical Research Council.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain online.

Abbreviations

    Abbreviations
     
  • SSRI

    selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor

  •  
  • SSRT

    Stop-Signal Reaction Time

  •  
  • UPDRS

    Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale

References

Agnoli
L
Carli
M
,
Dorsal-striatal 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors control impulsivity and perseverative responding in the 5-choice serial reaction time task
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2012
, vol.
219
(pg.
633
-
45
)
Albizu
L
Holloway
T
González-Maeso
J
Sealfon
SC
,
Functional crosstalk and heteromerization of serotonin 5-HT2A and dopamine D2 receptors
Neuropharmacology
,
2011
, vol.
61
(pg.
770
-
7
)
Anderson
IM
Clark
L
Elliott
R
Kulkarni
B
Williams
SR
Deakin
JF
,
5-HT(2C) receptor activation by m-chlorophenylpiperazine detected in humans with fMRI
Neuroreport
,
2002
, vol.
13
(pg.
1547
-
51
)
Arnone
D
Horder
J
Cowen
PJ
Harmer
CJ
,
Early effects of mirtazapine on emotional processing
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2009
, vol.
203
(pg.
685
-
91
)
Baarendse
PJ
Vanderschuren
LJ
,
Dissociable effects of monoamine reuptake inhibitors on distinct forms of impulsive behavior in rats
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2012
, vol.
219
(pg.
13
-
26
)
Badre
D
Wagner
AD
,
Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory
Neuropsychologia
,
2007
, vol.
45
(pg.
2883
-
901
)
Bari
A
Eagle
DM
Mar
AC
Robinson
ES
Robbins
TW
,
Dissociable effects of noradrenaline, dopamine, and serotonin uptake blockade on stop task performance in rats
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2009
, vol.
205
(pg.
273
-
83
)
Bentivoqlio
AR
Baldonero
E
Ricciardi
L
De Niqris
F
Daniele
A
,
Neuropsychological features of patients with Parkinson's disease and impulse control disorders
Neuro Sci
,
2012
, vol.
34
(pg.
1207
-
13
)
Borroto-Escuela
DO
Romero-Fernandez
W
Tarakanov
AO
Marcellino
D
Ciruela
F
Agnati
LF
et al.
,
Dopamine D2 and 5-hydroxytryptamine 5-HT(2A) receptors assemble into functionally interacting heteromers
Biochem Biophys Res Commun
,
2010
, vol.
401
(pg.
605
-
10
)
Browning
M
Reid
C
Cowen
PJ
Goodwin
GM
Harmer
CJ
,
A single dose of citalopram increases fear recognition in healthy subjects
J Psychopharmacol
,
2007
, vol.
21
(pg.
684
-
90
)
Burghardt
NS
Sullivan
GM
McEwen
BS
Gorman
JM
LeDoux
JE
,
The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram increases fear after acute treatment but reduces fear with chronic treatment: a comparison with tianeptine
Biol Psychiatry
,
2004
, vol.
55
(pg.
1171
-
8
)
Bymaster
FP
Zhang
W
Carter
PA
Shaw
J
Chernet
E
Phebus
L
et al.
,
Fluoxetine, but not other selective serotonin uptake inhibitors, increases norepinephrine and dopamine extracellular levels in prefrontal cortex
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2002
, vol.
160
(pg.
353
-
61
)
Chamberlain
SR
Blackwell
AD
Fineberg
NA
Robbins
TW
Sahakian
BJ
,
The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
,
2005
, vol.
29
(pg.
399
-
419
)
Chamberlain
SR
Del Campo
N
Dowson
J
Müller
U
Clark
L
Robbins
TW
et al.
,
Atomoxetine improved response inhibition in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Biol Psychiatry
,
2007a
, vol.
62
(pg.
977
-
84
)
Chamberlain
SR
Müller
U
Blackwell
AD
Clark
L
Robbins
TW
Sahakian
BJ
,
Neurochemical modulation of response inhibition and probabilistic learning in humans
Science
,
2006
, vol.
311
(pg.
861
-
3
)
Chamberlain
SR
Müller
U
Deakin
JB
Corlett
PR
Dowson
J
Cardinal
RN
et al.
,
Lack of deleterious effects of buspirone on cognition in healthy male volunteers
JPsychopharmacol
,
2007b
, vol.
21
(pg.
210
-
5
)
Cilia
R
Cho
SS
van Eimeren
T
Marotta
G
Siri
C
Ko
JH
et al.
,
Pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson's disease is associated with fronto-striatal disconnection: a path modeling analysis
Mov Disord
,
2011
, vol.
26
(pg.
225
-
33
)
Clark
L
Roiser
JP
Cools
R
Rubinsztein
DC
Sahakian
BJ
Robbins
TW
,
Stop signal response inhibition is not modulated by tryptophan depletion or the serotonin transporter polymorphism in healthy volunteers: implications for the 5-HT theory of impulsivity
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2005
, vol.
182
(pg.
570
-
8
)
Clarke
HF
Dalley
JW
Crofts
HS
Robbins
TW
Roberts
AC
,
Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion
Science
,
2004
, vol.
304
(pg.
878
-
80
)
Clarke
HF
Walker
SC
Dalley
JW
Robbins
TW
Roberts
AC
,
Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behaviorally and neurochemically specific
Cereb Cortex
,
2007
, vol.
17
(pg.
18
-
27
)
Cools
R
Blackwell
A
Clark
L
Menzies
L
Cox
S
Robbins
TW
,
Tryptophan depletion disrupts the motivational guidance of goal-directed behavior as a function of trait impulsivity
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2005
, vol.
30
(pg.
1362
-
73
)
Cools
R
Nakamura
K
Daw
ND
,
Serotonin and dopamine: unifying affective, activational, and decision functions
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2011
, vol.
36
(pg.
98
-
113
)
Costa
A
Riedel
M
Pogarell
O
Menzel-Zelnitschek
F
Schwarz
M
Reiser
M
et al.
,
Methylphenidate effects on neural activity during response inhibition in healthy humans
Cereb Cortex
,
2013
, vol.
23
pg.
117989
Crean
J
Richards
JB
de Wit
H
,
Effect of tryptophan depletion on impulsive behavior in men with or without a family history of alcoholism
Behav Brain Res
,
2002
, vol.
136
(pg.
349
-
57
)
Crockett
MJ
Clark
L
Robbins
TW
,
Reconciling the role of serotonin in behavioral inhibition and aversion: acute tryptophan depletion abolishes punishment-induced inhibition in humans
J Neurosci
,
2009
, vol.
29
(pg.
11993
-
9
)
Cubillo
A
Smith
AB
Barrett
N
Giampietro
V
Brammer
MJ
Simmons
A
et al.
,
Shared and drug-specific effects of atomoxetine and methylphenidate on inhibitory brain dysfunction in medication-naive ADHD boys
Cereb Cortex
,
2012
, vol.
24
(pg.
174
-
85
)
Danet
M
Lapiz-Bluhm
MD
Morilak
DA
,
A cognitive deficit induced in rats by chronic intermittent cold stress is reversed by chronic antidepressant treatment
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol
,
2012
, vol.
13
(pg.
997
-
1099
)
Dayan
P
Huys
QJ
,
Serotonin, inhibition, and negative mood
PLoS Comput Biol
,
2008
, vol.
4
pg.
e4
Del-Ben
CM
Deakin
JF
McKie
S
Delvai
NA
Williams
SR
Elliott
R
et al.
,
The effect of citalopram pretreatment on neuronal responses to neuropsychological tasks in normal volunteers: an FMRI study
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2005
, vol.
30
(pg.
1724
-
34
)
Drueke
B
Boecker
M
Schlaegel
S
Moeller
O
Hiemke
C
Gründer
G
et al.
,
Serotonergic modulation of response inhibition and re-engagement? Results of a study in healthy human volunteers
Hum Psychopharmacol
,
2010
, vol.
25
(pg.
472
-
80
)
Eagle
DM
Bari
A
Robbins
TW
,
The neuropsychopharmacology of action inhibition: cross-species translation of the stop-signal and go/no-go tasks
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2008
, vol.
199
(pg.
439
-
56
)
Eagle
DM
Lehmann
O
Theobald
DE
Pena
Y
Zakaria
R
Ghosh
R
et al.
,
Serotonin depletion impairs waiting but not stop-signal reaction time in rats: implications for theories of the role of 5-HT in behavioral inhibition
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2009
, vol.
34
(pg.
1311
-
21
)
Fletcher
PJ
Tampakeras
M
Sinyard
J
Higgins
GA
,
Opposing effects of 5-HT(2A) and 5-HT(2C) receptor antagonists in the rat and mouse on premature responding in the five-choice serial reaction time test
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2007
, vol.
195
(pg.
223
-
34
)
Furr
A
Lapiz-Bluhm
MD
Morilak
DA
,
5-HT2A receptors in the orbitofrontal cortex facilitate reversal learning and contribute to the beneficial cognitive effects of chronic citalopram treatment in rats
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol
,
2012
, vol.
15
(pg.
1295
-
305
)
Gauggel
S
Rieger
M
Feghoff
TA
,
Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson's disease
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
,
2004
, vol.
75
(pg.
539
-
44
)
Goldstein
DS
Sullivan
P
Holmes
C
Kopin
IJ
Basile
MJ
Mash
DC
,
Catechols in post-mortem brain of patients with Parkinson disease
Eur J Neurol
,
2011
, vol.
18
(pg.
703
-
10
)
Harmer
CJ
Bhagwagar
Z
Perrett
DI
Völlm
BA
Cowen
PJ
Goodwin
GM
,
Acute SSRI administration affects the processing of social cues in healthy volunteers
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2003
, vol.
28
(pg.
148
-
52
)
Harmer
CJ
Cowen
PJ
,
‘It's the way that you look at it’–a cognitive neuropsychological account of SSRI action in depression
Philos Trans R Soc B Biol
,
2013
, vol.
368
pg.
20120407
Harrison
AA
Everitt
BJ
Robbins
TW
,
Central serotonin depletion impairs both the acquisition and performance of a symmetrically reinforced go/no-go conditional visual discrimination
Behav Brain Res
,
1999
, vol.
100
(pg.
99
-
112
)
Haugbøl
S
Pinborg
LH
Regeur
L
Hansen
ES
Bolwig
TG
Nielsen
FA
et al.
,
Cerebral 5-HT2A receptor binding is increased in patients with Tourette's syndrome
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol
,
2007
, vol.
10
(pg.
245
-
52
)
Higgins
GA
Enderlin
M
Haman
M
Fletcher
PJ
,
The 5-HT2A receptor antagonist M100,907 attenuates motor and ‘impulsive-type’ behaviours produced by NMDA receptor antagonism
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2003
, vol.
170
(pg.
309
-
19
)
Homberg
JR
Pattij
T
Janssen
MC
Ronken
E
De Boer
SF
Schoffelmeer
AN
et al.
,
Serotonin transporter deficiency in rats improves inhibitory control but not behavioural flexibility
Eur J Neurosci
,
2007
, vol.
26
(pg.
2066
-
73
)
Huey
ED
Putnam
KT
Grafman
J
,
A systematic review of neurotransmitter deficits and treatments in frontotemporal dementia
Neurology
,
2006
, vol.
66
(pg.
17
-
22
)
Hughes
LE
Barker
RA
Owen
AM
Rowe
JB
,
Parkinson's disease and healthy aging: independent and interacting effects on action selection
Hum Brain Mapp
,
2010
, vol.
31
(pg.
1886
-
99
)
Humpston
CS
Wood
CM
Robinson
ES
,
Investigating the roles of different monoamine transmitters and impulse control using the 5-choice serial reaction time task
J Psychopharmacol
,
2013
, vol.
27
(pg.
213
-
21
)
Khullar
S
Michael
A
Correa
N
Adali
T
Baum
SA
Calhoun
VD
,
Wavelet-based fMRI analysis: 3-D denoising, signal separation, and validation metrics
Neuroimage
,
2011
, vol.
54
(pg.
2867
-
84
)
Kirby
LG
Zeeb
FD
Winstanley
CA
,
Contributions of serotonin in addiction vulnerability
Neuropharmacology
,
2011
, vol.
61
(pg.
421
-
32
)
LeMarquand
DG
Benkelfat
C
Pihl
RO
Palmour
RM
Young
SN
,
Behavioral disinhibition induced by tryptophan depletion in nonalcoholic young men with multigenerational family histories of paternal alcoholism
Am J Psych
,
1999
, vol.
156
(pg.
1771
-
9
)
Levy
BJ
Wagner
AD
,
Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating
Ann N Y Acad Sci
,
2011
, vol.
1224
(pg.
40
-
62
)
Lewis
SJ
Dove
A
Robbins
TW
Barker
RA
Owen
AM
,
Cognitive impairments in early Parkinson's disease are accompanied by reductions in activity in frontostriatal neural circuitry
JNeurosci
,
2003
, vol.
23
(pg.
6351
-
6
)
Logan
GD
Cowan
WB
,
On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a theory of an act of control
Psychol Rev
,
1984
, vol.
91
(pg.
295
-
327
)
Macoveanu
J
Hornboll
B
Elliott
R
Erritzoe
D
Paulson
OB
Siebner
H
et al.
,
Serotonin 2A receptors, citalopram and tryptophan-depletion: a multimodal imaging study of their interactions during response inhibition
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2013
, vol.
38
(pg.
996
-
1005
)
Mendelsohn
D
Riedel
WJ
Sambeth
A
,
Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on memory, attention and executive functions: a systematic review
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
,
2009
, vol.
33
(pg.
926
-
52
)
Miyazaki
KW
Miyazaki
K
Doya
K
,
Activation of the central serotonergic system in response to delayed but not omitted rewards
Eur J Neurosci
,
2011
, vol.
33
(pg.
153
-
60
)
Moret
C
Briley
M
,
Effects of acute and repeated administration of citalopram on extracellular levels of serotonin in rat brain
Eur J Pharmacol
,
1996
, vol.
295
(pg.
189
-
97
)
Nandam
LS
Hester
R
Wagner
J
Cummins
TD
Garner
K
Dean
AJ
et al.
,
Methylphenidate but not atomoxetine or citalopram modulates inhibitory control and response time variability
Biol Psychiatry
,
2011
, vol.
69
(pg.
902
-
4
)
Nombela
C
Rittman
T
Robbins
TW
Rowe
JB
,
Multiple modes of impulsivity in Parkinson's disease
PLoS One
,
2014
, vol.
9
pg.
e85747
Obeso
I
Wilkinson
L
Casabona
E
Bringas
ML
Álvarez
M
Álvarez
L
et al.
,
Deficits in inhibitory control and conflict resolution on cognitive and motor tasks in Parkinson's disease
Exp Brain Res
,
2011
, vol.
212
(pg.
371
-
84
)
Ortega
JE
Mendiguren
A
Pineda
J
Meana
JJ
,
Regulation of central noradrenergic activity by 5-HT(3) receptors located in the locus coeruleus of the rat
Neuropharmacology
,
2012
, vol.
62
(pg.
2472
-
9
)
Pauls
AM
O'Daly
OG
Rubia
K
Riedel
WJ
Williams
SC
Mehta
MA
,
Methylphenidate effects on prefrontal functioning during attentional-capture and response inhibition
Biol Psychiatry
,
2012
, vol.
72
(pg.
142
-
9
)
Peelle
JE
Eason
RJ
Schmitter
S
Schwarzbauer
C
Davis
MH
,
Evaluating an acoustically quiet EPI sequence for use in fMRI studies of speech and auditory processing
Neuroimage
,
2010
, vol.
52
(pg.
1410
-
9
)
Politis
M
Wu
K
Loane
C
Kiferle
L
Molloy
S
Brooks
DJ
et al.
,
Staging of serotonergic dysfunction in Parkinson's disease: an in vivo 11C-DASB PET study
Neurobiol Dis
,
2010
, vol.
40
(pg.
216
-
21
)
Politis
M
Wu
K
Loane
C
Quinn
NP
Brooks
DJ
Oertel
WH
et al.
,
Serotonin neuron loss and nonmotor symptoms continue in Parkinson's patients treated with dopamine grafts
Sci Transl Mede
,
2012
, vol.
4
pg.
128ra41
Rae
CL
Correia
MM
Altena
E
Hughes
L
Barker
RA
Rowe
JB
,
White matter pathology in Parkinson's disease: The effect of imaging protocol differences and relevance to executive function
Neuroimage
,
2012
, vol.
62
(pg.
1675
-
84
)
Rae
CL
Hughes
L
Weaver
C
Anderson
MC
Rowe
JB
,
Selecting and stopping in voluntary action: a meta-analysis and novel fMRI study
Neuroimage
,
2014
, vol.
86
(pg.
381
-
91
)
Rawlings
NB
Norbury
R
Cowen
PJ
Harmer
CJ
,
A single dose of mirtazapine modulates neural responses to emotional faces in healthy people
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2010
, vol.
212
(pg.
625
-
34
)
Robbins
TW
,
Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications
Philos Trans R Soc B Biol
,
2007
, vol.
362
(pg.
917
-
32
)
Robinson
ES
Dalley
JW
Theobald
DE
Glennon
JC
Pezze
MA
Murphy
ER
et al.
,
Opposing roles for 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors in the nucleus accumbens on inhibitory response control in the 5-choice serial reaction time task
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2008
, vol.
33
(pg.
2398
-
406
)
Robinson
OJ
Sahakian
BJ
,
Recurrence in major depressive disorder: a neurocognitive perspective
Psychol Med
,
2008
, vol.
38
(pg.
315
-
8
)
Rowe
JB
Hughes
L
Ghosh
BCP
Eckstein
D
Williams-Gray
CH
Fallon
S
et al.
,
Parkinson's disease and dopaminergic therapy—differential effects on movement, reward and cognition
Brain
,
2008
, vol.
131
(pg.
2094
-
105
)
Rubia
K
Lee
F
Cleare
AJ
Tunstall
N
Fu
CH
Brammer
M
et al.
,
Tryptophan depletion reduces right inferior prefrontal activation during response inhibition in fast, event-related fMRI
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
,
2005
, vol.
179
(pg.
791
-
803
)
Rubia
K
Russell
T
Overmeyer
S
Brammer
MJ
Bullmore
ET
Sharma
T
et al.
,
Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks
Neuroimage
,
2001
, vol.
13
(pg.
250
-
61
)
Sangkuhl
K
Klein
TE
Altman
RB
,
PharmGKB summary: citalopram pharmacokinetics pathway
Pharmacogenet Genomics
,
2011
, vol.
21
(pg.
769
-
72
)
Sharp
DJ
Bonnelle
V
De Boissezon
X
Beckmann
CF
James
SG
Patel
MC
et al.
,
Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing
Proc Natl Acad Scis USA
,
2010
, vol.
107
(pg.
6106
-
11
)
Smith
SM
Jenkinson
M
Johansen-Berg
H
Rueckert
D
Nichols
TE
Mackay
CE
et al.
,
Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data
Neuroimage
,
2006
, vol.
31
(pg.
1487
-
505
)
Smith
SM
Nichols
TE
,
Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference
Neuroimage
,
2009
, vol.
44
(pg.
83
-
98
)
Swick
D
Ashley
V
Turken
U
,
Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks
Neuroimage
,
2011
, vol.
56
(pg.
1655
-
65
)
Tomlinson
CL
Stowe
R
Patel
S
Rick
C
Gray
R
Clarke
CE
,
Systematic review of Levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson's disease
Mov Disord
,
2010
, vol.
25
(pg.
2649
-
85
)
Völlm
B
Richardson
P
McKie
S
Elliott
R
Deakin
JF
Anderson
IM
,
Serotonergic modulation of neuronal responses to behavioural inhibition and reinforcing stimuli: an fMRI study in healthy volunteers
Eur J Neurosci
,
2006
, vol.
23
(pg.
552
-
60
)
Voon
V
Mehta
MA
Hallett
M
,
Impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease: recent advances
Curr Opin Neurol
,
2011a
, vol.
24
(pg.
324
-
30
)
Voon
V
Sohr
M
Lang
AE
Potenza
MN
Siderowf
AD
Whetteckey
J
et al.
,
Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a multicenter case—control study
Ann Neurol
,
2011b
, vol.
69
(pg.
986
-
96
)
Walker
SC
Robbins
TW
Roberts
AC
,
Differential contributions of dopamine and serotonin to orbitofrontal cortex function in the marmoset
Cereb Cortex
,
2009
, vol.
19
(pg.
889
-
98
)
Weintraub
D
Doshi
J
Koka
D
Davatzikos
C
Siderowf
AD
Duda
JE
et al.
,
Neurodegeneration across stages of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease
Arch Neurol
,
2011
, vol.
68
(pg.
1562
-
8
)
Weintraub
D
Koester
J
Potenza
MN
Siderowf
AD
Stacy
M
Voon
V
et al.
,
Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a cross-sectional study of 3090 patients
Arch Neurol
,
2010
, vol.
67
(pg.
589
-
95
)
Winstanley
CA
Theobald
DE
Dalley
JW
Robbins
TW
,
Interactions between serotonin and dopamine in the control of impulsive choice in rats: therapeutic implications for impulse control disorders
Neuropsychopharmacology
,
2005
, vol.
30
(pg.
669
-
82
)
Yuen
EY
Jiang
Q
Chen
P
Gu
Z
Feng
J
Yan
Z
,
Serotonin 5-HT1A receptors regulate NMDA receptor channels through a microtubule-dependent mechanism
J Neurosci
,
2005
, vol.
25
(pg.
5488
-
501
)
Zepf
FD
Holtmann
M
Stadler
C
Demisch
L
Schmitt
M
Wöckel
L
et al.
,
Diminished serotonergic functioning in hostile children with ADHD: tryptophan depletion increases behavioural inhibition
Pharmacopsychiatry
,
2008
, vol.
41
(pg.
60
-
5
)
Zhang
J
Hughes
LE
Rowe
JB
,
Selection and inhibition mechanisms for human voluntary action decisions
Neuroimage
,
2012
, vol.
63
(pg.
392
-
402
)
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data