Abstract

Among mammals, bovids provide some of the most striking examples of sexual dimorphism in colouration and pelage appendages, such as beards and manes. This dimorphism is usually assumed to have evolved through sexual selection on males in the context of intra- or intersexual communication. However, the sexes coloration and pelage appendages look similar between the two sexes in several bovid species thought to be characterized by large opportunities for sexual selection, hinting at fitness costs of dimorphic traits due to other selection pressures. This study applies the comparative method with phylogenetic control to identify the factors promoting and constraining the evolution of dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages across bovids. We found that trait dimorphism correlated positively with large breeding group size, an indicator of the intensity of sexual selection, and negatively with male territoriality, which is also likely to affect the operation of sexual selection. The relative rarity of color and pelage dimorphism in species with territorial mating systems may be explained by weaker sexual selection due to difficulty in monopolizing females and/or sexual selection targeting other traits, such as territorial quality as an extended phenotype. We also found that dimorphism in color and pelage was reduced in species spending more time in mixed-sex groups outside the breeding season, possibly due to increased predation costs from non-uniformity. This suggests that benefits from integration into mixed-sex groups select against the extravagant male morphologies otherwise promoted by sexual selection.

INTRODUCTION

Marked differences in morphology between males and females can be found in a wide range of taxa, and often encompass a variety of traits, from body size and weaponry (Pérez-Barberìa et al. 2002; Cassini 2020) to coloration and various appendages (e.g., in birds: Møller and Pomiankowski 1993; in primates: Grueter et al. 2015; Cooney et al. 2019; Lüpold et al. 2019). Darwin’s formulation of sexual selection presented a breakthrough in our understanding of the evolutionary origin of sexual dimorphism (Darwin 1871). However, aspects of the taxonomic distribution of sexually dimorphic traits continue to puzzle evolutionary biologists (Lande 1980; Andersson and Iwasa 1996; Shultz and Burns 2017). In particular, why is sexual dimorphism seemingly weak in a large number of species thought to be characterized by intense sexual competition?

Among mammals, the Bovidae offers striking variation in sexual dimorphism, from the practically monomorphic wildebeest (Connochaetes taurinus) and duikers (Cephalopus sp.), to species where the bold appearances of males contrast conspicuously with those of females, such as the blackbuck (Antilope cervicapra), many wild goats and sheep, and members of the bushbuck tribe (Schaller 1977; Estes 1991a; Caro 2009). What may explain these differences between species? Although previous research has demonstrated that dimorphism in body size and horns is linked primarily to the opportunity for sexual selection (Loison et al 1999; Bro-Jørgensen 2007), dimorphism in coloration and the presence of manes, beards, hair tufts, and other pelage appendages is less well understood.

According to sexual selection theory, sexual dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages may result from stronger reproductive competition in males: such competition could lead to sex-specific benefits from signaling individual quality to mates and/or competitive ability to rivals (Lande and Arnold 1985; Andersson and Simmons 2006). Theoretically, inter- and intrasexual signals exclusive to males are expected to be more widespread in species with a high opportunity for sexual selection (Clutton-Brock et al. 1980; Grueter et al. 2015; Cassini 2020). The opportunity for sexual selection depends on the variance in mating success among reproductive males, which is in turn affected by the female group size and the male mating strategy: these factors influence the number of mates one male can potentially monopolize (Shuster 2009; Bro-Jørgensen 2011).

In bovids, male mating strategies can be divided into territorial and non-territorial. In territorial species, males defend defined areas attractive to females, due to either the resources they contain, or their location (notably in lek systems). Conversely, males of non-territorial species follow and defend females directly, particularly when they are in estrus (Gosling 1986; Isvaran 2005). Although molecular paternity studies remain few (Coltman et al. 2002; Festa-Bianchet et al. 2019), evidence from behavioral studies suggests that territoriality may reduce the potential for polygyny, as females often range over multiple territories, forcing males to share reproductive opportunities with rivals (Gosling 1986; Bro-Jørgensen 2007, 2011). By contrast, access to estrous females in non-territorial systems is generally controlled by males that attain top rank in dominance hierarchies and monopolize breeding groups by excluding subordinates from mating (Gosling 1986; Bro-Jørgensen 2007, 2011). For a given female group size, non-territorial strategies may therefore be expected to show a greater potential for male polygyny, which translates into heightened competition and stronger selection for male dimorphic traits than in territorial systems.

Male mating strategies can also affect the evolution of signaling systems by determining which male traits are primarily targeted by sexual selection (Andersson 1994). In territorial ungulates, attributes of male territories may substitute for morphological signals as sources of information in intra- and inter-sexual interactions (Owen-Smith 1977; Jarman 1983;Apollonio et al. 1989; Vanpé et al. 2008, 2009). Females in these systems often choose mating partners based on territorial location or availability of resources, rather than on male physical characteristics (e.g., Balmford et al. 1992; Bro-Jørgensen 2002; Bro-Jørgensen and Durant 2003). By using territorial quality in mate selection, females may obtain both direct benefits associated with increased resource availability and indirect genetic benefits because only competitively superior males can acquire high-quality territories (Owen-Smith 1977; Jarman 1983). If territorial properties are widely used as a spatial reference for male quality, it is possible that information encoded in costly morphological signal traits becomes redundant, resulting in reduced dimorphism. Specifically, in the handful of bovids that facultatively adopt lek territoriality, selection may not necessarily favor conspicuous signaling traits in males even if male mating skew is often thought to be pronounced. We therefore hypothesized that the evolution of male ornaments as signals of individual quality may be more pronounced: 1) in species forming larger groups and 2) in non-territorial species, the latter both because of a greater potential for polygyny and because territorial quality may serve as an alternative indicator of individual quality in territorial species (Sexual Selection hypothesis).

Differences in the intensity of sexual selection and in mating strategies, however, cannot account for why dimorphic colorations and appendages failed to evolve in many highly polygynous non-territorial bovids. A possibility is that variation in predation costs also influences differences between species in their degree of sexual dimorphism. The antipredator strategy of many social animals, from fish to mammals, relies on uniformity in morphology which makes it easier to confuse predators and prevent them from singling out a target in a group (Landeau and Terborgh 1986; Krause and Ruxton 2002). Because bovid species differ markedly in the degree to which they form mixed-sex groups, variation in sexual dimorphism may depend on the propensity for the sexes to aggregate outside the breeding season. Sexual segregation may be caused by sex-differences in predation risk, driving females to seek safer areas, differences in forage requirements, leading females to prioritize food quality more over quantity, and—especially—differences in activity budgets, with females spending more time feeding due to smaller body size (Ruckstuhl and Neuhaus 2002). We hypothesized that the selective pressure for monomorphism to facilitate predation confusion is strongest in species that spend more time in mixed-sex groups outside the breeding season (Visual Integration hypothesis).

Habitat-related differences in antipredator strategy may also affect the intensity of selection for conspicuous morphologies in males. Although open habitat species typically rely on safety-in-numbers for protection against predators, species in closed habitats rely more on concealment, which is expected to select for similar morphology in the two sexes (Zuk and Kolluru 1998; Stuart-Fox and Ord 2004; Caro 2005a). Moreover, closed habitats may also interfere with transmission of visual signals (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011; Bossu and Near 2015; Price 2017). Previous studies have found that grassland bovids are characterized by more elaborate and sharper pelage markings than the generally uniform or disruptive colorations of forest species, leading to the suggestion that the latter might be less reliant on vision than on other sensory channels (Stoner et al. 2003; Caro and Stankowich 2010). We thus hypothesized that color and pelage dimorphism is more pronounced in open habitats, where the predation cost of conspicuousness is lower and visual communication is not obstructed by physical barriers (Habitat hypothesis).

In this study, we applied the comparative method with phylogenetic control to test the key hypotheses proposed above to explain variation among bovid species in the dimorphism of coloration and pelage appendages, such as manes and beards (Table 1). According to the Sexual Selection hypothesis, we predicted that trait dimorphism increases with the potential for male polygyny, i.e., breeding group size (an indicator of the number of females per mating male; Wade and Shuster 2004), and with male non-territoriality (an indicator of female monopolisability as well as the importance of morphological rather than spatial attributes as targets for sexual selection). According to the Visual Integration hypothesis, we predicted that trait dimorphism would decrease with the propensity to form mixed-sex groups outside the breeding season. Finally, according to the Habitat hypothesis, we predicted that trait dimorphism would increase with the openness of habitats occupied.

Table 1

Hypotheses and predictions for the evolution of morphological dimorphism in bovids

HypothesesPredictors of morphological dimorphism
Group sizeMating strategySexual aggregationHabitat openness
Sexual selection++ (non-territorial)00
Visual integration000
Habitat000+
HypothesesPredictors of morphological dimorphism
Group sizeMating strategySexual aggregationHabitat openness
Sexual selection++ (non-territorial)00
Visual integration000
Habitat000+

Symbols in cells refers to the expected correlation between the degree of color and pelage dimorphism, and selected independent variables (0 = no correlation; + = positive correlation; − = negative correlation).

Table 1

Hypotheses and predictions for the evolution of morphological dimorphism in bovids

HypothesesPredictors of morphological dimorphism
Group sizeMating strategySexual aggregationHabitat openness
Sexual selection++ (non-territorial)00
Visual integration000
Habitat000+
HypothesesPredictors of morphological dimorphism
Group sizeMating strategySexual aggregationHabitat openness
Sexual selection++ (non-territorial)00
Visual integration000
Habitat000+

Symbols in cells refers to the expected correlation between the degree of color and pelage dimorphism, and selected independent variables (0 = no correlation; + = positive correlation; − = negative correlation).

METHODS

Indices for sexual dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages

Data on sexual dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages were collected for 110 of the 136 bovid species. The data used in the final analyses relate to the subspecies for which the most detailed information could be obtained. Domesticated subspecies were excluded from the study (e.g., in Bos frontalis, B. grunniens, and Capra aegagrus), because domestication is likely to alter adaptive colorations and morphologies. Sexual dimorphism was scored from photographs in lateral view compiled from Costello (2016) and online resources (http://arkive.org; www.encyclopediaoflife.com; www.ultimateungulate.com). Depending on availability, between two (one male, one female) and eight (four males, four females) images were selected for each species. All images used depicted the same subspecies and—for boreal species with different summer and winter coat—represented the same season.

Sexual dimorphism in coloration (CSD) was assessed on eight distinct body regions, following Caro and Stankowich (2010): 1) head; 2) neck; 3) flank, shoulder, humerus; 4) rump, femur; 5) upper front leg (i.e., elbow, ulna); 6) lower front leg (i.e., podials, metapodials); 7) upper hind leg (i.e., knee, tibia); and 8) lower hind leg (i.e., podials, metapodials). Coloration in bovids cannot be considered independently of the presence of contrasting white or black markings, in the form of distinctive stripes, blotches, or spots, against lighter or darker background hair (Stoner et al. 2003; Caro and Stankowich 2010). In order to account for variation in both hair pigmentation and presence/absence of contrast markings, dimorphism for each body region was scored according to a three-point scale: 0—no difference; 1—difference in background color or contrast markings (i.e., presence in one sex only); 2—difference in both background color and contrast markings. Background hair color was compared to a reference of five color categories (modified from Caro et al. 2017): 1) white (de-pigmented); 2) phaeomelanin—yellow/red; 3) eumelanin—brown; 4) eumelanin—grey; and 5) eumelanin—black (Figure 1). Background color was scored as dimorphic if the same body region was assigned to different categories in males and females. Differences in nuances within the established color categories (e.g., dark versus light brown) were not evaluated, because variable lighting conditions, hair length, and gland secretions could have affected the perceived intensity (Caro 2005b; Caro et al. 2017). Following Caro et al. (2017), the CSD index was calculated as the sum of the scores for all eight body regions.

Bovid hair color based on the dominant type of pigment (i.e., no pigment, phaeomelanin, or eumelanin). For each color category, different gradations are presented as examples of variability in pigment saturation. Bovids were conservatively scored as dimorphic for differences between these categories. This classification was based on examination of high-quality pictures where single hair could be easily detected by the naked eye. [Numbers indicate the species used for reference: 1. Dall’s sheep male (Ovis dalli); 2. Banteng female (Bos javanicus); 3. Oribi male (Ourebia ourebi); 4. Four-horned antelope male (Tetracerus quadricornis); 5. Bongo female (Tragelaphus eurycerus); 6. Barbary sheep male (Ammotragus lervia); 7. European bison female (Bison bonasus); 8. Alpine ibex male (Capra ibex); 9. Muskox male (Ovibos moschatus); 10. Blue sheep female (Pseudois nayaur); 11. Lesser kudu male (Tragelaphus imberbis); 12. Nilgai male (Boselaphus tragocamelus); 13. Asiatic wild water buffalo male (Bubalus bubalis); 14. Sable antelope male (Hippotragus niger)].
Figure 1

Bovid hair color based on the dominant type of pigment (i.e., no pigment, phaeomelanin, or eumelanin). For each color category, different gradations are presented as examples of variability in pigment saturation. Bovids were conservatively scored as dimorphic for differences between these categories. This classification was based on examination of high-quality pictures where single hair could be easily detected by the naked eye. [Numbers indicate the species used for reference: 1. Dall’s sheep male (Ovis dalli); 2. Banteng female (Bos javanicus); 3. Oribi male (Ourebia ourebi); 4. Four-horned antelope male (Tetracerus quadricornis); 5. Bongo female (Tragelaphus eurycerus); 6. Barbary sheep male (Ammotragus lervia); 7. European bison female (Bison bonasus); 8. Alpine ibex male (Capra ibex); 9. Muskox male (Ovibos moschatus); 10. Blue sheep female (Pseudois nayaur); 11. Lesser kudu male (Tragelaphus imberbis); 12. Nilgai male (Boselaphus tragocamelus); 13. Asiatic wild water buffalo male (Bubalus bubalis); 14. Sable antelope male (Hippotragus niger)].

Sexual dimorphism in pelage appendages (PASD) was scored as an index based on inter-sexual differences in: 1) frontal hair tuft; 2) beard or ventral mane; 3) dorsal mane; 4) cape (covering both dorsal and ventral parts of the neck); 5) front leg pantaloons; and 6) hind leg pantaloons (based on Estes 2000). A four-point visual scale was used: 0—no difference in the pelage appendage between the sexes; 1—the appendage differs in either size or color between the sexes; 2—the appendage differs in both size and color between the sexes; 3—the appendage is present in one sex only (always male; Grueter et al. 2015). The PASD index was calculated as the sum of scores for all the pelage appendages present in a given species.

To assess repeatability of the scoring system, CSD and PASD were scored by a second observer blind to the study hypotheses based on two randomly selected pictures (one male and one female) for each study species (n = 110). The two sets of scores were highly correlated (CSD: Kendall’s T = 0.978; P < 0.0001; PASD: Kendall’s T = 0.923; P < 0.0001).

Independent variables

Data on average breeding group size and male mating strategy were derived from literature sources listed in the Supplementary Material. Male mating strategy was classified as a binary trait: territorial (T), or non-territorial (NT). Lek breeding territoriality is reported from only four bovid species and always co-exists with resource-defense territoriality (Clutton-Brock et al. 1993); due to this small sample size, lekking species were not considered separately from other territorial species.

Following Estes (1991a) and Roberts (1996), a three-point scale was used to score the tendency for sexual aggregation outside the breeding season: 1—never forming mixed-sex groups with multiple males (including species in which males are solitary or live in segregated, unisexual groups, only joining female groups in search of mating opportunities); 2—sometimes forming multi-male mixed-sex groups (including species in which only some males are found in multimale mixed-sex groups, either because not all males join these groups or because associations are temporary, e.g., during migrations); and 3—generally forming multi-male mixed-sex groups (including species in which the sexes typically aggregate, although some males may temporarily join bachelor groups; Jarman 1983). The scoring was based on available published material (Supplementary Material).

Following Cabrera and Stankowich (2018), habitat openness was scored based on the probability of detecting large mammals in each of the nine different IUCN habitat categories (www.iucnredlist.org): tropical forest (0.1), temperate forest (0.2), wetland (0.3), tropical shrubland (0.5), temperate shrubland (0.6), savannah (0.7), grassland (0.8), rocky areas (0.8), and desert (0.95). The species-specific score for habitat openness was calculated as the mean detection probability across all habitat categories occupied by the species (Cabrera and Stankowich 2018).

Statistical analyses

All analyses were conducted in R 3.5.2 (R Development Core Team 2019), with the packages ape (Paradis et al. 2019) and caper (Orme et al. 2018) loaded in the main workspace. Phylogenetic Generalized Least Squares (PGLS) analyses were conducted to identify statistically significant predictors of colour and pelage dimorphism (Freckleton et al. 2002; Mundry 2014). The PGLS algorithm controls for correlations arising due to shared ancestry, which do not reflect independent evolutionary events, by including phylogeny as a variance-covariance matrix of branch lengths in the error structure of an ordinary least squares regression model (Freckleton et al. 2002). Branch lengths were scaled using Pagel’s lambda to estimate maximum likelihood (Freckleton et al. 2002), selecting the best fitting branch transformation using the profile.pgls function in the package caper (Orme et al. 2018). Delta and kappa branch transformations produced qualitatively similar results (not reported here). The phylogenetic tree was obtained from Bininda-Emonds et al. (2007), which incorporates both molecular and morphological data and offers a comprehensive species coverage for bovids. When using the phylogenetic tree in Upham et al. (2019), similar results (not shown) were obtained.

Dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages (CSD and PASD) was modeled as response variables in separate PGLS models. Explanatory variables initially included in both models were breeding group size, male mating strategy, sexual association pattern, and habitat openness. Normal distribution of residuals was achieved by log(x + 1) transformation of CSD, log-transformation of all continuous variables in the CSD models, and square-root-transformation of PASD and all continuous variables in the PASD models. Assumptions of residual normality and heteroscedasticity were tested using the plot.pgls function in the package caper (Orme et al. 2018). The final models were constructed by removing predictors in order of least significance until only significant predictors remained (P < 0.05, two-sided; Murtaugh 2014). Coefficient estimates for non-significant predictors refer to the results obtained when adding each of them separately to the final models. Variance Inflation Factors (VIFs) were calculated to assess the effect of multicollinearity between explanatory variables; in all cases, VIFs were ≤ 3 and thus below the accepted threshold of concern at 5–10 (Kutner et al. 2005).

RESULTS

The degree of sexual dimorphism in coloration (CSD) and in pelage appendages (PASD) in bovids was both predicted by breeding group size, male mating strategy, and sexual aggregation (CSD: F3,83 = 20.320; λ = 0.066; P = <0.001; R2 = 0.403; PASD: F3,83 = 4.785; λ = 0.952; P = 0.003; R2 = 0.117; Tables 2 and 3). Both measures of dimorphism were positively correlated with breeding group size and non-territorial mating strategies, as predicted by the Sexual Selection hypothesis (Figure 2). Only 4 of 64 territorial species exhibited some form of color dimorphism, in contrast to 26 of the 46 non-territorial species. Similarly, pelage dimorphism characterized 19 of 46 non-territorial bovids, but only one out of 64 territorial species (the sable antelope Hippotragus niger). Supporting the Visual Integration hypothesis, both CSD and PASD were negatively correlated with the level of sexual aggregation (Tables 2 and 3), with none of the species that consistently aggregate in mixed-sex groups outside the breeding season scored as dimorphic (Figure 3). Finally, no support was found for the Habitat hypothesis, as no correlation was found between habitat openness and neither CSD or PASD (Tables 2 and 3).

Table 2

PGLS correlations between the degree of color dimorphism in bovids and various socio-ecological traits

CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.2610.0813.2130.002
Non-territorial mating strategy0.7950.1724.621<0.001
Sexual aggregation−0.7720.161−4.791<0.001
Habitat openness−0.3860.382−1.0120.314
CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.2610.0813.2130.002
Non-territorial mating strategy0.7950.1724.621<0.001
Sexual aggregation−0.7720.161−4.791<0.001
Habitat openness−0.3860.382−1.0120.314

Coefficients estimates and statistical significance are provided for a multivariate model containing all significant predictors (F3,83 = 20.320; λ = 0.066; P = <0.001; R2 = 0.403).

Statistically significant P-values are highlighted in bold.

Table 2

PGLS correlations between the degree of color dimorphism in bovids and various socio-ecological traits

CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.2610.0813.2130.002
Non-territorial mating strategy0.7950.1724.621<0.001
Sexual aggregation−0.7720.161−4.791<0.001
Habitat openness−0.3860.382−1.0120.314
CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.2610.0813.2130.002
Non-territorial mating strategy0.7950.1724.621<0.001
Sexual aggregation−0.7720.161−4.791<0.001
Habitat openness−0.3860.382−1.0120.314

Coefficients estimates and statistical significance are provided for a multivariate model containing all significant predictors (F3,83 = 20.320; λ = 0.066; P = <0.001; R2 = 0.403).

Statistically significant P-values are highlighted in bold.

Table 3

PGLS correlations between degree of dimorphism in pelage appendages in bovids and various socio-ecological traits

CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.0760.0372.0640.042
Non-territorial mating strategy0.5170.2322.2320.028
Sexual aggregation−0.3260.117−2.7930.006
Habitat openness−0.2630.318−0.8280.501
CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.0760.0372.0640.042
Non-territorial mating strategy0.5170.2322.2320.028
Sexual aggregation−0.3260.117−2.7930.006
Habitat openness−0.2630.318−0.8280.501

Coefficients estimates and statistical significance are provided for a multivariate model containing all significant predictors (F3,83 = 4.785; λ = 0.952; P = 0.003; R2 = 0.117).

Statistically significant P-values (<0.05) are highlighted in bold.

Table 3

PGLS correlations between degree of dimorphism in pelage appendages in bovids and various socio-ecological traits

CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.0760.0372.0640.042
Non-territorial mating strategy0.5170.2322.2320.028
Sexual aggregation−0.3260.117−2.7930.006
Habitat openness−0.2630.318−0.8280.501
CoefficientS.E.tP
Group size (log)0.0760.0372.0640.042
Non-territorial mating strategy0.5170.2322.2320.028
Sexual aggregation−0.3260.117−2.7930.006
Habitat openness−0.2630.318−0.8280.501

Coefficients estimates and statistical significance are provided for a multivariate model containing all significant predictors (F3,83 = 4.785; λ = 0.952; P = 0.003; R2 = 0.117).

Statistically significant P-values (<0.05) are highlighted in bold.

Coloration dimorphism in bovid species with territorial (T) and non-territorial (NT) male mating strategies. The points correspond to the single species included in the study, whereas the violin plot describes the probability distribution of the data.
Figure 2

Coloration dimorphism in bovid species with territorial (T) and non-territorial (NT) male mating strategies. The points correspond to the single species included in the study, whereas the violin plot describes the probability distribution of the data.

Proportion of species dimorphic in color (A) and pelage appendages (B) according to sexual aggregation (n = 110).
Figure 3

Proportion of species dimorphic in color (A) and pelage appendages (B) according to sexual aggregation (n = 110).

DISCUSSION

This study provides evidence that sexual dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages of bovids is promoted by sexual selection on males. Dimorphism in both traits correlated positively with breeding group size and non-territorial rather than territorial mating strategies, two factors affecting the level of male polygyny and hence the intensity of sexual selection. The results also revealed negative correlations between sexual dimorphism in the two traits studied and the tendency to aggregate in mixed-sex groups outside the breeding season, suggesting that a selective pressure for monomorphism in such groups limits the evolution of divergent male morphology.

Male territoriality characterizes nearly two-thirds of the bovid species, and the reduced dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages found in these species may derive from a smaller potential for polygyny due to difficulty in monopolizing female groups (Gosling 1986; Cassini 2020). A parallel can be drawn to male weaponry, as horns and antlers are generally shorter and less elaborate in territorial ungulates (Bro-Jørgensen 2007; Plard et al. 2011). Furthermore, as mentioned above, sexual selection in territorial systems could target male traits other than morphological signals, including behaviors associated with territorial advertisement, such as scent marking and vocalizations (Owen-Smith 1977). Hence, several highly polygynous bovids in which males defend territories showed negligible differences between the sexes in colouration and pelage appendages [e.g., Alpine chamois (Rupicapra rupicapra); Corlatti et al. 2015; and impala; Aepyceros melampus; Jarman 1979]. The preponderance of color and pelage dimorphism in non-territorial species, on the other hand, suggests that in these taxa conspicuous and elaborate color and hair designs have a widespread function as signals in inter- or intrasexual interactions. Since female mate choice may have a more modest role in determining mating success than competition between males (with forced copulations by dominant individuals reported in some non-territorial taxa, e.g., Pelletier et al. 2006; Mainguy et al. 2008; Ihl and Bowyer 2011), dimorphic traits may have been selected through their role in male intrasexual confrontations (Loehr et al. 2008; Bowyer et al. 2020). Several field studies have indeed found support for male coloration and pelage appendages as signals of competitive ability, with trait expression mirroring changes in dominance rank of males in eland (Tragelaphus oryx; Bro-Jørgensen and Beeston 2015), Himalayan tahr (Hemitragus jemlahicus; Lovari et al. 2015) as well as in other mammalian families, most notably primates (Setchell and Wickings 2005; Bergman et al. 2009).

Our study found morphological dimorphism in bovids to be weaker in species where multiple males and females aggregate outside the breeding season. This was exemplified by the complete absence of CSD and PASD in all species which usually form mixed-sex groups (African buffalo Syncerus caffer, muskox Ovibos moschatus, addax Addax nasomaculatus, and oryx antelopes Oryx spp.), despite the fact that their large breeding group sizes and male non-territoriality suggest high potential for polygyny. Conversely, some of highest scores for the CSD and PASD indices were assigned to species that usually segregate into unisexual groups, even though their smaller group sizes point to lower potential for polygyny (e.g., sitatunga Tragelaphus spekii and greater kudu Tragelaphus strepsiceros). Limited evidence suggests that similar variation may be observed within species: in the mountain bongo (Tragelaphus eurycerus isaaci), which is reportedly more sexually segregated than the lowland subspecies (T. e. eurycerus), males have a more distinctive color, sometimes nearly black rather than reddish brown (Hillman 1986; Estes 1991b). The evidence thus suggests that extravagant male morphology might impose significant costs in mixed-sex groups, offsetting the fitness benefits deriving from sexual signaling.

The lack of dimorphic pelage ornaments in taxa where mixed-sex groups predominate agrees with our Visual Integration hypothesis. Although the exact function of female horns is still debated, previous studies suggest that also horns are less dimorphic in species forming mixed-sex groups (Estes 1991b; Roberts 1996), pointing to a general pattern of reduced sexual dimorphism in species where the sexes aggregate. In schooling fish, “confusion effects” generated by multiple, indistinguishable targets decrease the attack success of predators (Penry-Williams et al. 2018; Rystrom et al. 2018), and a selective advantage from reduced predation risk may thus cause male and female phenotypes to converge. Confusion effects are also employed in the antipredator tactics of gregarious ungulates (Geist and Bayer 1988; Caro et al. 2004; Caro 2005a), and this may promote monomorphism of species forming mixed-sex groups. Morphological dimorphism tends to be rare also in species where males join females only for shorter periods of time, but when this happens at times of the year when exposure to predation risk is particularly high, such as during resource shortages or mass movements (Owen-Smith 2008).

An alternative proposition attributes monomorphism in species forming mixed-sex groups to costs that subordinate males incur from sex-specific morphologies because revelation of sexual identity triggers aggression from dominants (Estes 1991a, 2000). Studies of oryx antelope (Oryx beisa) have thus linked the observation that subordinate males are not evicted from social groups as long as they do not attempt mating to their visual resemblance of females, which is thought to reduce intrasexual aggression (Walther 1978), presumably through some form of sensory exploitation. However, sexual identity is often detected through olfactory rather than visual cues in ungulates (Gosling and Roberts 2001).

We found no evidence that closed habitats attenuated selection on male visual signals due to reduced transmission efficacy or an antipredator strategy relying on hiding in that no correlation was found between habitat openness and CSD or PASD. Hence, some closed habitat species (habitat openness < 0.5) were highly dimorphic, e.g., in the bushbuck tribe (Estes 1991b). A general association between sexual monomorphism and dense habitats reported previously (e.g., Estes 2000) may thus be primarily due to smaller breeding group size in closed environments, which restrict opportunities for sexual selection in forest species (Jarman 1974).

A limitation of our study is that it relies on assumptions about the relationship between socioecological factors and male mating skew, which still awaits confirmation by molecular paternity assignment for the vast majority of bovid species in the wild (the few species from which information is available include the bighorn sheep Ovis canadensis and the mountain goat Oreamnos americanus; Coltman et al. 2002; Festa-Bianchet et al. 2019). From interspecific comparative studies, positive associations between both non-territoriality and female group size on one side and traits used in male combat, such as horn length and body size, on the other indirectly support reduced potential for polygyny in territorial species and species with smaller female groups (Jarman 1983; Bro-Jørgensen 2007). However, additional genetic studies on individual variance in lifetime reproductive success will be necessary to fully understand the association between different mating strategies and the intensity of sexual selection on males for dimorphic ornaments.

We conclude that interspecific variation in sexual dimorphism in coloration and pelage appendages in bovids reflects underlying differences in the intensity of sexual selection, and in the propensity to form mixed-sex groups outside the breeding season. It is possible that the selective advantage of monomorphism in taxa where the sexes aggregate is a more pervasive evolutionary force than previously acknowledged. For example, the absence of dimorphism in colouration and ornaments in other highly polygynous lineages of mammals, such as equids, kangaroos, and some primates (e.g., savannah baboons Papio spp. Jarman 1983; Dixson et al. 2005), may be explained by their tendency to aggregate in mixed-sex groups and warrants further comparative studies to test the applicability of our visual integration hypothesis at a broader taxonomic scale.

FUNDING

This study was supported by Natural Environmental Research Council of the UK, via an Adapting to the Challenges of a Changing Environment Doctoral Training scholarship (1793192 ACCE DTP) to G.D’A.

We thank the Natural Environment Research Council of the UK (NERC) and the University of Liverpool for financial support to G.D’A. We also express our gratitude to A. McElligott, T. Price, K. Meise, P. Stockley, J. Hurst, and all the staff and students of the Mammalian Behaviour and Evolution Group at the University of Liverpool, for constructive comments on the study. We are grateful to M. Festa-Bianchet and to an anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Analyses reported in this article can be reproduced using the data provided by D’Ammando et al. (2022).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interests.

REFERENCES

Andersson
M
.
1994
.
Sexual selection
.
Princeton (NJ)
:
Princeton University Press
.

Andersson
M
,
Iwasa
Y
.
1996
.
Sexual selection
.
Trends Ecol Evol.
11
:
53
58
.

Andersson
M
,
Simmons
LW
.
2006
.
Sexual selection and mate choice
.
Trends Ecol Evol.
21
:
296
302
.

Apollonio
M
,
Festa-Bianchet
M
,
Mari
F
.
1989
.
Correlates of copulatory success in a fallow deer lek
.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol.
25
:
89
97
.

Balmford
A
,
Rosser
AM
,
Albon
SD
.
1992
.
Correlates of female choice in resource-defending antelope
.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol.
31
:
107
114
.

Bergman
TJ
,
Ho
L
,
Beehner
JC
.
2009
.
Chest color and social status in male geladas (Theropithecus gelada)
.
Int J Primatol.
30
:
791
806
.

Bininda-Emonds
OR
,
Cardillo
M
,
Jones
KE
,
MacPhee
RD
,
Beck
RM
,
Grenyer
R
,
Purvis
A
.
2007
.
The delayed rise of present-day mammals
.
Nature
446
:
507
512
.

Bossu
CM
,
Near
TJ
.
2015
.
Ecological constraint and the evolution of sexual dichromatism in darters
.
Evolution
69
:
1219
1231
.

Bowyer
RT
,
McCullough
DR
,
Rachlow
JL
,
Ciuti
S
,
Whiting
JC
.
2020
.
Evolution of ungulate mating systems: integrating social and environmental factors
.
Ecol Evol.
11
:
5160
5178
.

Bradbury
JW
,
Vehrencamp
SL
.
2011
.
Principles of animal communication
.
Suderland (USA)
:
Sinauer
.

Bro-Jørgensen
J
.
2002
.
Overt female mate competition and preference for central males in a lekking antelope
.
Proc Nat Acad Sci.
99
:
9290
9293
.

Bro-Jørgensen
J
.
2007
.
The intensity of sexual selection predicts weapon size in male bovids
.
Evolution
61
:
1316
1326
.

Bro-Jørgensen
J
.
2011
.
Intra-and intersexual conflicts and cooperation in the evolution of mating strategies: lessons learnt from ungulates
.
Evol Biol.
38
:
28
41
.

Bro-Jørgensen
J
,
Beeston
J
.
2015
.
Multimodal signalling in an antelope: fluctuating facemasks and knee-clicks reveal the social status of eland bulls
.
Anim Behav.
102
:
231
239
.

Bro-Jørgensen
J
,
Durant
SM
.
2003
.
Mating strategies of topi bulls: getting in the centre of attention
.
Anim Behav.
65
:
585
594
.

Cabrera
D
,
Stankowich
T
.
2018
.
Stabbing slinkers: tusk evolution among Artiodactyls
.
J Mammal Evol
.
27
:
265
272
.

Caro
T
.
2005b
.
The adaptive significance of coloration in mammals
.
BioScience
55
:
125
136
.

Caro
T
.
2005a
.
Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals
.
Chicago (IL)
:
University of Chicago Press
.

Caro
T
.
2009
.
Contrasting coloration in terrestrial mammals
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci.
364
:
537
548
.

Caro
T
,
Stankowich
T
.
2010
.
The function of contrasting pelage markings in artiodactyls
.
Behav Ecol.
21
:
78
84
.

Caro
T
,
Walker
H
,
Rossman
Z
,
Hendrix
M
,
Stankowich
T
.
2017
.
Why is the giant panda black and white?
Behav Ecol.
28
:
657
667
.

Caro
TM
,
Graham
CM
,
Stoner
CJ
,
Vargas
JK
.
2004
.
Adaptive significance of antipredator behaviour in artiodactyls
.
Anim Behav.
67
:
205
228
.

Cassini
MH
.
2020
.
Sexual size dimorphism and sexual selection in artiodactyls
.
Behav Ecol.
31
:
792
797
.

Clutton-Brock
TH
,
Albon
SD
,
Harvey
PH
.
1980
.
Antlers, body size and breeding group size in the Cervidae
.
Nature
285
:
565
567
.

Clutton-Brock
TH
,
Deutsch
JC
,
Nefdt
RJC
.
1993
.
The evolution of ungulate leks
.
Anim Behav.
46
:
1121
1138
.

Coltman
DW
,
Festa-Bianchet
M
,
Jorgenson
JT
,
Strobeck
C
.
2002
.
Age-dependent sexual selection in bighorn rams
.
Proc Biol Sci.
269
:
165
172
.

Cooney
CR
,
Varley
ZK
,
Nouri
LO
,
Moody
CJ
,
Jardine
MD
,
Thomas
GH
.
2019
.
Sexual selection predicts the rate and direction of colour divergence in a large avian radiation
.
Nat Commun.
10
:
1
9
.

Corlatti
L
,
Bassano
B
,
Poláková
R
,
Fattorini
L
,
Pagliarella
MC
,
Lovari
S
.
2015
.
Preliminary analysis of reproductive success in a large mammal with alternative mating tactics, the Northern chamois, Rupicapra rupicapra
.
Biol J Linn Soc.
116
:
117
123
.

Costello
JR
.
2016
.
Bovids of the world
.
Princeton (NJ)
:
Princeton University Press
.

D’Ammando
G
,
Frank
DW
,
Bro-Jorgensen
J
2022
.
Data from: living in mixed-sex groups limits sexual dimorphism in colouration and pelage appendages in bovids
.
Behav Ecol
. doi: 10.5061/dryad.w0vt4b8tb.

Darwin
C
.
1871
.
The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex
.
London (UK)
:
John Murray
.

Dixson
A
,
Dixson
B
,
Anderson
M
.
2005
.
Sexual selection and the evolution of visually conspicuous sexually dimorphic traits in male monkeys, apes, and human beings
.
Ann Rev Sex Res.
16
:
1
19
.

Estes
RD
.
1991a
.
The behaviour guide to African mammals: including hoofed mammals, carnivores, primates
.
Johannesburg (South Africa)
:
Russell Friedman Books CC
.

Estes
RD
.
1991b
.
The significance of horns and other male secondary sexual characters in female bovids
.
Appl Anim Behav Sci.
29
:
403
451
.

Estes
RD
.
2000
.
Evolution of conspicuous coloration in the Bovidae: female mimicry of male secondary sexual characters as a catalyst.
In
Schaller
GB
,
Vrba
ES
, editors.
Antelopes, deer and relatives
.
New Haven (CT)
:
Yale University Press
. p.
234
246
.

Festa-Bianchet
M
,
Côté
SD
,
Hamel
S
,
Pelletier
F
.
2019
.
Long-term studies of bighorn sheep and mountain goats reveal fitness costs of reproduction
.
J Anim Ecol.
88
:
1118
1133
.

Freckleton
RP
,
Harvey
PH
,
Pagel
M
.
2002
.
Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: a test and review of evidence
.
Am Nat.
160
:
712
726
.

Geist
V
,
Bayer
M
.
1988
.
Sexual dimorphism in the Cervidae and its relation to habitat
.
J Zool.
214
:
45
53
.

Gosling
LM
.
1986
.
The evolution of mating strategies in male antelopes.
In:
Rubenstein
DI
,
Wrangham
RW
, editors.
Ecological aspects of social evolution
.
Princeton (NJ)
:
Princeton University Press
. p.
244
281
.

Gosling
LM
,
Roberts
SC
.
2001
.
Scent-marking by male mammals: cheat-proof signals to competitors and mates
.
Adv Study Behav.
30
:
169
217
.

Grueter
CC
,
Isler
K
,
Dixson
BJ
.
2015
.
Are badges of status adaptive in large complex primate groups?
Evol Human Behav.
36
:
398
406
.

Hillman
JC
.
1986
.
Aspects of the biology of the bongo antelope Tragelaphus eurycerus Ogilby 1837 in South West Sudan
.
Biol Conserv.
38
:
255
272
.

Ihl
C
,
Bowyer
RT
.
2011
.
Leadership in mixed-sex groups of muskoxen during the snow-free season
.
J Mammal.
92
:
819
827
.

Isvaran
K
.
2005
.
Variation in male mating behaviour within ungulate populations: patterns and processes
.
Curr Sci
.
89
:
1192
1199
.

Jarman
MV
.
1979
.
Impala social behaviour: territory, hierarchy, mating, and the use of space (Advances in
Ethology 21)
.
Hamburg (Germany)
:
Parey
.

Jarman
P
.
1974
.
The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecology
.
Behaviour
48
:
215
267
.

Jarman
P
.
1983
.
Mating system and sexual dimorphism in large terrestrial, mammalian herbivores
.
Biol Rev.
58
:
485
520
.

Krause
JD
,
Ruxton
GD
.
2002
Living in groups
.
Oxford (UK)
:
Oxford University Press
.

Kutner
MH
,
Nachtsheim
CJ
,
Neter
J
,
Li
W
.
2005
.
Applied linear statistical models
(Vol.
5
).
Irvine (CA)
:
McGraw-Hill
.

Lande
R
.
1980
.
Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection, and adaptation in polygenic characters
.
Evolution
34
:
292
305
.

Lande
R
,
Arnold
SJ
.
1985
.
Evolution of mating preference and sexual dimorphism
.
J Theor Biol.
117
:
651
664
.

Landeau
L
,
Terborgh
J
.
1986
.
Oddity and the “confusion effect” in predation
.
Anim Behav.
34
:
1372
1380
.

Loehr
J
,
Carey
J
,
Ylönen
H
,
Suhonen
J
.
2008
.
Coat darkness is associated with social dominance and mating behaviour in a mountain sheep hybrid lineage
.
Anim Behav.
76
:
1545
1553
.

Loison
A
,
Gaillard
JM
,
Pélabon
C
,
Yoccoz
NG
.
1999
.
What factors shape sexual size dimorphism in ungulates?
Evol Ecol Research
.
1
:
611
633
.

Lovari
S
,
Fattorini
N
,
Boesi
R
,
Bocci
A
.
2015
.
Male ruff colour as a rank signal in a monomorphic-horned mammal: behavioural correlates
.
Naturwissenschaften
102
:
39
.

Lüpold
S
,
Simmons
LW
,
Grueter
CC
.
2019
.
Sexual ornaments but not weapons trade off against testes size in primates
.
Proc Biol Sci.
286
:
20182542
.

Mainguy
J
,
Côté
SD
,
Cardinal
E
,
Houle
M
.
2008
.
Mating tactics and mate choice in relation to age and social rank in male mountain goats
.
J Mammal.
89
:
626
635
.

Møller
AP
,
Pomiankowski
A
.
1993
.
Why have birds got multiple sexual ornaments?
Behav Ecol Sociobiol.
32
:
167
176
.

Mundry
R
.
2014
.
Statistical issues and assumptions of phylogenetic generalized least squares.
In
Garamszegi
LZ
, editor.
Modern phylogenetic comparative methods and their application in evolutionary biology
.
Berlin and Heidelberg (Germany)
:
Springer
. p.
131
153
.

Murtaugh
PA
.
2014
.
In defense of P values
.
Ecology.
95
:
611
617
.

Orme
D
,
Freckleton
R
,
Thomas
G
,
Petzoldt
T
,
Fritz
S
.
2018
.
The caper package: comparative analysis of phylogenetics and evolution in R
. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=caper.

Owen-Smith
N
.
1977
.
On territoriality in ungulates and an evolutionary model
.
Q Rev Biol.
52
:
1
38
.

Owen-Smith
N
.
2008
.
Changing vulnerability to predation related to season and sex in an African ungulate assemblage
.
Oikos
117
:
602
610
.

Paradis
E
,
Blomberg
S
,
Bolker
B
,
Brown
J
,
Claude
J
,
Cuong
HS
,
Desper
R
.
2019
.
Package “ape”
. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ape.

Pelletier
F
,
Hogg
JT
,
Festa-Bianchet
M
.
2006
.
Male mating effort in a polygynous ungulate
.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol.
60
:
645
654
.

Penry-Williams
IL
,
Ioannou
CC
,
Taylor
MI
.
2018
.
The oddity effect drives prey choice but not necessarily attack time
.
Ethology
124
:
496
503
.

Pérez-Barbería
FJ
,
Gordon
IJ
,
Pagel
M
.
2002
.
The origins of sexual dimorphism in body size in ungulates
.
Evolution
56
:
1276
1285
.

Plard
F
,
Bonenfant
C
,
Gaillard
JM
.
2011
.
Revisiting the allometry of antlers among deer species: male–male sexual competition as a driver
.
Oikos
120
:
601
606
.

Price
TD
.
2017
.
Sensory drive, color, and color vision
.
Am Nat.
190
:
157
170
.

R Development Core Team
2019
.
A language environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing
,
Vienna, Austria
. https://www.R-project.org/.

Roberts
SC
.
1996
.
The evolution of hornedness in female ruminants
.
Behaviour
133
:
399
442
.

Ruckstuhl
KE
,
Neuhaus
P
.
2002
.
Sexual segregation in ungulates: a comparative test of three hypotheses
.
Biol Rev
.
77
:
77
96
.

Rystrom
TL
,
Clement
VF
,
Rick
IP
,
Bakker
TC
,
Mehlis
M
.
2018
.
Shoal sex composition and predation risk influence sub-adult threespine stickleback shoaling decisions
.
Behav Proc.
157
:
495
501
.

Schaller
GB
.
1977
.
Mountain monarchs. Wild sheep and goats of the Himalaya
.
Chicago (IL)
:
University of Chicago Press
.

Setchell
JM
,
Jean Wickings
E
.
2005
.
Dominance, status signals and coloration in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx)
.
Ethology
111
:
25
50
.

Shultz
AJ
,
Burns
KJ
.
2017
.
The role of sexual and natural selection in shaping patterns of sexual dichromatism in the largest family of songbirds (Aves: Thraupidae)
.
Evolution
71
:
1061
1074
.

Shuster
SM
.
2009
.
Sexual selection and mating systems
.
Proc N Acad Sci.
106
:
10009
10016
.

Stoner
CJ
,
Caro
TM
,
Graham
CM
.
2003
.
Ecological and behavioral correlates of coloration in artiodactyls: systematic analyses of conventional hypotheses
.
Behav Ecol.
14
:
823
840
.

Stuart–Fox
DM
,
Ord
TJ
.
2004
.
Sexual selection, natural selection and the evolution of dimorphic coloration and ornamentation in agamid lizards
.
Proc Biol Sci
.
271
:
2249
2255
.

Upham
NS
,
Esselstyn
JA
,
Jetz
W
.
2019
.
Inferring the mammal tree: species-level sets of phylogenies for questions in ecology, evolution, and conservation
.
PLoS Biol.
17
:
e3000494
.

Vanpé
C
,
Kjellander
P
,
Galan
M
,
Cosson
JF
,
Aulagnier
S
,
Liberg
O
,
Hewison
AM
.
2008
.
Mating system, sexual dimorphism, and the opportunity for sexual selection in a territorial ungulate
.
Behav Ecol.
19
:
309
316
.

Vanpé
C
,
Morellet
N
,
Kjellander
P
,
Goulard
M
,
Liberg
O
,
Hewison
AM
.
2009
.
Access to mates in a territorial ungulate is determined by the size of a male’s territory, but not by its habitat quality
.
J Anim Ecol.
78
:
42
51
.

Wade
MJ
,
Shuster
SM
.
2004
.
Sexual selection: harem size and the variance in male reproductive success
.
Am Nat.
164
:
83
89
.

Walther
FR
.
1978
.
Behavioral observations on oryx antelope (Oryx beisa) invading Serengeti National Park, Tanzania
.
J Mammal.
59
:
243
260
.

Zuk
M
,
Kolluru
GR
.
1998
.
Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids
.
Q Rev Biol.
73
:
415
438
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://dbpia.nl.go.kr/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)
Handling Editor: Michael D Jennions
Michael D Jennions
Handling Editor
Search for other works by this author on: