Abstract

Background

NULevel was a randomized control trial to evaluate a technology-assisted weight loss maintenance (WLM) program in the UK. The program included: (a) a face-to-face goal-setting session; (b) an internet platform, a pedometer, and wirelessly connected scales to monitor and report diet, physical activity, and weight, and; (c) regular automated feedback delivered by mobile phone, tailored to participants’ progress. Components were designed to target psychological processes linked to weight-related behavior. Though intervention participants showed increased physical activity, there was no difference in WLM between the intervention and control groups after 12 months (Sniehotta FF, Evans EH, Sainsbury K, et al. Behavioural intervention for weight loss maintenance versus standard weight advice in adults with obesity: A randomized controlled trial in the UK (NULevel Trial). PLoS Med. 2019; 16(5):e1002793. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002793). It is unclear whether the program failed to alter targeted psychological processes, or whether changes in these processes failed to influence WLM.

Purpose

We evaluate whether the program influenced 16 prespecified psychological processes (e.g., self-efficacy and automaticity toward diet and physical activity), and whether these processes (at 6 months) were associated with successful WLM (at 12 months).

Methods

288 adults who had previously lost weight were randomized to the intervention or control groups. The control group received wireless scales and standard advice via newsletters. Assessments occurred in person at 0, 6, and 12 months.

Results

The intervention significantly altered 10 of the 16 psychological processes, compared with the control group. However, few processes were associated with WLM, leading to no significant indirect effects of the intervention via the processes on WLM.

Conclusions

Changes in targeted processes were insufficient to support WLM. Future efforts may more closely examine the sequence of effects between processes, behavior, and WLM.

Lay Summary

Many tools exist to help people lose weight, but it is common for people to regain that weight over time. Thus, understanding how to support the maintenance of weight loss remains a priority. The NULevel program was a 12-month weight loss maintenance (WLM) intervention for individuals who had recently lost weight. It promoted psychological factors, shown to be tied to weight-related behaviors, using face-to-face and technology-based (e.g., mobile phone feedback) elements. For example, the program encouraged making plans to improve lifestyles (e.g., exercise, better diet) and promoted people’s confidence in these behaviors. However, the program was not more successful than a control condition in maintaining weight loss. We sought to understand why this occurred. We found that the program was indeed successful in influencing most of the psychological factors it targeted. Instead, it was the psychological factors that failed to predict WLM. Were the psychological factors insufficient to impact behavior? Or did the promoted behaviors fail to aid WLM? Future research should focus on answering such questions. Doing so would inform whether interventions should target different psychological factors to change behaviors, or choose different sets of behaviors to support WLM.

To help manage health effects related to high body weight, it is beneficial for intervention strategies to be available that not only help people pursue weight loss goals, but also maintain weight loss over time. Yet, strategies that support weight loss maintenance (WLM) have proven difficult to identify—an issue that remains a key challenge in obesity management [1]. The NULevel trial tested a scalable, low-intensity, technology-assisted intervention to support WLM over a 12-month period [2]. Participants, who had an initial body mass index (BMI) ≥30 and then lost at least 5% of their bodyweight prior to entering the trial, experienced limited weight gain (mean = 1.8 kg) during the trial period; however, the rate of weight gain did not differ significantly between those randomized to the intervention or the control condition [3]. Why was the intervention not superior to the control condition? Did it fail to engage the psychological processes it was designed to target (e.g., self-efficacy, planning, and automaticity)? Were changes in these processes unrelated to WLM? Or might another explanation underlie these results? The present paper addresses these questions using data from the NULevel trial that tracked changes in a set of 16 targeted psychosocial constructs over the 12-month study period.

Mapping an Intervention Effect for NULevel Using the Experimental Medicine Approach

The effectiveness of behavioral interventions relies on their ability to elicit change in one or more target constructs (e.g., self-efficacy, planning) that in turn change a desired health outcome (e.g., body weight). This framework is captured by a series of paths that comprise the Experimental Medicine approach (see Fig. 1A)—one path links the intervention to the targeted construct(s) (i.e., the engagement path) and the other links the targeted construct(s) to the primary outcome (i.e., the validity path [4, 5]). Elucidating the operation of these paths is a key part of intervention process evaluation as it can clarify why an intervention is or is not effective.

Theoretical models guiding our analyses. The upper panel (Panel A) outlines our project within the experimental medicine (EM) approach to health behavior change. The lower panel (Panel B) provides a directed acyclic graph (DAG) delineating how the variables in Panel A play are theorized to operate over the three time points of the NULevel trial. Panel B also represents a path diagram that matches the path analytic models computed in the current project. Path a captures the direct effect of the intervention on psychological processes at 6 months (6M). Path b captures the direct effect of the psychological process (measured at 6 months) on weight at 12 months (12M). The composite Path a × b captures the indirect effect of the intervention on weight at 12 months (12M) attributed to changes on the process at 6 months (6M; accounting for paths c and d × e). Path c captures the direct effect of the intervention on weight at 12 months (12M; accounting for paths a × b and d × e). Path d captures the direct effect of the intervention on weight at 6 months (6M). Path e captures the direct effect of weight at 6 months (6M) on weight at 12 months (12M). Finally, the composite Path d × e captures the indirect effect of the intervention on weight at 12 months (12M) attributed to changes on weight at 6 months (6M). Panel B also shows the theorized roles of weight and the psychological processes at baseline (B) as they relate to other variables in the model.
Fig. 1.

Theoretical models guiding our analyses. The upper panel (Panel A) outlines our project within the experimental medicine (EM) approach to health behavior change. The lower panel (Panel B) provides a directed acyclic graph (DAG) delineating how the variables in Panel A play are theorized to operate over the three time points of the NULevel trial. Panel B also represents a path diagram that matches the path analytic models computed in the current project. Path a captures the direct effect of the intervention on psychological processes at 6 months (6M). Path b captures the direct effect of the psychological process (measured at 6 months) on weight at 12 months (12M). The composite Path a × b captures the indirect effect of the intervention on weight at 12 months (12M) attributed to changes on the process at 6 months (6M; accounting for paths c and d × e). Path c captures the direct effect of the intervention on weight at 12 months (12M; accounting for paths a × b and d × e). Path d captures the direct effect of the intervention on weight at 6 months (6M). Path e captures the direct effect of weight at 6 months (6M) on weight at 12 months (12M). Finally, the composite Path d × e captures the indirect effect of the intervention on weight at 12 months (12M) attributed to changes on weight at 6 months (6M). Panel B also shows the theorized roles of weight and the psychological processes at baseline (B) as they relate to other variables in the model.

The NULevel intervention was grounded on the premise that WLM is a function of sustained patterns of dietary behavior, physical activity, and weight monitoring. To promote such sustained behaviors, the program was developed in line with a systematic review of theories of behavioral maintenance [6], with a particular focus on self-regulation theories [7, 8]—which we observed to underpin previously successful WLM interventions [9]. Such theories emphasize that a person’s success at maintaining behaviors (e.g., greater physical activity, improved diet) generally depends on their ability to confidently enact self-regulation strategies (e.g., self-monitoring, action planning), particularly in the face of challenges (e.g., using coping planning to resist temptations). The theories further emphasize the importance of sustaining motivation throughout goal pursuit (e.g., such as by feeling satisfaction with obtained changes) and of maintaining adequate regulatory resources (e.g., automatizing behaviors to conserve energy [6]).

The current intervention engaged with the themes above by targeting five classes of constructs: (a) people’s perceptions of control over, and confidence in, engaging in weight-related behaviors (i.e., physical activity, dietary change, and self-weighing); (b) their energy levels for these behaviors; (c) their ability to develop and implement plans to manage these behaviors; (d) the degree to which these behaviors become automated; and (e) their satisfaction with the outcomes afforded by changes in their weight. To target these constructs, intervention participants engaged in an initial goal-setting session and used an internet-based platform to track their diet, physical activity, and weight. Participants were also given a pedometer and a wireless scale, with instructions to weigh themselves daily. Feedback via text messages and phone were then incorporated to further promote the psychological variables [2, 3].

Leveraging measures of the targeted constructs at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months, we examined two exploratory questions: (a) During the first 6 months of the trial, did the intervention elicit greater change in the targeted constructs than did the control condition?, and (b) To what extent did initial changes in the targeted constructs lead to change in weight during the last 6 months of the trial? Answers to the first question will indicate whether the intervention engaged the constructs it was designed to target, and answers to the second question will indicate whether the targeted constructs were successful predictors of weight maintenance.

Methods

The NULevel trial was a registered two-armed randomized controlled superiority trial [10]. The analyses making up the current work were also preregistered prior to conducting analyses [11], outlining measures to be used, how measures were to be scored, and analyses to be conducted. The current work reports all preregistered analyses without deviation.

Sample

Participants were recruited in England between April 2014 and May 2015. Participants were 288 adults (≥18 years) who (a) had lost 5% or more of their weight in the 12 months preceding the trial and (b) had had a BMI equal to or greater than 30 kg/m2 (or ≥28 kg/m2 for individuals of South Asian descent) within 12 months preceding the NULevel trial. Participants had a mean age of 41.8 years, a mean baseline weight of 85.6 kg, and a mean baseline BMI of 30.9 kg/m2. Additionally, 77% identified as female (33% as male), about half (49%) had a household income of £40,000 or above, and most were employed full (61%) or part-time (17%). Overall, 144 participants were randomly allocated to the intervention group and 144 to the control group. Further details on recruitment, inclusion and exclusion criteria, randomization, and participant demographics were previously reported [2, 3].

NULevel Arms

Detailed descriptions of the intervention design and control groups are reported elsewhere [2, 3]. In brief, the intervention lasted 12 months and consisted of elements such as a face-to-face goal-setting session, along with access to an internet-based platform, a pedometer, and a wirelessly connected scale—which participants were instructed to use to regularly monitor and report their diet, physical activity, and weight. The intervention group was given regular feedback tailored to people’s goal progress, delivered via short message service (SMS) messages with links to mobile internet content. A “traffic light” system was also set up to trigger contacts (by phone) with the intervention team based on participant weight regain. This system allowed the intervention to be less intensive when participants’ weight was stable.

Intervention elements were designed to target processes tied to WLM behavior change, including the 16 variables in Table 1. For example, during the face-to-face sessions, participants made plans to engage in physical activity and healthy eating, and generated strategies to overcome barriers–engaging in action planning and coping planning, while bolstering their confidence and capability in enacting the behaviors. SMS messages were used to encourage further planning and goal regulation, prompt behaviors and habit-formation (automaticity), and to highlight past and current successes (reinforcing satisfaction with outcomes).

Table 1

Summary of the 16 Psychological Process Variables Measured

Process variableDescriptionExample item (number of items)Scalingαs
01. Satisfaction with changesSatisfaction with weight change related outcomes (e.g., self-esteem, fit in clothes).How satisfied are you with [change in] your self-esteem? (11)1 = unhappy/dissatisfied; 3 = happy/satisfied0.91
0.94
02. PBC: Healthy eatingConfidence in and perceived ease of eating healthy foods in moderation.How confident are you in your ability to eat healthy foods in moderation? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.75
0.86
03. PBC: Physical activityConfidence in and perceived ease of being physically active every day.How confident are you in your ability to be physically active every day? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.83
0.86
04. Confidence: Weight lossConfidence in ability to lose weight.How confident are you in your ability to lose weight? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
05. Confidence: WLMConfidence in ability to maintain weight loss.How confident are you in your ability to maintain weight loss? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
06. SE: Emotional eatingPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods when experiencing negative affectI can resist eating unhealthy food when I am anxious. (3)1 = False; 4 = True0.91
0.95
07. SE: Unhealthy food contextPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods in the face of contextual barriers.I can resist eating unhealthy food even when I am at a party. (10)1 = False; 4 = True0.86
0.93
08. SE: Physical activity barriersPerceived capacity to engage in physical activity even in the face of barriers.I will be physically active every day even when I am sad. (12)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.96
09. Action planning: physical activityMaking concrete plans to be physically active.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to be physically active. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.95
0.96
10. Action planning: Healthy eatingMaking concrete plans to make healthy food choices.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to make healthy food choices. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.97
11. Coping planning: Physical activityMaking plans to overcome barriers against physical activity.I have made a detailed plan regarding how to keep on being physically active in difficult situations. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.96
12. Coping planning: Healthy eatingPlanning to overcome barriers and temptations against healthy eating.I have made a detailed plan regarding what to do if I’m tempted by unhealthy foods. (4)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.94
0.95
13. Automaticity: Healthy eatingAutomaticity of healthy eating behavior.Making healthy food choices is something I do automatically. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.93
0.95
14. utomaticity: Physical activityAutomaticity of doing physical activity behavior.Being physically active is something I do without thinking. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.96
0.97
15. Automaticity: Self-weighingAutomaticity of engaging in self-weighing behavior.Weighing myself is something I do without having to consciously remember. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.97
16. Energy and driveFeeling energetic and driven, as opposed to exhausted and fatigued.Over the last 2 weeks I have felt full of vitality. (12)1 = None of the time; 5 = All of the time0.93
0.94
Process variableDescriptionExample item (number of items)Scalingαs
01. Satisfaction with changesSatisfaction with weight change related outcomes (e.g., self-esteem, fit in clothes).How satisfied are you with [change in] your self-esteem? (11)1 = unhappy/dissatisfied; 3 = happy/satisfied0.91
0.94
02. PBC: Healthy eatingConfidence in and perceived ease of eating healthy foods in moderation.How confident are you in your ability to eat healthy foods in moderation? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.75
0.86
03. PBC: Physical activityConfidence in and perceived ease of being physically active every day.How confident are you in your ability to be physically active every day? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.83
0.86
04. Confidence: Weight lossConfidence in ability to lose weight.How confident are you in your ability to lose weight? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
05. Confidence: WLMConfidence in ability to maintain weight loss.How confident are you in your ability to maintain weight loss? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
06. SE: Emotional eatingPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods when experiencing negative affectI can resist eating unhealthy food when I am anxious. (3)1 = False; 4 = True0.91
0.95
07. SE: Unhealthy food contextPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods in the face of contextual barriers.I can resist eating unhealthy food even when I am at a party. (10)1 = False; 4 = True0.86
0.93
08. SE: Physical activity barriersPerceived capacity to engage in physical activity even in the face of barriers.I will be physically active every day even when I am sad. (12)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.96
09. Action planning: physical activityMaking concrete plans to be physically active.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to be physically active. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.95
0.96
10. Action planning: Healthy eatingMaking concrete plans to make healthy food choices.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to make healthy food choices. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.97
11. Coping planning: Physical activityMaking plans to overcome barriers against physical activity.I have made a detailed plan regarding how to keep on being physically active in difficult situations. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.96
12. Coping planning: Healthy eatingPlanning to overcome barriers and temptations against healthy eating.I have made a detailed plan regarding what to do if I’m tempted by unhealthy foods. (4)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.94
0.95
13. Automaticity: Healthy eatingAutomaticity of healthy eating behavior.Making healthy food choices is something I do automatically. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.93
0.95
14. utomaticity: Physical activityAutomaticity of doing physical activity behavior.Being physically active is something I do without thinking. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.96
0.97
15. Automaticity: Self-weighingAutomaticity of engaging in self-weighing behavior.Weighing myself is something I do without having to consciously remember. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.97
16. Energy and driveFeeling energetic and driven, as opposed to exhausted and fatigued.Over the last 2 weeks I have felt full of vitality. (12)1 = None of the time; 5 = All of the time0.93
0.94

αs lowest and highest Standardized Reliability Alphas (for 3+ item scales) or r (for 2 items scales) across the three time points of the study; n/a not applicable; PBC perceived behavioral control; Scaling example anchors used with items, reflecting the minimum/maximum possible scores; SE self-efficacy; WLM weight loss maintenance.

Table 1

Summary of the 16 Psychological Process Variables Measured

Process variableDescriptionExample item (number of items)Scalingαs
01. Satisfaction with changesSatisfaction with weight change related outcomes (e.g., self-esteem, fit in clothes).How satisfied are you with [change in] your self-esteem? (11)1 = unhappy/dissatisfied; 3 = happy/satisfied0.91
0.94
02. PBC: Healthy eatingConfidence in and perceived ease of eating healthy foods in moderation.How confident are you in your ability to eat healthy foods in moderation? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.75
0.86
03. PBC: Physical activityConfidence in and perceived ease of being physically active every day.How confident are you in your ability to be physically active every day? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.83
0.86
04. Confidence: Weight lossConfidence in ability to lose weight.How confident are you in your ability to lose weight? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
05. Confidence: WLMConfidence in ability to maintain weight loss.How confident are you in your ability to maintain weight loss? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
06. SE: Emotional eatingPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods when experiencing negative affectI can resist eating unhealthy food when I am anxious. (3)1 = False; 4 = True0.91
0.95
07. SE: Unhealthy food contextPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods in the face of contextual barriers.I can resist eating unhealthy food even when I am at a party. (10)1 = False; 4 = True0.86
0.93
08. SE: Physical activity barriersPerceived capacity to engage in physical activity even in the face of barriers.I will be physically active every day even when I am sad. (12)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.96
09. Action planning: physical activityMaking concrete plans to be physically active.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to be physically active. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.95
0.96
10. Action planning: Healthy eatingMaking concrete plans to make healthy food choices.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to make healthy food choices. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.97
11. Coping planning: Physical activityMaking plans to overcome barriers against physical activity.I have made a detailed plan regarding how to keep on being physically active in difficult situations. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.96
12. Coping planning: Healthy eatingPlanning to overcome barriers and temptations against healthy eating.I have made a detailed plan regarding what to do if I’m tempted by unhealthy foods. (4)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.94
0.95
13. Automaticity: Healthy eatingAutomaticity of healthy eating behavior.Making healthy food choices is something I do automatically. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.93
0.95
14. utomaticity: Physical activityAutomaticity of doing physical activity behavior.Being physically active is something I do without thinking. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.96
0.97
15. Automaticity: Self-weighingAutomaticity of engaging in self-weighing behavior.Weighing myself is something I do without having to consciously remember. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.97
16. Energy and driveFeeling energetic and driven, as opposed to exhausted and fatigued.Over the last 2 weeks I have felt full of vitality. (12)1 = None of the time; 5 = All of the time0.93
0.94
Process variableDescriptionExample item (number of items)Scalingαs
01. Satisfaction with changesSatisfaction with weight change related outcomes (e.g., self-esteem, fit in clothes).How satisfied are you with [change in] your self-esteem? (11)1 = unhappy/dissatisfied; 3 = happy/satisfied0.91
0.94
02. PBC: Healthy eatingConfidence in and perceived ease of eating healthy foods in moderation.How confident are you in your ability to eat healthy foods in moderation? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.75
0.86
03. PBC: Physical activityConfidence in and perceived ease of being physically active every day.How confident are you in your ability to be physically active every day? (2)1 = Not confident; 7 = confident0.83
0.86
04. Confidence: Weight lossConfidence in ability to lose weight.How confident are you in your ability to lose weight? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
05. Confidence: WLMConfidence in ability to maintain weight loss.How confident are you in your ability to maintain weight loss? (1)1 = Not confident; 7 = confidentn/a
06. SE: Emotional eatingPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods when experiencing negative affectI can resist eating unhealthy food when I am anxious. (3)1 = False; 4 = True0.91
0.95
07. SE: Unhealthy food contextPerceived capacity to avoid unhealthy foods in the face of contextual barriers.I can resist eating unhealthy food even when I am at a party. (10)1 = False; 4 = True0.86
0.93
08. SE: Physical activity barriersPerceived capacity to engage in physical activity even in the face of barriers.I will be physically active every day even when I am sad. (12)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.96
09. Action planning: physical activityMaking concrete plans to be physically active.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to be physically active. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.95
0.96
10. Action planning: Healthy eatingMaking concrete plans to make healthy food choices.I have made a detailed plan regarding when to make healthy food choices. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.97
11. Coping planning: Physical activityMaking plans to overcome barriers against physical activity.I have made a detailed plan regarding how to keep on being physically active in difficult situations. (3)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.96
0.96
12. Coping planning: Healthy eatingPlanning to overcome barriers and temptations against healthy eating.I have made a detailed plan regarding what to do if I’m tempted by unhealthy foods. (4)1 = Totally disagree; 4 = Totally agree0.94
0.95
13. Automaticity: Healthy eatingAutomaticity of healthy eating behavior.Making healthy food choices is something I do automatically. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.93
0.95
14. utomaticity: Physical activityAutomaticity of doing physical activity behavior.Being physically active is something I do without thinking. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.96
0.97
15. Automaticity: Self-weighingAutomaticity of engaging in self-weighing behavior.Weighing myself is something I do without having to consciously remember. (5)1 = False; 4 = True0.95
0.97
16. Energy and driveFeeling energetic and driven, as opposed to exhausted and fatigued.Over the last 2 weeks I have felt full of vitality. (12)1 = None of the time; 5 = All of the time0.93
0.94

αs lowest and highest Standardized Reliability Alphas (for 3+ item scales) or r (for 2 items scales) across the three time points of the study; n/a not applicable; PBC perceived behavioral control; Scaling example anchors used with items, reflecting the minimum/maximum possible scores; SE self-efficacy; WLM weight loss maintenance.

The control group received a wirelessly connected scale—which they could use to monitor their weight, though they were not instructed to do so—and standard National Health Service lifestyle advice delivered using an SMS link once every three months. Both groups were aware that data from the wireless scales would be sent to the research team.

Variables/Measures

Each variable was measured at three time points: baseline (B; pre-intervention), 6 months into the intervention (6M), and at 12 months, immediately upon completing the intervention (12M; post-intervention).

Weight

The primary outcome was body weight, in kilograms (kg) assessed at 12 m. At B and 12M, weight was measured using a digital portable scale by research staff who were blind to group allocations. At 6M participants self-reported their weight, and a subset of participants (N = 68) also provided self-reports at 12M (in addition to measurements by staff). The self-reports and objective measures at 12M were correlated at r = 0.998, and most participants reported weights equal to that obtained via objective measurement. This suggests that participants’ self-reports were highly accurate (see Supplementary Files for more details). Thus, the current project used objective weight measurements at B and 12M, supplemented by self-reports at 6M.

16 Psychological Processes

At all three time points, participants completed measures of 16 psychological processes that have previously been tied to weight-related behaviors. These included measures of participants’ satisfaction with (weight-related) changes (using the Weight Outcomes Satisfaction Scale [12]); perceived behavioral control (PBC), self-efficacy (SE), and confidence toward healthy eating, physical activity, weight loss and WLM (using items adapted from previous scales [13, 14]); action planning and coping planning (using items adapted from previous scales [15]); automaticity of healthy eating, physical activity and self-weighing (using items adapted from the Self-Report Behavioral Automaticity Index [16, 17]), and; participants’ energy and drive (using a measure developed for the current study, previously referred to as “ego depletion” [3]). Table 1 provides descriptions and example items for each process, and the Supplementary Materials provide additional details on each measure, including the full list of items, notes on the development of each measure, psychometric results (e.g., confirmatory factor analyses [CFA]), and descriptive statistics (e.g., means, standard deviations) by time point. Scales had high reliability (alphas usually above 0.90), and CFAs found all items to consistently and significantly load onto their respective factors above 0.40.

For each multi-item measure, a mean score was calculated using available responses. To ease interpretation, each scale was scored such that higher numbers reflected a more positive disposition to engage in weight-related behaviors (higher score = higher satisfaction, SE, PBC, etc.). Apart from this adjustment, analyses used raw scores, without transformations.

Analyses

Path analytical models were specified according to Panel B of Fig. 1 and computed individually for each of the 16 process variables in Table 1. Our primary inferences focus on three pathways from Fig. 1. Path a captures the direct effect of the intervention on the psychological processes at 6 months (6M), accounting for earlier levels of the processes at baseline. Path b captures the direct effect of the processes (measured at 6 months) on weight at 12 months (12M), accounting for past weight. Path a × b captures the indirect effect of the intervention on weight at 12 months (12M) attributed to changes on the processes at 6 months (6M)—accounting for direct effects of the intervention on 12M weight, and indirect effects on 12M weight through 6M weight. Path a × b was used to evaluate mediation [18, 19]. Analyses were conducted using the lavaan [20] package in R [21]. To draw inferences, we used null hypothesis significance testing with an alpha level of 0.05. We constructed 95% confidence intervals (CI) using a bias-corrected bootstrap percentile method with 10,000 bootstrap samples. Full information maximum likelihood was used to account for missing data [22–24]. Our analysis script is available at: https://osf.io/5qnu7/.

Results

Weight per Time Point

There was no difference in weight maintenance across the two conditions, reproducing past findings [3]. In the control group, mean weight [and 95% CI] at each time point was: B = 85.51 kg [82.92, 88.11]; 6M = 83.92 kg [80.93, 86.91]; and 12M = 86.67 kg [83.63, 89.71]. In the intervention group, the weights at each time point were: B = 85.58 kg [82.71, 88.44]; 6M = 84.63 kg [80.99, 88.26]; and 12M = 87.03 kg [83.81, 90.26].

Change in Processes from B to 6M

To contextualize our findings, Table 2 provides the results of paired-sample t-tests evaluating the degree to which participants’ scores on each psychological process changed from B to 6M, expressed as Cohen’s d, by study group. Overall, the control group showed notable declines in many processes from B to 6M (10 of 16 variables decreased by d = −0.21 to −0.62). In contrast, declines were generally less pronounced, often even absent, in the intervention group, which even showed significant increases in three process variables (increases of d = 0.23 to 0.51).

Table 2

Summary of Results with Path Analytic Models Specified According to Fig. 1 (Panel B)

Process variableDescriptive meansProcess change
(t-tests: B → 6M)
Path analytic
findings
ControlInterventionControlInterventionPath aPath bPath a × b
B (6M)B (6M)dsig.dsig.βsig.βsig.βsig.
01. Satisfaction with changes2.63 (2.22)2.65 (2.47)−0.62***−0.34***0.18**−0.020.00
02. PBC: Healthy eating5.10 (4.82)5.41 (5.60)−0.26**0.110.21***−0.09t0.02t
03. PBC: Physical activity4.63 (4.29)4.70 (4.56)−0.21*0.110.07−0.05*0.00
04. Confidence: Weight loss5.15 (4.33)5.51 (5.00)−0.56***−0.31***0.16**0.040.01
05. Confidence: WLM4.16 (4.08)4.30 (5.20)−0.090.51***0.30***0.040.01
06. SE: Emotional eating2.44 (2.56)2.76 (2.61)0.080.140.040.010.00
07. SE: Unhealthy food context2.94 (2.75)2.94 (2.94)−0.35***0.000.15**0.020.00
08. SE: Physical activity barriers2.92 (2.70)2.96 (2.80)−0.34***−0.21*0.050.050.00
09. Action planning: Physical activity2.91 (2.59)2.83 (2.66)−0.33***0.110.070.000.00
10. Action planning: Healthy eating3.15 (2.78)3.30 (3.20)−0.43***0.120.20***0.020.00
11. Coping planning: Physical activity2.17 (2.06)2.22 (2.28)0.16t0.070.11t0.030.00
12. Coping planning: Healthy eating2.62 (2.43)2.81 (2.85)−0.24*0.030.19**0.030.01
13. Automaticity: Healthy eating2.60 (2.63)2.74 (2.94)0.050.23*0.14*−0.14*0.02t
14. Automaticity: Physical activity2.36 (2.36)2.42 (2.49)0.020.100.050.050.00
15. Automaticity: Self-weighing2.68 (2.67)2.72 (3.19)0.000.41***0.25***0.010.00
16. Energy and drive3.39 (3.15)3.51 (3.24)−0.42***−0.34***0.02−0.06*0.00
Process variableDescriptive meansProcess change
(t-tests: B → 6M)
Path analytic
findings
ControlInterventionControlInterventionPath aPath bPath a × b
B (6M)B (6M)dsig.dsig.βsig.βsig.βsig.
01. Satisfaction with changes2.63 (2.22)2.65 (2.47)−0.62***−0.34***0.18**−0.020.00
02. PBC: Healthy eating5.10 (4.82)5.41 (5.60)−0.26**0.110.21***−0.09t0.02t
03. PBC: Physical activity4.63 (4.29)4.70 (4.56)−0.21*0.110.07−0.05*0.00
04. Confidence: Weight loss5.15 (4.33)5.51 (5.00)−0.56***−0.31***0.16**0.040.01
05. Confidence: WLM4.16 (4.08)4.30 (5.20)−0.090.51***0.30***0.040.01
06. SE: Emotional eating2.44 (2.56)2.76 (2.61)0.080.140.040.010.00
07. SE: Unhealthy food context2.94 (2.75)2.94 (2.94)−0.35***0.000.15**0.020.00
08. SE: Physical activity barriers2.92 (2.70)2.96 (2.80)−0.34***−0.21*0.050.050.00
09. Action planning: Physical activity2.91 (2.59)2.83 (2.66)−0.33***0.110.070.000.00
10. Action planning: Healthy eating3.15 (2.78)3.30 (3.20)−0.43***0.120.20***0.020.00
11. Coping planning: Physical activity2.17 (2.06)2.22 (2.28)0.16t0.070.11t0.030.00
12. Coping planning: Healthy eating2.62 (2.43)2.81 (2.85)−0.24*0.030.19**0.030.01
13. Automaticity: Healthy eating2.60 (2.63)2.74 (2.94)0.050.23*0.14*−0.14*0.02t
14. Automaticity: Physical activity2.36 (2.36)2.42 (2.49)0.020.100.050.050.00
15. Automaticity: Self-weighing2.68 (2.67)2.72 (3.19)0.000.41***0.25***0.010.00
16. Energy and drive3.39 (3.15)3.51 (3.24)−0.42***−0.34***0.02−0.06*0.00

β standardized coefficient; 6M 6-month follow-up; B baseline; d Cohen’s d; PBC perceived behavioral control; SE self-efficacy; sig. significance level; WLM weight loss maintenance. Bold font indicates significance at p < .05.

aSee Supplementary Materials (Table S49) for full t-test results (e.g., for unstandardized difference scores, 95% CIs, and exact p-values).

bSee Supplementary Materials (Tables S33–S48) for full path analytic results (e.g., for unstandardized effects, standard errors, 95% CIs, exact p-values, fit indices).

***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05; tp < .10.

Table 2

Summary of Results with Path Analytic Models Specified According to Fig. 1 (Panel B)

Process variableDescriptive meansProcess change
(t-tests: B → 6M)
Path analytic
findings
ControlInterventionControlInterventionPath aPath bPath a × b
B (6M)B (6M)dsig.dsig.βsig.βsig.βsig.
01. Satisfaction with changes2.63 (2.22)2.65 (2.47)−0.62***−0.34***0.18**−0.020.00
02. PBC: Healthy eating5.10 (4.82)5.41 (5.60)−0.26**0.110.21***−0.09t0.02t
03. PBC: Physical activity4.63 (4.29)4.70 (4.56)−0.21*0.110.07−0.05*0.00
04. Confidence: Weight loss5.15 (4.33)5.51 (5.00)−0.56***−0.31***0.16**0.040.01
05. Confidence: WLM4.16 (4.08)4.30 (5.20)−0.090.51***0.30***0.040.01
06. SE: Emotional eating2.44 (2.56)2.76 (2.61)0.080.140.040.010.00
07. SE: Unhealthy food context2.94 (2.75)2.94 (2.94)−0.35***0.000.15**0.020.00
08. SE: Physical activity barriers2.92 (2.70)2.96 (2.80)−0.34***−0.21*0.050.050.00
09. Action planning: Physical activity2.91 (2.59)2.83 (2.66)−0.33***0.110.070.000.00
10. Action planning: Healthy eating3.15 (2.78)3.30 (3.20)−0.43***0.120.20***0.020.00
11. Coping planning: Physical activity2.17 (2.06)2.22 (2.28)0.16t0.070.11t0.030.00
12. Coping planning: Healthy eating2.62 (2.43)2.81 (2.85)−0.24*0.030.19**0.030.01
13. Automaticity: Healthy eating2.60 (2.63)2.74 (2.94)0.050.23*0.14*−0.14*0.02t
14. Automaticity: Physical activity2.36 (2.36)2.42 (2.49)0.020.100.050.050.00
15. Automaticity: Self-weighing2.68 (2.67)2.72 (3.19)0.000.41***0.25***0.010.00
16. Energy and drive3.39 (3.15)3.51 (3.24)−0.42***−0.34***0.02−0.06*0.00
Process variableDescriptive meansProcess change
(t-tests: B → 6M)
Path analytic
findings
ControlInterventionControlInterventionPath aPath bPath a × b
B (6M)B (6M)dsig.dsig.βsig.βsig.βsig.
01. Satisfaction with changes2.63 (2.22)2.65 (2.47)−0.62***−0.34***0.18**−0.020.00
02. PBC: Healthy eating5.10 (4.82)5.41 (5.60)−0.26**0.110.21***−0.09t0.02t
03. PBC: Physical activity4.63 (4.29)4.70 (4.56)−0.21*0.110.07−0.05*0.00
04. Confidence: Weight loss5.15 (4.33)5.51 (5.00)−0.56***−0.31***0.16**0.040.01
05. Confidence: WLM4.16 (4.08)4.30 (5.20)−0.090.51***0.30***0.040.01
06. SE: Emotional eating2.44 (2.56)2.76 (2.61)0.080.140.040.010.00
07. SE: Unhealthy food context2.94 (2.75)2.94 (2.94)−0.35***0.000.15**0.020.00
08. SE: Physical activity barriers2.92 (2.70)2.96 (2.80)−0.34***−0.21*0.050.050.00
09. Action planning: Physical activity2.91 (2.59)2.83 (2.66)−0.33***0.110.070.000.00
10. Action planning: Healthy eating3.15 (2.78)3.30 (3.20)−0.43***0.120.20***0.020.00
11. Coping planning: Physical activity2.17 (2.06)2.22 (2.28)0.16t0.070.11t0.030.00
12. Coping planning: Healthy eating2.62 (2.43)2.81 (2.85)−0.24*0.030.19**0.030.01
13. Automaticity: Healthy eating2.60 (2.63)2.74 (2.94)0.050.23*0.14*−0.14*0.02t
14. Automaticity: Physical activity2.36 (2.36)2.42 (2.49)0.020.100.050.050.00
15. Automaticity: Self-weighing2.68 (2.67)2.72 (3.19)0.000.41***0.25***0.010.00
16. Energy and drive3.39 (3.15)3.51 (3.24)−0.42***−0.34***0.02−0.06*0.00

β standardized coefficient; 6M 6-month follow-up; B baseline; d Cohen’s d; PBC perceived behavioral control; SE self-efficacy; sig. significance level; WLM weight loss maintenance. Bold font indicates significance at p < .05.

aSee Supplementary Materials (Table S49) for full t-test results (e.g., for unstandardized difference scores, 95% CIs, and exact p-values).

bSee Supplementary Materials (Tables S33–S48) for full path analytic results (e.g., for unstandardized effects, standard errors, 95% CIs, exact p-values, fit indices).

***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05; tp < .10.

Inferential Tests of the Role of Each Process

Table 2 presents the results of path analytic models, specified in line with Fig. 1 (Panel B), for each of the 16 psychological processes. We present standardized coefficients for Path a, Path b, and the indirect effect of Path a × b. For these paths (and other paths in Fig. 1), the Supplementary Materials additionally provide unstandardized regression coefficients, standard errors, 95% CIs, exact p-values, fit indices, and coefficients of determination (R2). Path a was significant for 9 of the 16 processes; indicating that compared with the control condition, the intervention was better able to sustain (i.e., avoid declines in) or increase people’s levels of satisfaction, PBC (for healthy eating), confidence (toward weight loss and WLM), SE (in the face of unhealthy food), action and coping planning (for healthy eating), and automaticity (for healthy eating and self-weighing). Yet, there was limited evidence that the psychological processes were associated with weight change; Path b was significant in only three instances (PBC for physical activity; automaticity for healthy eating; energy and drive), and the mediational Path a × b was never significant.

Discussion

The degree to which behavioral interventions are successful often depends on their ability to change psychological processes, and, in turn, on the degree to which changes in those processes are sustained and impact people’s behaviors [4, 5]. In this project, we conducted a process analysis of the NULevel trial to identify why the intervention did not outperform a control group to impact WLM. To do this, we examined the influence of the intervention on 16 psychological processes targeted by the intervention (Fig. 1, Path a), the degree to which change in these processes was associated with weight change over time (Path b), and the extent to which these two forces jointly impacted weight change outcomes (Path a × b).

Our analyses revealed several patterns. First, the intervention appeared to alter how most of the psychological processes it targeted changed over time (Path a). Generally, participants in the control group showed substantial decline on most processes over time (e.g., in satisfaction, weight loss confidence, and action planning), but this decline was attenuated within the intervention condition. For a few processes (e.g., confidence in WLM, automaticity), participants in the intervention condition showed increased scores over time, relative to the control group. Despite the intervention’s ability to stabilize or promote psychological processes, we found limited support for higher scores on these processes being associated with successful WLM (Path b) over time within the context of this trial. Consequently, the operation of the intervention on weight outcomes through these processes (the composite Path a × b) was never significant. This occurred despite an observation that within any given time point (i.e., cross-sectionally), the majority of processes were significantly correlated with weight.

If the intervention successfully engaged the psychological processes it targeted, then why did these changes not lead to greater WLM? One explanation would be that, though the processes are amenable to influence via intervention, they may not be reliable determinants of WLM. Given that these processes were selected based on their theoretical relevance to behavioral maintenance [6–8], replicating our findings in other interventions would be necessary before seeking to dismiss the processes. This is particularly so as other explanations could also account for our findings. In particular, we note that both the intervention and the control group performed well, each gaining an average of less than 1.5 kg over the 12 months of the trial. It is possible that the provision of elements such as a wireless scale to the control group, along with the knowledge that data from these scales would be accessible to the research team, was sufficient to encourage certain processes (e.g., self-monitoring) that supported WLM [25–27]. This possibility is consistent with the observation that, during the trial, control group participants frequently engaged in self-weighing, averaging twice a week [3]. If so, this could have attenuated our chances of finding the intervention to be favorable relative to the control (though the link between self-weighing and WLM remains to be verified). Similarly, attenuated variability in WLM across the groups may have interfered with observing associations over time between processes and weight change (Path b).

A second possible explanation for our findings is that although the intervention impacted WLM processes, the effects may have been too small to differentially affect WLM. Consistent with this point, when the effects of the NULevel intervention on processes at 6 months were present (i.e., statistically significant for 9 of 16 variables) these could only be qualified as small-to-medium in magnitude [28]. Perhaps more important, however, is that the processes themselves are thought of as direct precursors to behavior, not weight change, and it is behavior (not the processes themselves) that ultimately affects weight. Thus, the lack of substantial effects from the processes to WLM could reflect either a failure of the processes to impact behavior, or that changes in behavior had only a modest impact on weight. For example, the association between weight and change in behaviors such as physical activity, diet, and self-weighing is known to be variable [26, 29–32]. Such a dynamic could considerably attenuate the overall impact of targeting processes to alter behavior to then impact weight. The lack of a behavioral analysis is an important limitation of the current study, and should be a major focus for future work. Additionally, inferences are limited by the time frames that were used to assess the constructs of interest. For instance, it is possible that assessing weight at 6-month intervals was too infrequent, and that using shorter time intervals (e.g., weekly or monthly) would reveal more consistent associations between the proposed processes and weight. Likewise, many of the process measures were brief in scope, and it may be that clearer mediational patterns would emerge by using more comprehensive and frequent measures of the processes.

Ultimately, establishing whether the processes elicit change in desired WLM behaviors (healthier diet, physical activity), and how reliably such behaviors determine WLM, will not only provide further insights on the NULevel trial, but will inform future intervention efforts as well. For instance, if both pathways hold, but only show modest effects, it could be that the intensity of the intervention was too low, and that stronger impacts on the 16 psychological processes should be sought. In the current trial, most of the significant effects were also related to the intervention dampening reductions in the processes otherwise seen in the control group rather than bolstering them in the intervention group. If processes need to be maintained above a certain threshold level to have an impact on behavior and WLM [33], then the intervention might need to achieve stronger bolstering effects on the processes to achieve WLM. As the intervention was designed to be low-intensity to facilitate scalability [2, 3], implementing methods to augment the strength of the intervention could remain promising. This could be by increasing the “dosage” of the behavior change techniques (BCTs) [34] used, or by tailoring their use to better account for people’s needs/preferences [35].

In contrast, if there is no effect between the processes and the behaviors, or between behaviors and WLM, then this would suggest targeting other psychological processes than assessed in the current study (using appropriate BCTs) [34], or more specific forms of behaviors that have more consistent effects on WLM. Both of these possibilities offer substantial avenues for future intervention work. In paying attention to the effects between processes, behaviors, and WLM, it may also be useful to examine the degree to which these vary across groups of participants, as it is unlikely all participants experience benefits via the same mechanisms of action. Such information would be invaluable to further personalize future WLM interventions.

Funding

The NULevel trial was funded by the UK National Prevention Research Initiative (NPRI) Phase 4 (grant MR/J000477/1). The NPRI includes the following Funding Partners (in alphabetical order): Alzheimer’s Research Trust, Alzheimer’s Society, Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council, British Heart Foundation, Cancer Research UK, Chief Scientist Office, Scottish Government Health Directorate, Department of Health, Diabetes UK, Economic and Social Research Council, Health and Social Care Research and Development Division of the Public Health Agency (HSC & R&D Division), Medical Research Council, The Stroke Association, Wellcome Trust, Welsh Assembly Government, and World Cancer Research Fund. K.J.D. received funds from the Fonds de Recherche du Québec, in partnership with the Quebec Strategy for Patient-Oriented Research Support Unit. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards Keven Joyal-Desmarais, Alexander J. Rothman, Elizabeth H. Evans, Vera Araújo-Soares, Falko F. Sniehotta declare that they have no conflict of interest. All procedures, including the informed consent process, were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000.

Authors’ Contributions Keven Joyal-Desmarais (Conceptualization [lead], Data curation [equal], Formal analysis [lead], Methodology [lead], Project administration [lead], Software [lead], Validation [lead], Visualization [lead], Writing – original draft [lead], Writing – review & editing [lead], Alexander J. Rothman (Investigation [supporting], Methodology [supporting], Project administration [equal], Supervision [lead], Writing – original draft [supporting], Writing – review & editing [equal]), Elizabeth H. Evans (Conceptualization [equal], Data curation [lead], Investigation [equal], Methodology [supporting], Writing – review & editing [equal]), Vera Araújo-Soares (Conceptualization [equal], Funding acquisition [equal], Investigation [equal], Methodology [equal], Supervision [equal], Writing – review & editing [equal]), and Falko F. Sniehotta (Conceptualization [equal], Funding acquisition [lead], Investigation [equal], Methodology [equal], Supervision [equal], Writing – review & editing [equal])

Transparency Statements The current project was preregistered through the Open Science Framework at osf.io/w8ryh. This preregistration covered the full analysis plan. Data are available from the NIHR Clinical Trials Unit North East for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data ([email protected]). A project page for this work can be accessed at osf.io/5qnu7. Our project page contains the analysis script files for the results we report, along with our Supplementary Files (e.g., survey items used).

References

1.

MacLean
 
PS
,
Wing
 
RR
,
Davidson
 
T
, et al. .
NIH working group report: innovative research to improve maintenance of weight loss
.
Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.)
.
2015
;
23
(
1
):
7
15
. doi:10.1002/oby.20967

2.

Evans
 
EH
,
Araújo-Soares
 
V
,
Adamson
 
A
, et al. .
The NULevel trial of a scalable, technology-assisted weight loss maintenance intervention for obese adults after clinically significant weight loss: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial
.
Trials
.
2015
;
16
(
1
):
1
17
. doi:10.1186/s13063-015-0931-7

3.

Sniehotta
 
FF
,
Evans
 
EH
,
Sainsbury
 
K
, et al. .
Behavioural intervention for weight loss maintenance versus standard weight advice in adults with obesity: a randomised controlled trial in the UK (NULevel Trial)
.
PLoS Med.
 
2019
;
16
(
5
):
e1002793
. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002793

4.

Sheeran
 
P
,
Klein
 
WM
,
Rothman
 
AJ.
 
Health behavior change: moving from observation to intervention
.
Annu Rev Psychol.
 
2017
;
68
:
573
600
. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044007

5.

Nielsen
 
L
,
Riddle
 
M
,
King
 
JW
, et al. ;
NIH Science of Behavior Change Implementation Team
.
The NIH science of behavior change program: transforming the science through a focus on mechanisms of change
.
Behav Res Ther.
 
2018
;
101
:
3
11
. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2017.07.002

6.

Kwasnicka
 
D
,
Dombrowski
 
SU
,
White
 
M
,
Sniehotta
 
F.
 
Theoretical explanations for maintenance of behaviour change: a systematic review of behaviour theories
.
Health Psychol Rev
.
2016
;
10
(
3
):
277
296
. doi:10.1080/17437199.2016.1151372

7.

Kanfer
 
FH
,
Goldstein
 
AP.
 
Helping People Change
.
Elmsford
:
Pergamon Press
;
1975
.

8.

Schwarzer
 
R.
 
Modeling health behavior change: how to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors
.
Appl Psychol
.
2008
;
57
(
1
):
1
29
. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x

9.

Dombrowski
 
SU
,
Knittle
 
K
,
Avenell
 
A
,
Araújo-Soares
 
V
,
Sniehotta
 
FF.
 
Long term maintenance of weight loss with non-surgical interventions in obese adults: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials
.
BMJ
.
2014
;
348
:
g2646
.

10.

Sniehotta
 
FF.
 
Weight loss maintenance intervention for obese adults after clinically significant weight loss
. ISRCTNregistry identifier: ISRCTN14657176. Updated August 5,
2019
. doi: 10.1186/ISRCTN14657176.
Available at
: https://www.isrctn.com/ISRCTN14657176.
Accessibility verified March 7, 2023
.

11.

Joyal-Desmarais
 
K
,
Rothman
 
AJ
,
Sniehotta
 
FF
,
Evans
 
EH
,
Araujo-Soares
 
V.
 
NULevel evaluation of individual mechanisms
. OSF Registries. Registered October 25,
2019
. Available at https://osf.io/w8ryh. Accessibility verified
March 7, 2023
.

12.

Finch
 
EA
,
Linde
 
JA
,
Jeffery
 
RW
,
Rothman
 
AJ
,
King
 
CM
,
Levy
 
RL.
 
The effects of outcome expectations and satisfaction on weight loss and maintenance: correlational and experimental analyses-a randomized trial
.
Health Psychol.
 
2005
;
24
(
6
):
608
616
.

13.

Schwarzer
 
R
,
Renner
 
B.
 
Health-specific self-efficacy scales
.
2009
. Available at https://userpage.fu-berlin.de/health/healself.pdf. Accessibility verified
March 7, 2023
.

14.

Clark
 
MM
,
Abrams
 
DB
,
Niaura
 
RS
,
Eaton
 
CA
,
Rossi
 
JS.
 
Self-efficacy in weight management
.
J Consult Clin Psychol.
 
1991
;
59
(
5
):
739
744
. doi:10.1037//0022-006x.59.5.739

15.

Sniehotta
 
FF
,
Schwarzer
 
R
,
Scholz
 
U
,
Schüz
 
B.
 
Action planning and coping planning for long‐term lifestyle change: theory and assessment
.
Eur J Soc Psychol
.
2005
;
35
(
4
):
565
576
. doi:10.1002/ejsp.258

16.

Verplanken
 
B
,
Orbell
 
S.
 
Reflections on past behavior: a self-report index of habit strength
.
J Appl Soc Psychol
.
2003
;
33
(
6
):
1313
1330
. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x

17.

Gardner
 
B
,
Abraham
 
C
,
Lally
 
P
,
de Bruijn
 
G-J.
 
Towards parsimony in habit measurement: testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index
.
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
.
2012
;
9
(
1
):
102
112
. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-9-102

18.

Preacher
 
KJ
,
Hayes
 
AF.
 
Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models
.
Behav Res Methods.
 
2008
;
40
(
3
):
879
891
. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

19.

Preacher
 
KJ
,
Hayes
 
AF.
 
SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models
.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput.
 
2004
;
36
(
4
):
717
731
. doi:10.3758/BF03206553

20.

Rosseel
 
Y.
 
An R package for structural equation modeling
.
J Stat Softw
.
2012
;
48
(
2
):
1
36
. doi:10.18637/jss.v048.i02

21.

R Core Team
.
R: A language and environment for statistical computing
.
2018
.
Available at
 https://www.R-project.org/.
Accessibility verified February 10, 2024
.

22.

Graham
 
JW.
 
Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world
.
Annu Rev Psychol.
 
2009
;
60
:
549
576
. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530

23.

Schafer
 
JL
,
Graham
 
JW.
 
Missing data: our view of the state of the art
.
Psychol Methods.
 
2002
;
7
(
2
):
147
177
. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147

24.

Schlomer
 
GL
,
Bauman
 
S
,
Card
 
NA.
 
Best practices for missing data management in counseling psychology
.
J Couns Psychol
.
2010
;
57
(
1
):
1
10
. doi:10.1037/a0018082

25.

VanWormer
 
JJ
,
French
 
SA
,
Pereira
 
MA
,
Welsh
 
EM.
 
The impact of regular self-weighing on weight management: a systematic literature review
.
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
.
2008
;
5
(
1
):
54
. doi:10.1186%2F1479-5868-5-54

26.

Zheng
 
Y
,
Klem
 
ML
,
Sereika
 
SM
,
Danford
 
CA
,
Ewing
 
LJ
,
Burke
 
LE.
 
Self-weighing in weight management: a systematic literature review
.
Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.)
.
2015
;
23
(
2
):
256
265
.

27.

Shieh
 
C
,
Knisely
 
MR
,
Clark
 
D
,
Carpenter
 
JS.
 
Self-weighing in weight management interventions: a systematic review of literature
.
Obes Res Clin Pract
.
2016
;
10
(
5
):
493
519
. doi:10.1016/j.orcp.2016.01.004

28.

Cohen
 
J.
 
Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences
. 2nd ed.
New York
:
Routledge
;
1988
.

29.

Foright
 
RM
,
Presby
 
DM
,
Sherk
 
VD
, et al. .
Is regular exercise an effective strategy for weight loss maintenance
?
Physiol Behav.
 
2018
;
188
:
86
93
. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.01.025

30.

Madigan
 
CD
,
Daley
 
AJ
,
Lewis
 
AL
,
Aveyard
 
P
,
Jolly
 
K.
 
Is self-weighing an effective tool for weight loss: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis
.
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
.
2015
;
12
(
1
):
104
. doi:10.1186/s12966-015-0267-4

31.

Johansson
 
K
,
Neovius
 
M
,
Hemmingsson
 
E.
 
Effects of anti-obesity drugs, diet, and exercise on weight-loss maintenance after a very-low-calorie diet or low-calorie diet: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials
.
Am J Clin Nutr.
 
2013
;
99
(
1
):
14
23
. doi:10.3945%2Fajcn.113.070052

32.

Jon Schoenfeld
 
B
,
Albert Aragon
 
A
,
Krieger
 
JW.
 
Effects of meal frequency on weight loss and body composition: a meta-analysis
.
Nutr Rev.
 
2015
;
73
(
2
):
69
82
. doi:10.1093/nutrit/nuu017

33.

Rothman
 
AJ
,
Sheeran
 
P.
 
What is slowing us down? six challenges to accelerating advances in health behavior change
.
Ann Behav Med.
 
2020
;
54
(
12
):
948
959
. doi:10.1093/abm/kaaa090

34.

Michie
 
S
,
Richardson
 
M
,
Johnston
 
M
, et al. .
The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions
.
Ann Behav Med.
 
2013
;
46
(
1
):
81
95
. doi:10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6

35.

Joyal-Desmarais
 
K
,
Scharmer
 
AK
,
Madzelan
 
MK
,
See
 
JV
,
Rothman
 
AJ
,
Snyder
 
M.
 
Appealing to motivation to change attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a systematic review and meta-analysis of 702 experimental tests of the effects of motivational message matching on persuasion
.
Psychol Bull.
 
2022
;
148
(
7–8
):
465
517
. doi:10.1037/bul0000377

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.